Nothing Special   »   [go: up one dir, main page]

Next Article in Journal
Applying a Support Vector Machine (SVM-RFE) Learning Approach to Investigate Students’ Scientific Literacy Development: Evidence from Asia, Europe, and South America
Next Article in Special Issue
The Development of Intellect in Emerging Adults: Predictors of Longitudinal Trajectories
Previous Article in Journal
Breaking Down the Concept of Students’ Thinking and Reasoning Skills for Implementation in the Classroom
Previous Article in Special Issue
Crystallized Intelligence, Fluid Intelligence, and Need for Cognition: Their Longitudinal Relations in Adolescence
You seem to have javascript disabled. Please note that many of the page functionalities won't work as expected without javascript enabled.
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Cognitive Engagement and Subjective Well-Being in Adults: Exploring the Role of Domain-Specific Need for Cognition

Institute of Psychology, Chemnitz University of Technology, 09107 Chemnitz, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Intell. 2024, 12(11), 110; https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12110110
Submission received: 21 August 2024 / Revised: 22 October 2024 / Accepted: 29 October 2024 / Published: 3 November 2024
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Cognitive Motivation)

Abstract

:
Need for Cognition (NFC) is an investment trait reflecting interindividual differences in intrinsically motivated engagement in cognitive endeavors. While other motivational constructs like the ability self-concept are typically conceptualized domain-specific, NFC was initially conceived to describe domain-general behavior. Building on a previous study reporting the relevance of domain-specific aspects of NFC in the school context, we investigated the domain-specificity of NFC in predicting various aspects of subjective well-being across three samples with N = 1074 participants and subsamples of students and professionals ranging from n = 140 to n = 346. Our findings reveal positive associations of both domain-general and domain-specific NFC with positive affect, life satisfaction, and job- and study-related satisfaction. In part, domain-specific NFC was more strongly related to domain-specific life satisfaction compared to domain-general NFC. Additionally, we found evidence for small incremental values of domain-specific NFC over and above domain-general NFC in predicting subjective well-being. Confirming previous research, self-control and the emotion regulation strategy reappraisal partially mediated the prediction of well-being by NFC. Our results indicate that additionally considering NFC as domain-specific can meaningfully complement the domain-general conceptualization.

1. Introduction

Need for Cognition (NFC) describes relatively stable interindividual differences in approaching cognitively challenging situations and engaging in effortful cognition (Cacioppo and Petty 1982). Individuals with higher NFC levels tend to have more elaborated information processing (Cacioppo et al. 1996). NFC has been categorized as being a core investment or intellect trait (von Stumm and Ackerman 2013), also being related to Curiosity or Openness to Ideas (Mussel 2013; von Stumm and Ackerman 2013), describing “[…] when, where, and how people invest their time and effort in their intellect” (von Stumm and Ackerman 2013, p. 841). Hence, NFC is clearly distinguishable from ability, and relations with cognitive abilities are weak to moderate at most, depending on the studied ability and research design (Fleischhauer et al. 2010; Hill et al. 2016; von Stumm and Ackerman 2013).
Defining research on NFC had its origin in social–psychological research on forming judgments and attitudes (for an overview, Cacioppo et al. 1996). In the last four decades, research has produced reliable findings on the nomological network of NFC and on implications of different NFC levels in various contexts (decision making: e.g., Harman 2011; advertising: e.g., Haugtvedt et al. 1992; media usage: e.g., Henning and Vorderer 2001; school: e.g., Lavrijsen et al. 2021; and health: e.g., Williams-Piehota et al. 2003). One research topic of increasing interest has been the relation of NFC to various dimensions of human well-being (for an overview, Zerna et al. 2024).

1.1. Need for Cognition and Subjective Well-Being

Early research indicated negative associations of NFC with depression and anxiety symptoms (Epstein et al. 1996; Reeves et al. 1995), which were confirmed in later research (Baer et al. 2013; Bertrams and Dickhäuser 2012; Nishiguchi et al. 2016; Zainal and Newman 2022). Higher NFC levels have been related to more reported positive life events (Strobel et al. 2017) in most studies to increased positive affect (e.g., Fleischhauer et al. 2010; Strobel et al. 2017; Yazdani and Siedlecki 2021) and sometimes significantly to reduced negative affect (e.g., Yazdani and Siedlecki 2021). Further, NFC was positively associated with subjective evaluations of one’s life (i.e., cognitive subjective well-being; Diener 1984), known as general or domain-specific life satisfaction. Reported associations with global life satisfaction ranged from r = −0.01 to r = 0.62 with ρ = 0.18 (Zerna et al. 2024). Studies on university students reported associations between NFC and study-related well-being (Grass et al. 2017, 2018, 2023) that are comparable to those between NFC and academic achievement (ρ = 0.20; Liu and Nesbit 2024). All findings indicate that NFC as a cognitive-motivational trait does not only predict intellectual achievements but does also play a relevant role for a broad range of aspects of subjective well-being. Recent meta-analytic findings report a medium overall relationship of ρ = 0.20 between NFC and well-being considering different well-being indicators (Lua et al. 2024; Zerna et al. 2024).

1.2. Explaining Processes

Different explanations for positive associations of NFC with well-being indicators have been posited theoretically and partly examined. For academic jobs and study-related satisfaction, one line of reasoning is that individuals with higher NFC levels are intrinsically motivated to cope with challenging academic problems (e.g., Grass et al. 2017). Hence, they do not only strive for positive career outcomes but can really enjoy learning, for example, for an exam at university. In general, higher NFC levels are likely to be associated with rather active coping behavior (e.g., Bye and Pushkar 2009), taking flexible perspectives on (decision) situations (for an overview, Cacioppo et al. 1996), goal-oriented behavior (e.g., Fleischhauer et al. 2010), and approaching situations perceived as challenging, which can be considered to promote well-being in the long run (e.g., Zerna et al. 2024). These explaining approaches follow the idea of personality influencing in what way individuals adapt to certain life events, resulting in different well-being levels (Diener et al. 2006). For positive and negative affect, one longitudinal study provided evidence that differences in NFC are associated with different evaluations of positive and negative life events resulting in differences in affective well-being as follows: Positive life events were experienced more positively, which additionally enhanced positive affective well-being (Strobel et al. 2017). All of that can be considered as paths to increased well-being associated with higher NFC levels (for review, see Zerna et al. 2024).
To better understand the relations of NFC to well-being, research has to consider potentially underlying processes and mediating variables. Thereby, self-control and emotion regulation strategies refer to processes that predict subjective well-being (e.g., de Ridder et al. 2012; Gross and John 2003), were associated with NFC in previous research (e.g., Bertrams and Dickhäuser 2012; Grass et al. 2018), and have been shown to mediate associations of NFC with well-being and mental health (e.g., Bertrams and Dickhäuser 2012; Nishiguchi et al. 2016).

1.2.1. Trait Self-Control

Since Walter Mischel reported results of the popular marshmallow experiment in the 1960s (for review, e.g., Mischel et al. 2011), much research has examined the predictive value of individuals’ tendency to resist temptations and to delay gratification for desirable outcomes like academic achievement, life satisfaction, and (mental) health (e.g., de Ridder et al. 2012; Moffitt et al. 2011; Tangney et al. 2004). Thereby, self-control can be considered as one type of self-regulation, referring to processes to override impulses to resolve a conflict between competing behavioral options (for reviews, Fujita 2011; Inzlicht et al. 2021; Wennerhold and Friese 2023). From a trait perspective, trait self-control addresses relatively stable individual tendencies to behave in conformity with desired goals in self-control dilemmas, for example, to learn for an exam instead of going out (de Ridder et al. 2018). Thereby, conceptualizations of trait self-control explain interindividual differences with differences in regulatory abilities and motivational dispositions (Wennerhold and Friese 2023). A meta-analysis reported an association of r = 0.33 between trait self-control and well-being (de Ridder et al. 2012). The distinct behaviors that reflect low vs. high trait self-control and lead to the (un)successful resolution of conflicts with relevance for important life outcomes are still discussed (de Ridder et al. 2018). With NFC referring to cognitive effort investment per definition (Cacioppo et al. 1996) as well as being empirically associated with goal-orientation (Fleischhauer et al. 2010) and increased recruitment of resources for cognitively demanding tasks (Mussel et al. 2016), higher NFC levels are likely to promote increased self-control. Research on NFC has repeatedly provided evidence for a positive association with trait self-control and a mediating role of trait self-control for predicting well-being indicators by NFC (e.g., Bertrams and Dickhäuser 2012; Nishiguchi et al. 2016; Zerna et al. 2022, 2024).

1.2.2. Emotion Regulation Strategies

During the last decades, research on well-being, psychotherapeutic interventions, or coaching has shown increasing interest in the way individuals manage their emotions (e.g., Aldao et al. 2010; Scherer et al. 2017). Emotion regulation describes different processes modulating the emotional states of individuals, which may be executed consciously but are also often executed without conscious deliberation (Gross and John 2003). Emotion regulation can be antecedent-focused, referring to processes before emotional responses are fully activated (Gross and John 2003), or response-focused, referring to response modulation after emotional reactions have already been activated (Gross and John 2003).
A common example of antecedent-focused strategies is cognitive reappraisal, which describes cognitively reconstruing a situation in a way that changes emotional responses; an example for response-focused strategies is suppression, which refers to modulating the behavioral expression of emotions with inhibiting the expression of an ongoing emotional response (Gross and John 2003). Concerning well-being and psychopathology, research has provided evidence for beneficial effects of reappraisal and disadvantageous effects of suppression (e.g., Aldao et al. 2010; Goldin et al. 2008; Gross and John 2003). This knowledge has been implemented in psychotherapeutic techniques like cognitive restructuring to improve individual well-being (e.g., Grecucci et al. 2020). Higher NFC is associated with being more open for information deviating from one’s opinion or attitude and to approach cognitive challenges (for an overview, Cacioppo et al. 1996). Being confronted with complex, potentially overwhelming situations can be regarded as both cognitively and emotionally challenging. From a theoretical perspective, approaching thoughts about such situations and adopting different perspectives could be beneficial for well-being compared to avoidance behavior or keeping one (rigid) perspective on a situation. Additionally, higher NFC was associated with self-reporting more positive life events (Strobel et al. 2017), possibly resulting from more positively evaluating everyday situations. Hence, higher NFC levels are associated with more flexible perspectives on situations and topics, which in turn should encourage adaptive reappraisal of situations. Those processes may explain why higher NFC levels have been reported to be associated with emotion regulation strategies, especially with cognitive reappraisal (Grass et al. 2018; Hui et al. 2023; Zerna et al. 2022). Previous research has identified cognitive reappraisal as a mediating process for the relations of NFC to well-being indicators (e.g., Grass et al. 2018; Hui et al. 2023).

1.3. Domain Specificity of Need for Cognition

NFC has been defined as a general behavioral tendency “[…] to engage in and enjoy effortful cognitive endeavors rather than as chronic tendencies toward processing information in particular domains” (Cacioppo et al. 1996, p. 199). Theoretical approaches to NFC and other investment traits have proposed that investment trait level develops in interaction with intellectual experiences like a cognitively stimulating or rewarding environment during one’s educational biography (Ackerman 1996; Cacioppo et al. 1996; von Stumm and Ackerman 2013). Keller et al. (2017) highlighted that these experiences for children may differ depending on the field of interest or school subject, proposing that domain-specific NFC profiles may exist additionally to domain-general NFC. Their investigation on ninth-graders provided evidence for the meaningfulness and validity of domain-specific aspects of NFC in addition to domain-general NFC in the context of subject-specific school achievements (Keller et al. 2017).
Following the symmetry principle for hierarchical constructs and the idea of bandwidth-fidelity (e.g., Kretzschmar et al. 2018), research on implications of NFC for well-being could profit from a domain-specific perspective on NFC because the strength of associations probably depends on the fit of construct levels. Subjective well-being refers to subjective evaluations of life both in general and specific domains (e.g., job satisfaction). Based on Keller et al. (2017) and the abovementioned symmetry principle of constructs, conceptualizing NFC in adults also domain-specifically could improve realistic estimations of its role, especially for domain-specific well-being measures.

1.4. Present Research

Based on previous research on schoolchildren (Keller et al. 2017), we examined the value of conceptualizing NFC in adults not only domain-generally but also domain-specifically in the context of subjective well-being using data of three samples recruited between 2020 and 2023. Based on previous research providing evidence for the predictive value of NFC for well-being (for a review, Zerna et al. 2024), we assumed at least medium positive associations with positive affect (H1) and low negative associations with negative affect (H2), as well as medium positive associations between NFC and global life satisfaction (H3). For domain-specific life satisfaction in students, we assumed increasing NFC to be associated with increasing study-related satisfaction, expecting strongest associations with subject-related satisfaction based on previous research (H4a; Grass et al. 2023). For employed individuals, we assumed a positive association between NFC and job satisfaction (H4b). Taking a domain-specific perspective on NFC comparable to the study by Keller et al. (2017), we assumed incremental predictive values over and above general NFC of study-specific NFC for predicting study-related satisfaction and of job-specific NFC for predicting job satisfaction (H5). We assumed trait self-control and cognitive reappraisal as partly explaining processes and examined them as mediators for significant associations of NFC with well-being indicators (H6). Data collections of Samples 2 and 3 aimed at replicating the first data collection and at providing an increased robustness of findings and conclusions.

2. Material and Methods

In this article, we report about different data collections at three time periods between June 2020 and February 2023. We report how we determined our sample size, all data exclusions, all manipulations, and all measures in this study. All data collections have been preregistered. Preregistrations are available under https://aspredicted.org/95pq-hyhb.pdf (accessed on 28 October 2024; Sample 1), https://aspredicted.org/2tn4-xtgv.pdf (accessed on 28 October 2024; Sample 2), and https://aspredicted.org/2swy-sd7s.pdf (accessed on 28 October 2024; Sample 3). The procedure was ethically approved by the local ethics committee (reference: #101551806). Raw data and SPSS syntax are available at https://doi.org/10.17605/osf.io/y6evn (accessed on 28 October 2024).

2.1. Procedure

The data of all three samples were collected via German online surveys of similar structure and very comparable content. For Sample 1, we used the Software EFS Survey (Questback GmbH 2019). Due to updated requirements of our institution, all questionnaires were provided with LimeSurvey (2022, 2023) for Samples 2 and 3. At the beginning, all participants had to give their informed consent for participation. First questions referred to demographic information followed by questionnaires assessing the psychological constructs of interest: domain-general NFC, emotion regulation, self-control, affective well-being, global life satisfaction, domain-specific NFC, and domain-specific life satisfaction. Domain-specific questionnaires referred to study-specific NFC and study-related satisfaction for students and to job-specific NFC and job satisfaction for employed participants. They were provided only for participants that stated to currently study at university or to be employed, respectively. Finally, control variables were assessed, and information about options for compensation was provided. Participants studying psychology or a related subject at the university affiliated with the first author could receive study credit for compensation. All participants of Samples 1 and 2 could take part in a raffle to win 10, 20, or 50 euros. All valid participations in Sample 3 were compensated with 3.45 euros via prolific.co. For Sample 3, we followed requirements of the platform prolific.co and included two attention checks following the platform’s policy (Prolific 2023).

2.2. Participants

All three samples were recruited in Germany. Following the recommendation for robust correlation coefficients of Schönbrodt and Perugini (2013), a sample size of 250 was aimed at for all samples. The main characteristics of all samples are displayed in Table 1. For Samples 1 and 2, participants were recruited via social platforms, mailing lists, and private contacts. Data collection for Sample 1 took place during the first COVID-19 wave, so we used mainly online recruiting methods. For Sample 2, we used additionally printed flyers in public spaces. Sample 3 was recruited via prolific.co. Samples 2 and 3 aimed at replicating the results of Sample 1 to provide a valid basis for conclusions about the value of domain-specific conceptualizations of NFC in the context of subjective well-being.
Sample 1. The first dataset was collected between 17 June and 30 July 2020, and 549 individuals provided their consent to participate. Of them, 460 finalized all relevant questions, including a statement about their honesty of responses. We excluded participations when participants indicated answering dishonestly or without reading the questions, stated technical problems, or stated to currently suffer from a severe psychiatric disorder. The final sample consisted of N1 = 451 participants who were highly educated, with 92% having a university entrance qualification or university degree. The student subsample consisted of n1s = 258 participants, and the employed subsample of n1w = 159 participants, ranging between full-time jobs and part-time jobs less than 15 h a week. Of the student subsample, 34.7% (n = 90) studied psychology or a closely related subject. For the subsample of employed participants, the most frequent fields of occupation were social and cultural services (23.9%), health (18.2%), and business management and organization (8.8%). Effect size sensitivity was calculated post hoc with G*Power (Faul et al. 2009). It identified ρN1 ≥ 0.11, ρn1s ≥ 0.16, and ρn1w ≥ 0.20 as minimum population effect sizes detectable with 1 − β = 0.80 and α = 0.05 (one-tailed).
Sample 2. In the second data collection, from 30 May to 26 June 2022, 435 individuals agreed to the conditions of participation. Of them, 340 finalized all relevant questions, including a statement about their honesty of responses. Three participants were excluded because they stated to have answered dishonestly or without reading the questions. Two participants were excluded due to their information about suffering currently from severe psychiatric symptoms (psychiatric comorbidities for years, one person with recent inpatient treatment). Hence, the final sample size was N2 = 335 with n2s = 157 students and an employed subsample of n2w = 156 with participants working between full-time jobs and part-time jobs less than 15 h a week. Participants were highly educated, with 85.1% having a university entrance qualification or university degree. For the employed subsample, the most frequent fields of occupation were social and cultural services (17.3%), health (12.8%), business-related service (12.8%), construction (11.5%), and IT and science (9.6%). Of the student subsample, 54.1% (n = 85) studied psychology or a closely related subject. The data were collected after two years of the COVID-19 pandemic and three months after Russia invaded Ukraine. In total, 51.6% of participants stated to feel more burdened, 46.9% to worry more than usual. Effect size sensitivity was calculated post hoc with G*Power (Faul et al. 2009). It identified ρN2 ≥ 0.14 and ρn2s/n2w ≥ 0.20 as minimum population effect sizes detectable (1 − β = 0.80, α = 0.05, one-tailed).
Sample 3. Because the sizes of employed subsamples in Samples 1 and 2 were smaller than a priori analyses specified, the third data collection aimed only at employed participants. The survey was conducted between 20 January and 9 February 2023. Hence, for the recruitment via prolific corresponding inclusion/exclusion criteria were selected out of different criteria provided on the platform for sample selection: part-time or full-time employed with at least 20 h/week and fluency in German language at C1 level. An a priori power analysis (Faul et al. 2009) showed that 346 subjects would be needed to detect a small effect of r = 0.15 (α = 0.05, 1 − β = 0.80, two-tailed). Hence, 346 participants were the target sample size requested from Prolific. After 346 participations were completed, inclusion criteria were checked by answers to demographic questions in our survey. Five participants were excluded because they did not match the abovementioned selection criteria. Three participants were excluded because they failed attention checks, stated not having read questions completely, or were exceptionally fast. For those rejected participations, eight participants were additionally recruited so that the final sample consisted of N3 = 346 data sets. The most frequent fields of occupation were IT and science (31.8%), business-related service (16.8%), social and cultural services (14.2%), and health (8.4%). The final sample was highly educated, with 88.7% having a university entrance qualification or university degree. Similar to Sample 2, participants were asked whether they worry more or feel more burdened than usual. About one-third of participants agreed to at least one statement (worries: 29.5% and burdened: 31.8%).

2.3. Materials and Methods

2.3.1. Need for Cognition

The assessment of NFC was based on the study by Keller et al. (2017). Domain-general NFC was assessed with five German items, for example, “I like situations that require me to think hard” (Keller et al. 2017). Participants had to answer items on a 7-point scale ranging from −3 (completely disagree) to +3 (completely agree). Answers were aggregated as sum scores, with higher scores indicating higher NFC levels, theoretically ranging from −15 to +15.
Domain-specific NFC was assessed with five items based on Keller et al. (2017) and adapted to work and study contexts, respectively. Table 2 displays all items. Response format and scoring were similar to domain-general NFC. In all samples, domain-specific NFC correlated strongly with domain-general NFC (0.71 ≤ rs ≤ 0.82, p < 0.001).

2.3.2. Domain-General and Domain-Specific Life Satisfaction

For the assessment of global life satisfaction, we used a German version of the Satisfaction with Life Scale (Glaesmer et al. 2011). It consists of five items (e.g., “In most ways, my life is close to my ideal”) rated on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (completely disagree) to 7 (completely agree). Individual scores were the sum of item responses.
Job satisfaction was assessed with a German short scale consisting of eight items like “I’m really enjoying my work” (Fischer and Lück 2014). Items were rated on a 5-point scale whose labels differed depending on the respective item (e.g., 1 = wrong to 5 = true). All answers were summed up.
We assessed study-related satisfaction with a German nine-item short scale (Westermann et al. 1996) representing three dimensions: satisfaction with study conditions, coping with study-related stress, and subject-related satisfaction (e.g., “I really enjoy what I study”). Participants had to answer on an 11-point rating scale ranging from 0 (completely disagree) to 100 (completely agree). Inverted items were recoded, and answers were averaged per dimension so that higher scores indicated higher study-related satisfaction for every dimension.

2.3.3. Affective Well-Being

Positive and negative affect were assessed with the German version of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (Janke and Glöckner-Rist 2014). It consists of 10 adjectives describing positive affect (e.g., “active”) and 10 adjectives describing negative affect (e.g., “distressed”). Participants rated each adjective on a 5-point rating scale (1 = not at all to 5 = very), referring to how they feel in general. Scores were calculated separately for positive and negative affect as sums of item responses.

2.3.4. Emotion Regulation Strategies

Emotion regulation was assessed with a German version of the Regulation Questionnaire (Abler and Kessler 2009) with six items assessing cognitive reappraisal (e.g., changing thoughts for more positive emotions) and four items assessing suppression (e.g., not expressing emotions). Item responses referred to a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not true at all) to 7 (completely true). Scores were calculated for each type of strategy as averaged item responses.

2.3.5. Trait Self-Control

We assessed trait self-control with a German 13-item questionnaire (Bertrams and Dickhäuser 2009). Items represent behavior indicating dispositional self-control capacity (e.g., ability to work effectively toward long-term goals) and were rated on a 5-point scale from 1 (completely disagree) to 5 (completely agree). Item responses were averaged.
Table 3 presents descriptive statistics and reliability for all implemented instruments.

2.3.6. Further Variables

We assessed demographic data and information about living situation, job situation, or subject and duration of study. The assessment of further variables differed slightly between the three samples due to different social conditions at the time of each data collection. As the surveys of Sample 1 and Sample 2 were conducted when the COVID-19 pandemic had stronger influence on everyday life and potentially on well-being, too, we added items focusing on COVID-19-related conditions (https://osf.io/y6evn/?view_only=4ae7e4377a5546ed8f7c9ae3dc527292, accessed on 28 October 2024). In Sample 3, two more general questions were asked as follows: whether individuals currently feel more burdened than usual and whether they worry more than usual.
In all samples, two questions referring to data validity were placed at the end of the survey, referring to honesty of responses and reading all questions before answering.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

All analyses were conducted with IBM SPSS Statistics (version 29). The distribution of variables of interest was tested with QQ-Plots, histograms, and Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests. Due to deviations from normality, Spearman rank coefficients were used for correlation analyses. For hypothesis testing, we applied the Bonferroni correction to adjust the significance level by considering 80 single comparisons (α = 0.05/80), resulting in α ≈ 0.001. Correlations were interpreted following the recommendations by Gignac and Szodorai (2016).
Incremental predictive value was tested using hierarchical regression analyses using robust analyses based on 1000 bootstrap samples with 95% bias-corrected and accelerated (BCa) confidence intervals. In a first step, age or gender were included in the regression analyses if correlated with the respective criterion with p ≤ 0.05. Due to low frequencies of participants choosing “diverse” for gender (per subsample, n = 0–2), point-biserial correlations as well as regression analyses, including gender, were calculated excluding diverse participants. Gender and age were not generally included in all analyses because results would change very unlikely if no correlations were observed, and generally including them would shift the focus to demographic variables that are not theoretically driven relevant for the current research. Second, domain-general NFC was included before domain-specific NFC related to one’s job or studies.
For estimating overall effects of domain-specific NFC across all samples, we analyzed multilevel models with data collection (i.e., Sample 1, Sample 2, and Sample 3) and domain (studies vs. work) as random factors. Multilevel analyses were conducted with RStudio (version 2023.12.0; Posit Team 2023) using R (Version 4.2.1; R Core Team 2022) and the packages lme4 (Bates et al. 2015) and lmerTest (Kuznetsova et al. 2017). Mediation analyses were calculated for the prediction of all well-being indicators NFC was associated with. Analyses were performed with PROCESS (version 4.0; Hayes 2022), calculating 95% BCa confidence intervals based on 1000 bootstrap samples. Significant effects were indicated by confidence intervals excluding zero.

3. Results

3.1. Associations of Need for Cognition with Subjective Well-Being

Results of correlation analyses with NFC are displayed in Table 4 and Table 5. Intercorrelations of all well-being variables, emotion regulation, and self-control are uploaded in the electronic supplementary material (see Tables S1–S3).
In all samples, both domain-general and domain-specific NFC was strongest associated with positive affect (0.25 ≤ rs ≤ 0.39). Negative affect was not associated with NFC. In most (sub)samples, NFC was weakly associated with life satisfaction, which was strongest for job-specific NFC (0.21 ≤ rs ≤ 0.24). Associations of NFC with domain-specific life satisfaction were strongest for job satisfaction and subject-related satisfaction with one’s studies (0.16 ≤ rs ≤ 0.36). NFC was not associated with satisfaction with study conditions and only in Sample 2 weakly with satisfaction related to coping with study-related stress.
The sizes of correlations of NFC with cognitive reappraisal and self-control were heterogenous, being very small and non-significant in some sub(samples) and moderate in others (e.g., with self-control rs = 0.20/0.26 in Sample 3, with reappraisal 0.16 ≤ rs ≤ 0.22 in Sample 1). NFC did not correlate with suppression.

3.2. Incremental Validity of Domain-Specific Need for Cognition

For all well-being indicators that were significantly correlated with domain-specific or domain-general NFC, hierarchical regression analyses were conducted to identify the incremental value of domain-specific NFC. Additionally, multilevel models considering time of data collection (i.e., Sample 1 to Sample 3) and domain (study-related vs. job-related) as random factors (random intercept) were calculated. Similar to hierarchical regression analyses, multilevel models were calculated for well-being indicators correlated with NFC in at least one sample. Due to sample-dependent (non-significant) effects of gender and age together with differences in assessing age in Sample 1 compared to Samples 2 and 3, control variables were not considered in these multilevel analyses. We calculated two models for predicting each criterion by including general NFC first and domain-specific NFC second to calculate ∆R2. For domain-specific well-being (i.e., study-related and job satisfaction), we calculated separate analyses in the overall working sample and the overall study sample, respectively. In these analyses, we used only data collection as a random factor because domain-specific well-being assessments were not as comparable as necessary to define them as two domain-specific aspects of the same factor.

3.2.1. Positive Affect

Results of sample-specific regression analyses for positive affect are displayed in Table 6. In both student samples, general NFC predicted positive affect with β = 0.17/0.32 (p = 0.005, p < 0.001). Adding study-specific NFC explained no significant additional variance (p = 0.100/0.121). In employed subsamples, general NFC predicted positive affect with 0.30 ≤ β ≤ 0.35 (p < 0.001). Job-specific NFC had a small incremental predictive value (0.018 ≤ ∆R2 ≤ 0.037, 0.24 ≤ β ≤ 0.29, 0.008 ≤ p ≤ 0.015).
Results of random effect models to calculate an overall effect across all samples and both types of domain-specificity of NFC (i.e., study-specific and job-specific) are displayed in Table A1. They show a small incremental value of domain-specific NFC with ∆R2marginal = 0.024 and β = 0.22 [0.13, 0.30] over and above domain-general NFC. The predictive value of domain-general NFC decreased from β = 0.31 to β = 0.15 after including domain-specific NFC, which indicates redundancy effects. Together, domain-general and domain-specific NFC explained 12% of variance in positive affect (R2marginal = 0.115).

3.2.2. Life Satisfaction

Table 7 displays sample-specific hierarchical regression analyses for life satisfaction.
In all samples, domain-specific NFC incrementally predicted life satisfaction over and above general NFC (study-specific NFC: ∆R2 = 0.022/0.041, β = 0.20/0.26, p = 0.017/0.010; job-specific NFC: 0.021 ≤ ∆R2 ≤ 0.027, 0.22 ≤ β ≤ 0.27, 0.006 ≤ p ≤ 0.044).
Multilevel results for identifying an overall effect across all samples and both study- and job-specific NFC are displayed in Table A2. The overall predictive value of domain-specific NFC for predicting life satisfaction was β = 24 [0.16, 0.33]. The incremental predictive value over and above domain-general NFC was small, with ∆R2marginal = 0.026. The predictive value of domain-general NFC decreased from β = 0.11 to β = −0.06 (n.s.) after including domain-specific NFC, indicating redundancy effects. Together, domain-general and domain-specific NFC explained 4% of variance in life satisfaction (R2marginal = 0.039).

3.2.3. Study-Related Satisfaction

We analyzed the incremental value of study-specific NFC for predicting subject-related satisfaction in both samples and for predicting satisfaction related to coping with study-related stress in Sample 2, corresponding with significant correlational findings for dimensions of study-related satisfaction (see Table 8).
In the first step, study-related satisfaction was predicted by general NFC only in Sample 1, referring to subject-related satisfaction with β = 0.24, p < 0.001. Further, study-specific NFC had an incremental value for predicting subject-related satisfaction in both samples (∆R2 = 0.082/0.071, β = 0.39/0.35, p < 0.001) and coping with study-related stress in Sample 2 (∆R2 = 0.033, β = 0.24, p = 0.022).
Results of random effect models to calculate an overall effect across all samples are displayed in Table A3. The overall incremental value of study-specific NFC was ∆R2marginal = 0.079 for predicting subject-related satisfaction (β = 0.38 [0.26, 0.50]) and ∆R2marginal = 0.016 for predicting satisfaction referring to study-related coping (β = 0.17 [0.04, 0.30]). Together, domain-general and study-specific NFC explained 12% of variance in subject-related satisfaction (R2marginal = 0.117) and 2% of variance in satisfaction related to coping with study-related stress (R2marginal = 0.021). For subject-related satisfaction, redundancy effects were indicated by β decreasing from 0.20 to −0.06 (n.s.) after including study-specific NFC.

3.2.4. Job Satisfaction

The results of regression analyses predicting job satisfaction are provided in Table 9. In Samples 1 and 3, job-specific NFC incrementally predicted job satisfaction over and above general NFC (∆R2 = 0.07, β = 0.38/0.48, p < 0.001). In Sample 2, general NFC explained 7.7% of variance with no incremental value of job-specific NFC (∆R2 = 0.007, β = 0.15, p = 0.278).
Results of random effect models to calculate an overall effect across all samples are displayed in Table A4. The overall incremental predictive value of job-specific NFC for predicting job satisfaction over and above domain-general NFC was ∆R2marginal = 0.047 with β = 0.36 [0.24, 0.47]. Redundancy effects were indicated for domain-general NFC with a decreased β from 0.16 to −0.12 after including job-specific NFC. Together, domain-general and job-specific NFC explained 7% of variance in job satisfaction (R2marginal = 0.072).

3.3. Mediation Analyses

To examine the role of self-regulatory processes for predicting well-being by NFC, we ran separate mediation analyses in each sample using emotion regulation strategies and self-control as parallel mediators (see Table 10). Analyses were calculated using general or domain-specific NFC as a predictor and a well-being indicator as a criterion. As predicted criteria, we used all well-being indicators except negative affect and satisfaction with study conditions because both had no observable associations with NFC.
Except for two subsamples, respectively, we found evidence for a mediating role of emotion regulation and/or self-control for the prediction of positive affect and life satisfaction by NFC. Thereby, for predicting positive affect, a direct effect of NFC remained in all cases. For predicting life satisfaction, a direct effect remained only for job-specific NFC in Sample 3, with an existing indirect effect through regulatory processes. The other mediation effects for predicting life satisfaction were complete mediations.
Results for domain-specific satisfaction related to one’s studies or job were more ambiguous. Study-related satisfaction referring to coping was not predicted indirectly by NFC but directly by study-specific NFC in Sample 2. Satisfaction with one’s study subject was predicted by study-specific NFC directly in Sample 1 and Sample 2, with an additional indirect effect in Sample 2. For job satisfaction, emotion regulation and self-control had mediating effects in half of the (sub)samples. In the other cases, (domain-specific) NFC predicted job satisfaction without mediating regulatory processes. Only in Sample 1, the effect of job-specific NFC was completely mediated. In general, significant mediating paths mostly refer to reappraisal and self-control.

4. Discussion

The current research had two aims. We investigated the potential advantages of considering the NFC domain-specifically in addition to the common domain-general conceptualization in the context of subjective well-being. Furthermore, we examined the mediating role of emotion regulation strategies and self-control as explaining processes.
Our results are based on three data sets, including subsamples of students and employed participants, with a total sample size of N = 1132. As expected, domain-specific and domain-general NFC were highly correlated. Descriptively, NFC levels were higher in working compared to student subsamples. Compared to domain-general NFC, domain-specific NFC was about 0.5 SD reduced in students. We can only speculate about the reasons for those differences. One reason for lower study-specific NFC levels compared to domain-general NFC might be that study subjects cover a broader range of content that may be of less interest for some students, so that study-related effort is likely to be both sometimes intrinsically motivated and related to NFC as well as sometimes extrinsically motivated. Further, opportunities to enact higher NFC levels when learning for exams and dealing with study content are likely to depend on study curricula, which may have influenced answering study-specific NFC items. Furthermore, the reduction in study-specific NFC compared to domain-general NFC may have been associated with adverse study conditions with long periods of home studying instead of regularly studying on campus. Although NFC levels above the theoretical mean conform with previous findings in students, we would not have expected similar and even higher NFC levels for working participants. Prospective studies should examine whether our NFC levels above the theoretical mean are representative for working individuals or mirror selection bias.

4.1. Associations Between NFC and Subjective Well-Being

Consistently in all samples, we found moderate associations of domain-general and domain-specific NFC with positive affect (0.25 ≤ rs ≤ 0.39), which was in line with a meta-analytically reported moderate association of ρ = 0.20 (Zerna et al. 2024). Also in line with previous results, we found no association with negative affect and satisfaction with study conditions (Grass et al. 2018, 2023; Zerna et al. 2024). Similar to previous meta-analytic findings (Zerna et al. 2024), associations with life satisfaction were small to moderate, ranging between 0.09 ≤ rs ≤ 0.24 and being strongest for job-specific NFC.
Job satisfaction was weakly to moderately associated with domain-general NFC (0.11 ≤ rs ≤ 0.26). Associations with job-specific NFC were at least medium and descriptively stronger (0.25 ≤ rs ≤ 0.33). Subject-related satisfaction with one’s studies correlated also weakly to moderately with domain-general NFC (0.16 ≤ rs ≤ 0.31) as well as with study-specific NFC (0.25 ≤ rs ≤ 0.36). Again, associations with domain-specific NFC were descriptively stronger than with domain-general NFC. We found small to medium associations (p < 0.05) between both domain-general and domain-specific NFC, with satisfaction referring to coping with study-related stress only in Sample 2 (rs = 0.16/0.22). That is, both domain-general and domain-specific NFC showed comparable correlational patterns in all samples.
Our results provide cautious evidence for stronger associations between domain-specific life satisfaction and domain-specific NFC compared to domain-general NFC. Global life satisfaction was also strongly associated with job-specific NFC, indicating that NFC in the job context can be relevant not only for domain-specific life satisfaction.

4.2. Incremental Predictive Value of Domain-Specific Need for Cognition

Inspired by a previous study on children (Keller et al. 2017), we examined the meaningfulness of a domain-specific conceptualization of NFC in the sense of incremental predictive validity for subjective well-being criteria. Subjective well-being refers to emotional experience in the sense of positive or negative affect and global or domain-specific evaluations of one’s life (e.g., Busseri 2018; Diener 1984). We thereby examined the incremental value of domain-specific NFC for predicting both domain-general and domain-specific well-being (except for negative affect, which was uncorrelated with NFC).
Corresponding with at least moderate associations of (domain-specific) NFC with general positive affect, domain-general NFC explained a small to moderate amount of variance in all samples (overall R2 = 0.091 across all samples). The overall incremental value of domain-specific NFC across domains and samples was ∆R2 = 0.024. Although study-specific NFC had no incremental predictive value over and above domain-general NFC, job-specific NFC explained small incremental variance in all three samples. Hence, the prediction of positive affect was improved by including job-specific NFC for employed participants.
Only in two out of five subsamples, domain-general NFC had a small, significant predictive value for predicting life satisfaction. Adding study-specific or job-specific NFC in a further step incrementally improved the prediction of life satisfaction in all subsamples. The overall incremental value of domain-specific NFC for predicting life satisfaction across samples and domains was ∆R2 = 0.026. Hence, for the prediction of life satisfaction, domain-specific NFC was equally to even more important than domain-general NFC. This finding was partly surprising because, following the Brunswik symmetry principle (e.g., Kretzschmar et al. 2018), we would have assumed general NFC to be more meaningful for the prediction of global life satisfaction compared to domain-specific NFC. One possible explanation refers to the idea that individuals may think of specific life contexts as anchoring references when asked to evaluate their life satisfaction in general.
That is supported by moderate to strong associations of both job satisfaction and study-subject-related satisfaction with global life satisfaction in our samples (0.36 ≤ rs ≤ 0.48). Enjoying study-related or job-related cognitive activities in the sense of domain-specific NFC may be of relevance for life satisfaction, especially when studying or working is a large part of everyday life. The latter was probably true for most employed participants because most of them stated to work full-time. For students, we assume that the relevance of studies for individual evaluations of life is more heterogeneous. To gain more insight into possible explanations for the predictive value of domain-specific NFC for life satisfaction, future studies could further examine the frame of reference when individuals rate global life satisfaction. We also cannot exclude situation-specific influences on our results since data collections for Samples 1 and 2 were conducted during the COVID-19 pandemic, which changed life circumstances, including studies and job conditions at that time meaningfully.
Remarkably, the explained variance proportions by domain-general and domain-specific NFC were quite different comparing positive affect and life satisfaction with R2marginal = 0.12 for positive affect and R2marginal = 0.04 for life satisfaction when controlling for variance explained by the random factors domain and time of data collection. Hence, positive affect could be much better predicted by domain-specific and domain-general NFC than a cognitive evaluation in the sense of life satisfaction, which underlines the relevance of NFC as a resource for affective outcomes and emotional adaptation (Bertrams and Dickhäuser 2012; Grass et al. 2023; Zerna et al. 2024).
While general NFC predicted subject-related satisfaction with one’s studies only in Sample 1, study-specific NFC had an incremental predictive value in all conducted regressions: Subject-related satisfaction with one’s studies was incrementally predicted by study-specific NFC in both samples. Study-specific NFC explained small to medium variance proportions with overall ∆R2 = 0.079 and β = 0.38 [0.26, 0.50] across both samples. In Sample 2, also satisfaction referring to coping with study-related stress was incrementally predicted by study-specific NFC. Overall, NFC explained a much smaller variance proportion in coping-related satisfaction (R2 = 0.021) compared to subject-related satisfaction (R2 = 0.117). The findings confirmed our hypothesis of study-specific NFC adding predictive value, especially for the prediction of subject-related satisfaction with one’s studies. They also highlight that study-specific NFC, though strongly correlated to general NFC, is not completely redundant to it. Instead, it covers unique facets of individual behavior for predicting some criteria.
Two of three analyses regarding job satisfaction indicated an incremental value of job-specific NFC over and above general NFC. The random effect model across all samples identified an overall effect of ∆R2 = 0.047 with β = 0.36 [0.24, 47]. This finding further supports the idea of a meaningful conceptualization of NFC both domain-generally and domain-specifically (Keller et al. 2017).

4.3. Mediating Processes

For adding to insights in processes underlying implications of NFC on subjective well-being, we built on previous research (e.g., Bertrams and Dickhäuser 2012; Grass et al. 2023; Nishiguchi et al. 2016; Zerna et al. 2022) and examined emotion regulation and trait self-control as mediating variables. Confirming previous research, NFC was at least weakly positively related to cognitive reappraisal and self-control and not associated with emotion suppression in most subsamples. Matching this result, indirect effects of NFC in the prediction of well-being indicators occurred mainly via self-control or reappraisal. Mediation analyses provided evidence for a mediating role of reappraisal and self-control, mainly for the prediction of positive affect, global life satisfaction, and, in part, job satisfaction. In many cases, a direct effect of NFC remained. Overlapping confidence intervals indicated that indirect effects of domain-specific and domain-general NFC were comparable in size. Altogether, the results provided evidence for an at least partial mediating role of trait self-control and/or reappraisal for the prediction of positive affect, job-specific, and global life satisfaction. That finding confirms previous research identifying NFC as a resource for (emotional) challenges in life and self-regulatory processes (e.g., Bertrams and Dickhäuser 2012; Bye and Pushkar 2009; Strobel et al. 2021).
In some subsamples, associations between NFC and job-specific or global life satisfaction were completely attributable to differences in self-control and emotion regulation, whereas the association between positive affect and NFC was only in part attributable to self-control and reappraisal in all samples. Individuals with higher NFC levels may not only report increased positive affect because they tend to be more long-term oriented and adapt helpful perspectives. Additional possible explanations are, for example, that they tend to approach challenges that can reduce anxiety and avoidance behavior or that they tend to really enjoy potentially exhausting and strenuous cognitive tasks, which are likely to be part of one’s studies and jobs following higher education, as most participants had. In general, the remaining direct effects of NFC indicated more complex explanatory paths for associations with subjective well-being. Between-sample differences in effect sizes of (in)direct effects suggest that future studies should investigate possible moderating variables.

4.4. Limitations

To allow for increased generalizability of results, we examined our research questions in different samples and collected data at different times. Although we used different methods for recruitment, all three samples were highly educated, which may have led to variance restriction and reduced generalizability. To reach not only highly educated individuals, future studies should use platforms like we used in Sample 3 and choose selection criteria that aim at low to middle education levels for more balanced proportions of different education levels in samples. However, even in Sample 3, NFC values were above the theoretical mean of 0, and higher NFC values may be confounded with increased willingness to participate due to increased scientific interest (Feist 2012). Higher education levels among survey participants are likely to be found due to more familiarity with science and respective self-selection bias. Furthermore, especially Samples 1 and 2 participated during a challenging time of a worldwide pandemic (COVID-19) and the beginning of the Russia–Ukraine war that had negative implications for well-being in European countries (e.g., Pavlova et al. 2024; Scharbert et al. 2024). On the one hand, this limits generalizability. On the other hand, the war is still ongoing, other political and social challenges like climate change can be important situational influences on well-being (Barchielli et al. 2022), and results for most well-being indicators were quite stable among subsamples and comparable to previous results. That is, we assume mean level changes in well-being due to crises like the COVID-19 pandemic are not necessarily changing correlations of well-being with NFC, which is indicated by results of multilevel analyses.
For assessing NFC domain-specifically, we used items closely based on Keller et al. (2017), which have been developed for the purpose of assessing NFC domain-specifically but have not been used in many studies yet. Consequently, broad evidence for its validity is still lacking for domain-specific NFC items, and our results are promising but preliminary, as described in Keller et al. (2017). Estimates of reliability (i.e., Cronbach’s α) were very good, and our results with quite comparable patterns to general NFC argue for the validity of both scales. Future research should continue to use these items and provide further indicators of their psychometric quality. Questionnaires to assess NFC domain-specifically and domain-generally were presented always in the same order with domain-general items first with the idea that participants would pay more attention to the domain-specific character when having answered similar items without domain-specific context compared to the opposite order. The downside of that decision are possible sequence effects for completing the questionnaires referring to domain-general and domain-specific NFC comprising very similar items. As expected, domain-specific and domain-general NFC were highly correlated, and due to probable multicollinearity, regression weights should be interpreted carefully when both are included. Our research questions have focused on examining the incremental value of domain-specific NFC. Consequently, our conclusions did not rely on regression weights but mainly considered changes in explained variance in models including both domain-specific and domain-general NFC compared to models including only domain-general NFC.
Online surveys are very common to efficiently assess individuals in different regions and to lower the threshold for participation. However, they entail the risk that we were not able to ensure comparable conditions and an undisturbed environment during responding for the participants, so that conditions of participation may have differed and may have affected the validity of our results. Anonymous participation should encourage honest responses, but we cannot rule out socially desirable response patterns completely.
The cross-sectional design of our study does not allow for conclusions on causal relations between NFC and subjective well-being, so unidirectional theoretical explanations for associations between NFC and well-being could not be explicitly tested with our data. Hence, using NFC as a predictor and well-being indicators as criteria in regression and mediation analyses followed theoretical assumptions (for an overview, see also Zerna et al. 2024) and only one directional path of a probably reciprocal relation. Examining causality with longitudinal designs should be part of future research. For example, one previous study on depressive patients indicated NFC as “a personal capacity that could not only favor the change in self-regulation as a consequence of decreasing depressive symptomatology but rather serve as an independent and at least partial catalyst for this change” (Strobel et al. 2021, p. 11). For research on reciprocal paths, including effects of well-being on NFC, prospective studies should also consider cognitive motivation from a state perspective (Blaise et al. 2021) instead of focusing on its trait aspects as we did. Examining causal pathways between NFC and well-being with longitudinal designs should be part of future research.

5. Conclusions

The current research provides encouraging evidence that assessing NFC domain-specifically meaningfully complements its domain-general conceptualization. Domain-specific and domain-general NFC were strongly associated. We found comparable correlation patterns of domain-specific and domain-general NFC with well-being indicators, self-control, and emotion regulation. Especially life satisfaction, both domain-general and domain-specific, was partly strongly associated with domain-specific NFC. A domain-specific perspective incrementally increased explained variances of study-related and job satisfaction. For employed individuals, job-specific NFC additionally had an incremental value for the prediction of domain-general well-being in the sense of positive affect and global life satisfaction. Further, our research confirmed previous research that reported self-control and the emotion regulation strategy reappraisal as partially mediating variables for the prediction of well-being by NFC. Our results provide further evidence for NFC being a resource for subjective well-being and suggest that domain-specific aspects of NFC can relevantly add to our understanding of the implications of interindividual differences in NFC.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/jintelligence12110110/s1, Table S1: Intercorrelations Sample 1; Table S2: Intercorrelations Sample 2; Table S3: Intercorrelations Sample 3.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.G. and A.S.; methodology, J.G.; validation, J.G. and A.S.; formal analysis, J.G.; investigation, J.G.; resources, A.S.; data curation, J.G.; writing—original draft preparation, J.G.; writing—review and editing, J.G. and A.S.; supervision, A.S.; project administration, J.G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee, Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences, TU Chemnitz, and Ethics Committee TU Chemnitz (ref.: V-387-15-JG-Domänenspezifisch-18052020; #101551806).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in this study.

Data Availability Statement

The original data presented in this study are openly available in https://doi.org/10.17605/osf.io/y6evn.

Acknowledgments

We cordially thank Lisa Knauer, Clara Stosch, Julie Gallert, Lea Hennig, and Anica Stabel for their important support during the preparation and conduct of data collections. We further thank Markus Burkhardt for his support with multilevel analyses.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Appendix A. Additional Statistical Results

Table A1. Hierarchical random effects model for predicting positive affect by Need for Cognition.
Table A1. Hierarchical random effects model for predicting positive affect by Need for Cognition.
Model 1Model 2
PredictorsbSEβtpdfbSEβtpdf
(Intercept)29.390.85−0.03 [−0.29, 0.23]34.54<0.001107129.520.69−0.02 [0.23, 0.18]42.77<0.0011070
NFCgeneral0.370.040.31 [0.25, 0.36]10.44<0.00110710.170.050.15 [0.06, 0.23]3.380.0011070
NFCdomain 0.22 0.040.22 [0.13, 0.30]5.02<0.0011070
Random effects
τ00 sample0.580.44
τ00 domain0.860.46
ICC.04.03
R2marg./R2cond.0.091/0.1280.115/0.138
R2marg.0.0910.024
Note. N/observations = 1076. NFCgeneral = domain-general Need for Cognition. NFCdomain = study-specific Need for Cognition or job-specific Need for Cognition. τ00 sample = variance explained by time of data collection (N = 3: Sample 1, Sample 2, and Sample 3). τ00 domain = variance explained by domain (N = 2: study vs. job-related). ICC = intraclass correlation coefficient.
Table A2. Hierarchical random effects model for predicting life satisfaction by Need for Cognition.
Table A2. Hierarchical random effects model for predicting life satisfaction by Need for Cognition.
Model 1Model 2
PredictorsbSEβtpdfbSEβtpdf
(Intercept)4.640.22−0.02 [−0.36, 0.32]21.20<0.00110714.67 0.19−0.01 [−0.30, 0.28]24.36<0.0011070
NFCgeneral0.030.010.11 [0.06, 0.17]3.82<0.0011071−0.020.01−0.06 [−0.15, 0.02]−1.430.1531070
NFCdomain 0.05 0.010.24 [0.16, 0.33]5.53<0.0011070
Random effects
τ00 sample0.120.10
τ00 domain0.010.00
ICC0.080.07
R2marg./R2cond.0.013/0.0930.039/0.103
R2marg.0.0130.026
Note: N/observations = 1076. NFCgeneral = domain-general Need for Cognition. NFCdomain = study-specific Need for Cognition or job-specific Need for Cognition. τ00 sample = variance explained by time of data collection (N = 3: Sample 1, Sample 2, and Sample 3). τ00 domain = variance explained by domain (N = 2: study vs. job-related). ICC = intraclass correlation coefficient.
Table A3. Hierarchical random effects model for predicting study-related satisfaction by Need for Cognition.
Table A3. Hierarchical random effects model for predicting study-related satisfaction by Need for Cognition.
Satisfaction with Study Subject
Model 1Model 2
PredictorsbSEβtpdfbSEβtpdf
(Intercept)68.631.660.00 [−0.09, 0.09]41.42<0.00141169.47 1.600.00 [−0.09, 0.09]43.55<0.001410
NFCgeneral0.78 0.190.20 [0.10, 0.29]4.06<0.001411−0.240.25−0.06 [−0.18, 0.06]−0.950.342410
NFCstudy 1.26 0.210.38 [0.26, 0.50]6.07<0.001410
Random effects
τ00 sample0.000.00
R2marg.0.0380.117
R2marg.0.0380.079
Coping with Study-Related Stress
(Intercept)58.62 2.16−0.00 [−0.10, 0.10]27.11<0.00141159.12 2.16−0.00 [−0.10, 0.10]27.42<0.001410
NFCgeneral0.37 0.250.07 [−0.02, 0.17]1.480.140411−0.22 0.34−0.04 [−0.17, 0.09]−0.660.510410
NFCstudy 0.74 0.280.17 [0.04, 0.30]2.620.009410
Random effectsa
τ00 sample0.000.00
R2marg.a0.0050.021
R2marg.0.0050.016
Note. N/observations = 415. NFCgeneral = domain-general Need for Cognition. NFCstudy = study-specific Need for Cognition. τ00 sample = variance explained by time of data collection (N = 2: Sample 1, Sample 2). a Intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) and R2conditional not reported because no variance explained by time of data collection (τ = 0.0).
Table A4. Hierarchical random effects model for predicting job satisfaction by Need for Cognition.
Table A4. Hierarchical random effects model for predicting job satisfaction by Need for Cognition.
Model 1Model 2
PredictorsbSEβtpdfbSEβtpdf
(Intercept)28.25 0.880.07 [−0.20, 0.34]31.95<0.00165728.32 0.820.07 [−0.19, 0.32]34.36<0.001656
NFCgeneral0.18 0.040.16 [0.09, 0.23]4.20<0.001657−0.14 0.07−0.12 [−0.24, −0.00]−2.000.046656
NFCjob 0.360.060.36 [0.24, 0.47]5.88<0.001656
Random effects
τ00 sample1.801.52
ICC0.050.05
R2marg./R2cond.0.025/0.0770.072/0.116
R2marg.0.0250.047
Note. N/observations = 661. NFCgeneral = domain-general Need for Cognition. NFCjob = job-specific Need for Cognition. τ00 sample = variance explained by time of data collection (N = 3: Sample 1, Sample 2, and Sample 3). τ00 domain = variance explained by domain (N = 2: study vs. job-related). ICC = intraclass correlation coefficient.

References

  1. Abler, B., and H. Kessler. 2009. Emotion Regulation Questionnaire—Eine deutschsprachige Fassung des ERQ von Gross und John [Emotion Regulation Questionnaire—A German version of the ERQ by Gross and John]. Diagnostica 55: 144–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Ackerman, P. 1996. A theory of adult intellectual development: Process, personality, interests, and knowledge. Intelligence 22: 227–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Aldao, A., S. Nolen-Hoeksema, and S. Schweizer. 2010. Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review 30: 217–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Baer, L. H., N. Tabri, M. Blair, D. Bye, K. Z. H. Li, and D. Pushkar. 2013. Longitudinal associations of Need for Cognition, cognitive activity, and depressive symptomatology with cognitive function in recent retirees. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B 68: 655–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Barchielli, B., C. Cricenti, F. Gallè, E. A. Sabella, F. Liguori, G. Da Molin, G. Liguori, G. B. Orsi, A. M. Giannini, S. Ferracuti, and et al. 2022. Climate changes, natural resources depletion, COVID-19 pandemic, and Russian-Ukrainian war: What is the impact on habits change and mental health? International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Ealth 19: 11929. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Bates, D., M. Mächler, B. Bolker, and S. Walker. 2015. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67: 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Bertrams, A., and O. Dickhäuser. 2009. Messung dispositioneller Selbstkontroll-Kapazität. Eine deutsche Adaptation der Kurzform der Self-Control Scale (SCS-K-D) [Measuring dispositional self-control capacity. A German adaptation of the short form of the Self-Control Scale (SCS-K-D)]. Diagnostica 55: 2–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Bertrams, A., and O. Dickhäuser. 2012. Passionate thinkers feel better: Self-control capacity as mediator of the relationship between need for cognition and affective adjustment. Journal of Individual Differences 33: 69–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Blaise, M., T. Marksteiner, A. Krispenz, and A. Bertrams. 2021. Measuring motivation for cognitive effort as state. Frontiers in Psychology 12: 785094. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Busseri, M. A. 2018. Examining the structure of subjective well-being through meta-analysis of the associations among positive affect, negative affect, and life satisfaction. Personality and Individual Differences 122: 68–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Bye, D., and D. Pushkar. 2009. How need for cognition and perceived control are differentially linked to emotional outcomes in the transition to retirement. Motivation and Emotion 33: 320–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Cacioppo, J. T., and R. E. Petty. 1982. The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 42: 116–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Cacioppo, J. T., R. E. Petty, J. A. Feinstein, and W. B. G. Jarvis. 1996. Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychological Bulletin 119: 197–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. de Ridder, D., F. Kroese, and M. Gillebaart. 2018. Whatever happened to self-control? A proposal for integrating notions from trait self-control studies into state self-control research. Motivation Science 4: 39–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. de Ridder, D., G. Lensvelt-Mulders, C. Finkenauer, F. M. Stok, and R. F. Baumeister. 2012. Taking stock of self-control: A meta-analysis of how trait self-control relates to a wide range of behaviors. Personality and Social Psychology Review 16: 76–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Diener, E. 1984. Subjective Well-Being. Psychological Bulletin 95: 542–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Diener, E., R. E. Lucas, and C. N. Scollon. 2006. Beyond the hedonic treadmill: Revising the adaptation theory of well-being. American Psychologist 61: 305–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Epstein, S., R. Pacini, V. Denes-Raj, and H. Heier. 1996. Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-rational thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 71: 390–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Faul, F., E. Erdfelder, A. Buchner, and A.-G. Lang. 2009. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods 41: 1149–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Feist, G. J. 2012. Predicting interest in and attitudes toward science from personality and need for cognition. Personality and Individual Differences 52: 771–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Fischer, L., and H. E. Lück. 2014. Allgemeine Arbeitszufriedenheit [General job satisfaction]. Zusammenstellung Sozialwissenschaftlicher Items und Skalen. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Fleischhauer, M., S. Enge, B. Brocke, J. Ullrich, A. A. Strobel, and A. A. Strobel. 2010. Same or different? Clarifying the relationship of need for cognition to personality and intelligence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 36: 82–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Fujita, K. 2011. On conceptualizing self-control as more than the effortful inhibition of impulses. Personality and Social Psychology Review 15: 352–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Gignac, G. E., and E. T. Szodorai. 2016. Effect size guidelines for individual differences researchers. Personality and Individual Differences 102: 74–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Glaesmer, H., G. Grande, E. Braehler, and M. Roth. 2011. The German version of the satisfactionwith life scale (SWLS) psychometric properties, validity, and population-based norms. European Journal of Psychological Assessment 27: 127–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Goldin, P. R., K. McRae, W. Ramel, and J. J. Gross. 2008. The neural bases of emotion regulation: Reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion. Biological Psychiatry 63: 577–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Grass, J., A. Strobel, and A. Strobel. 2017. Cognitive investments in academic success: The role of Need for Cognition at university. Frontiers in Psychology 8: 790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Grass, J., J. Dörendahl, T. Losereit, S. Greiff, and A. Strobel. 2023. Thinking to promote happiness: Need for cognition, subjective well-being, and burnout in different populations. Journal of Research in Personality 105: 104383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Grass, J., N. John, and A. Strobel. 2018. Freude am Denken als Schlüssel zum Erfolg? Die Bedeutung von Need for Cognition für subjektives Erleben und Leistung im Studium [The joy of thinking as the key to success? The importance of Need for Cognition for subjective experience and achievement in academic studies]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie 32: 145–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Grecucci, A., I. Messina, L. Amodeo, G. Lapomarda, C. Crescentini, H. Dadomo, M. Panzeri, A. Theuninck, and J. Frederickson. 2020. A dual route model for regulating emotions: Comparing models, techniques and biological mechanisms. Frontiers in Psychology 11: 930. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Gross, J. J., and O. P. John. 2003. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 85: 348–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Harman, J. L. 2011. Individual differences in need for cognition and decision making in the Iowa Gambling Task. Personality and Individual Differences 51: 112–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Haugtvedt, C. P., R. E. Petty, and J. T. Cacioppo. 1992. Need for cognition and advertising: Understanding the role of personality variables in consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology 1: 239–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Hayes, A. F. 2022. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach, 3rd ed. New York: The Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  35. Henning, B., and P. Vorderer. 2001. Psychological escapism: Predicting the amount of television viewing by need for cognition. Journal of Communication 51: 100–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Hill, B. D., J. D. Foster, C. Sofko, E. M. Elliott, and J. T. Shelton. 2016. The interaction of ability and motivation: Average working memory is required for Need for Cognition to positively benefit intelligence and the effect increases with ability. Personality and Individual Differences 98: 225–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Hui, Q., C. Yao, S. Huang, and X. You. 2023. Need for Cognition and depressive symptoms: A mediation model of dysexecutive function and reappraisal. Current Psychology 42: 14673–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Inzlicht, M., K. M. Werner, J. L. Briskin, and B. W. Roberts. 2021. Integrating models of self-regulation. Annual Review of Psychology 72: 319–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Janke, S., and A. Glöckner-Rist. 2014. Deutsche Version der Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) [German Version of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS)]. Zusammenstellung Sozialwissenschaftlicher Items und Skalen 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Keller, U., A. Strobel, R. Martin, and F. Preckel. 2017. Domain-specificity of Need for Cognition among high school students. European Journal of Psychological Assessment 35: 607–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Kretzschmar, A., M. Spengler, A. -L. Schubert, R. Steinmayr, and M. Ziegler. 2018. The relation of personality and intelligence—What can the brunswik symmetry principle tell us? Journal of Intelligence 6: 30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Kuznetsova, A., P. B. Brockhoff, and R. H. B. Christensen. 2017. lmerTest Package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software 82: 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Lavrijsen, J., M. Vansteenkiste, M. Boncquet, and K. Verschueren. 2021. Does motivation predict changes in academic achieve- ment beyond intelligence and personality? A multitheoretical perspective. Journal of Educational Psychology 114: 772–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. LimeSurvey GmbH. 2022. LimeSurvey: An Open Source Survey Tool, Version 5.3.19; LimeSurvey GmbH. Available online: https://help.limesurvey.org/portal/en/kb/articles/how-to-cite-limesurvey (accessed on 28 October 2024).
  45. LimeSurvey GmbH. 2023. LimeSurvey: An Open Source Survey Tool, Version 5.6.9; LimeSurvey GmbH. Available online: https://www.limesurvey.org (accessed on 28 October 2024).
  46. Liu, Q., and J. C. Nesbit. 2024. The relation between Need for Cognition and academic achievement: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research 94: 155–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Lua, V. Y., W. M. Ooi, A. I. Najib, C. Tan, N. M. Majeed, A. K. Y. Leung, and A. Hartanto. 2024. Think your way to happiness? Investigating the role of need for cognition in well-being through a three-level meta-analytic approach. Motivation and Emotion 48: 75–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Mischel, W., O. Ayduk, M. G. Berman, B. J. Casey, I. H. Gotlib, J. Jonides, E. Kross, T. Teslovich, N. L. Wilson, V. Zayas, and et al. 2011. ‘Willpower’ over the life span: Decomposing self-regulation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 6: 252–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Moffitt, T. E., L. Arseneault, D. Belsky, N. Dickson, R. J. Hancox, H. Harrington, R. Houts, R. Poulton, B. W. Roberts, S. Ross, and et al. 2011. A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108: 2693–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Mussel, P. 2013. Intellect: A theoretical framework for personality traits related to intellectual achievements. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 104: 885–906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Mussel, P., N. Ulrich, J. J. B. Allen, R. Osinsky, and J. Hewig. 2016. Patterns of theta oscillation reflect the neural basis of individual differences in epistemic motivation. Scientific Reports 6: 29245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Nishiguchi, Y., K. Takano, and Y. Tanno. 2016. The need for cognition mediates and moderates the association between depressive symptoms and impaired effortful control. Psychiatry Research 241: 8–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Pavlova, I., A. M. Rogowska, and S. X. Zhang. 2024. Mental Health and well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic and after the Russian invasion of Ukraine. Journal of Community Health 49: 173–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Posit Team. 2023. RStudio: Integrated Development Environment for R. Boston: Posit Software, PBC. Available online: http://www.posit.co/ (accessed on 1 October 2024).
  55. Prolific. 2023. Prolific’s Attention and Comprehension Check Policy. Available online: https://researcher-help.prolific.com/en/article/fb63bb (accessed on 28 October 2024).
  56. QuestBack GmbH. 2019. Enterprise Feedback Suite: EFS Survey. Version EFS Fall. Köln: QuestBack GmbH. Available online: http://www.unipark.de/ (accessed on 10 July 2020).
  57. R Core Team. 2022. R: A language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 1 October 2024).
  58. Reeves, A. L., P. J. Watson, A. Ramsey, and R. J. Morris. 1995. Private self-consciousness factors, need for cognition, and depression. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality 10: 431–43. [Google Scholar]
  59. Scharbert, J., S. Humberg, L. Kroencke, T. Reiter, S. Sakel, J. ter Horst, K. Utesch, S. D. Gosling, G. Harari, S. C. Matz, and et al. 2024. Psychological well-being in Europe after the outbreak of war in Ukraine. Nature Communications 15: 1202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Scherer, A., M. Boecker, M. Pawelzik, S. Gauggel, and T. Forkmann. 2017. Emotion suppression, not reappraisal, predicts psychotherapy outcome. Psychotherapy Research 27: 143–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Schönbrodt, F. D., and M. Perugini. 2013. At what sample size do correlations stabilize? Journal of Research in Personality 47: 609–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Strobel, A., A. Farkas, J. Hoyer, U. Melicherova, V. Köllner, and A. Strobel. 2021. Cognitive motivation as a resource for affective adjustment and mental health. Frontiers in Psychology 12: 581681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Strobel, A., K. Anacker, and A. Strobel. 2017. Cognitive engagement mediates the relationship between positive life events and positive emotionality. Frontiers in Psychology 8: 1861. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Tangney, J. P., R. F. Baumeister, and A. L. Boone. 2004. High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality 72: 271–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. von Stumm, S., and P. Ackerman. 2013. Investment and intellect: A review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 139: 841–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Wennerhold, L., and M. Friese. 2023. Challenges in the conceptualization of trait self-control as a psychological construct. Social and Personality Psychology Compass 17: 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Westermann, R., E. Heise, K. Spies, and U. Trautwein. 1996. Identifikation und Erfassung von Komponenten der Studienzufriedenheit [Identifiying and assessing components of study satisfaction]. Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht 43: 1–22. [Google Scholar]
  68. Williams-Piehota, P., T. R. Schneider, J. Pizarro, L. Mowad, and P. Salovey. 2003. Matching health messages to information-processing styles: Need for cognition and mammography utilization. Health Communication 15: 375–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Yazdani, N., and K. L. Siedlecki. 2021. Mediators of the relationship between cognition and subjective well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies 22: 3091–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Zainal, N. H., and M. G. Newman. 2022. Curiosity helps: Growth in need for cognition bidirectionally predicts future reduction in anxiety and depression symptoms across 10 years. Journal of Affective Disorders 296: 642–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Zerna, J., A. Strobel, and A. Strobel. 2024. The role of need for cognition in well-being—Review and meta-analyses of associations and potentially underlying mechanisms. Collabra: Psychology 10: 1–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Zerna, J., N. Engelmann, A. Strobel, and A. Strobel. 2022. Need for cognition and burnout in teachers—A replication and extension study. Health Psychology Open 9: 205510292211396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Demographic data of (sub)samples.
Table 1. Demographic data of (sub)samples.
Sample 1Sample 2Sample 3
N451335346
Age Mdn = 21–29 years aM = 30.75 ± 12.28 yearsM = 32.4 ± 8.5 years
Gender73.4% female
26.6% male
72.5% female
27.2% male
0.3% diverse
32.7% female
66.8% male
0.6% diverse
Student Subsamples
N258157-
AgeMdn = 21–29 years aM = 23.03 ± 3.35 years-
Gender76.0% female
24.0% male
79.0% female
20.4% male
0.6% diverse
-
-
Employed (Sub)samples
N159156346
Age Mdn = 30–39 years aM = 37.2 ± 12.2 years32.4 ± 8.5 years
Gender69.2% female
30.8% male
64.1% female
35.9% male
32.7% female
66.8% male
0.6% diverse
Work status 66.0% full-time78.2% full-time81.5% full-time
Note. a In Sample 1, age was assessed as an ordinal variable with age categories.
Table 2. Items for domain-specific Need for Cognition.
Table 2. Items for domain-specific Need for Cognition.
GermanEnglish a
1. Beim Arbeiten [im Studium] löse ich gerne Aufgaben, bei denen man richtig nachdenken muss.At work [at university], I like solving problems that require hard thinking.
2. Beim Arbeiten [im Studium] mag ich Situationen, in denen ich richtig nachdenken muss. At work [at university], I like situations that require me to think hard.
3. Wenn ich beim Arbeiten [im Studium] Aufgaben zum Nachdenken bekomme, dann freue ich mich.At work [at university], I’m happy when I get an assignment that requires me to think hard.
4. Beim Arbeiten [im Studium] denke ich sehr gerne nach. At work [at university], I like to think a lot.
5. Beim Arbeiten [im Studium] macht mir Nachdenken Spaß.At work [at university], thinking is fun for me.
Note. Text in italics was used for assessing job-specific NFC; text in brackets for study-specific NFC. a Not an official translation, and this is only provided for this article.
Table 3. Descriptive statistics of used instruments.
Table 3. Descriptive statistics of used instruments.
MSDCronbach’s α
S1S2S3S1S2S3S1S2S3
NFC8.37 c6.52 d8.034.89 c5.32 d5.010.910.920.94
NFCstudy a5.683.82-5.886.18-0.940.94-
NFCjob b8.416.406.715.266.025.940.950.950.96
Positive affect33.0331.9832.105.886.316.320.830.860.85
Negative affect19.0318.6518.256.055.797.060.850.840.90
Life satisfaction5.094.944.531.141.181.320.840.870.90
Job satisfaction b30.5830.5728.185.635.405.950.860.840.86
Study satisfaction a
Subject-related 74.9472.70-20.2119.90-0.920.90-
Conditions59.1149.55-26.3126.65-0.850.84-
Coping with stress62.2059.62-26.7024.23-0.870.84-
Self-control3.133.163.120.610.600.620.820.810.83
Reappraisal4.704.454.591.040.951.010.830.780.85
Suppression3.603.414.111.201.161.270.750.740.82
Note. N = 335 - 451. S1 = Sample 1. S2 = Sample 2. S3 = Sample 3. a n = 157/258. b n = 156–346. c Student subsample: M ± SD = 7.60 ± 4.80, working subsample: M ± SD = 9.29 ± 4.82. d Student subsample: M ± SD = 5.96 ± 5.26, working subsample: M ± SD = 7.12 ± 5.45.
Table 4. Correlations of NFC with general well-being, emotion regulation, and self-control.
Table 4. Correlations of NFC with general well-being, emotion regulation, and self-control.
PANALife SatisfactionReappraisalSuppressionSelf-Control
NFCgeneralSample 10.29 ***−0.060.090.20 ***0.11 *0.25 ***
Sample 20.31 ***−0.020.14 *0.16 **0.040.11 *
Sample 30.36 ***−0.060.17 **0.12 *0.020.20 ***
NFCstudySample 10.25 ***−0.080.16 **0.16 **0.070.13 *
Sample 20.25 **−0.100.150.090.010.20 *
NFCjobSample 10.39 ***−0.120.21 **0.22 **0.010.27 ***
Sample 20.28 ***0.060.22 **0.14−0.020.03
Sample 30.38 ***−0.090.24 ***0.13 *−0.020.26 ***
Note. Spearman rank correlations. Sample 1: N = 451. Sample 2: N = 335. Sample 3: N = 346. Subsample sizes for correlations with NFCstudy = 157/258, for NFCjob = 156–346. PA = positive affect. NA = negative affect. * p < 0.05. ** p < 0.01. *** p < 0.001 (Bonferroni-adjusted significance level).
Table 5. Correlations of NFC with domain-specific life satisfaction.
Table 5. Correlations of NFC with domain-specific life satisfaction.
Job SatisfactionStudy Satisfaction
Subject-RelatedWith ConditionsCoping with Stress
NFCgeneralSample 10.110.31 ***−0.030.07
Sample 20.26 **0.16 *0.010.16 *
Sample 30.20 ***---
NFCdomainSample 10.25 **0.36 ***0.010.12
Sample 20.25 **0.25 **0.020.22 **
Sample 30.33 ***---
Note. Spearman rank correlations. Sample 1: N = 451. Sample 2: N = 355. Sample 3: N = 346. NFCgeneral = domain-general Need for Cognition. NFCdomain = study-specific Need for Cognition or job-specific Need for Cognition corresponding with domain of life satisfaction. Subsample sizes for correlations with NFCstudy = 157/258, for NFCjob = 156–346. * p < 0.05. ** p < 0.01. *** p < 0.001 (Bonferroni-adjusted significance level).
Table 6. Hierarchical regression of NFC predicting positive affect.
Table 6. Hierarchical regression of NFC predicting positive affect.
Student Sample 1 (N = 258)
PredictorModel 1Model 2
BSEβBSEβ
Constant30.72 ***0.68 30.81 ***0.68
NFCgeneral0.21 **0.080.171.000.100.08
NFCstudy 0.140.080.14
R2/R2korr0.030/0.0260.040/0.033
R20.030 **0.010
Student Sample 2 (N = 157)
PredictorModel 1Model 2Model 3
BSEβBSEβBSEβ
Constant21.91 ***3.30 19.99 ***3.28 20.59 ***3.35
Age0.38 **0.140.190.36 *0.140.180.34 *0.140.17
NFCgeneral 0.40 ***0.100.320.28 *0.130.22
NFCstudy 0.160.120.15
R2/R2korr0.038/0.0310.240/0.1290.153/0.137
R20.038 *0.102 ***0.013
Employed Sample 1 (N = 159)
PredictorModel 1Model 2Model 3
BSEβBSEβBSEβ
Constant30.50 ***1.65 27.72 ***1.71 26.91 ***1.79
Age0.85 *0.380.180.72 *0.350.150.82 *0.360.18
NFCgeneral 0.36 ***0.080.300.140.110.12
NFCjob 0.29 **0.110.27
R2/R2korr0.033/0.0270.125/0.1140.162/0.146
R20.0330.092 ***0.037 **
Employed Sample 2 (N = 156)
PredictorModel 1Model 2Model 3
BSEβBSEβBSEβ
Constant34.57 ***0.76 31.89 ***0.96 31.67 ***0.95
Gender a−2.30 *0.95−0.19−1.85 *0.91−0.16−1.690.89−0.14
NFCgeneral 0.34 ***0.080.320.100.120.10
NFCjob 0.28*0.110.29
R2/R2korr0.037/0.0310.137/0.1250.170/0.153
R20.037 *0.099 ***0.033 *
Employed Sample 3 (N = 346)
PredictorModel 1Model 2
BSEβBSEβ
Constant28.49 ***0.60 28.83 ***0.61
NFCgeneral0.45 ***0.060.360.180.120.15
NFCjob 0.27 **0.100.25
R2/R2korr0.127/0.1240.146/0.141
R20.127 ***0.019 **
Note. NFCgeneral = domain-general Need for Cognition. NFCstudy = study-specific Need for Cognition. NFCjob = job-specific Need for Cognition. SE and significance of coefficients based on 1000 Bootstrap samples. Variance inflation factor: 1.7–3.3. a 0 = male, 1 = female. * p < 0.05. ** p < 0.01. *** p < 0.001. Specific NFC explained 4% of variance in life satisfaction (R2marginal = 0.039).
Table 7. Hierarchical regression of NFC predicting life satisfaction.
Table 7. Hierarchical regression of NFC predicting life satisfaction.
Student Sample 1 (N = 258)
PredictorModel 1Model 2Model 3
BSEβBSEβBSEβ
Constant6.39 ***0.51 6.31 ***0.54 6.31 ***0.53
Age−0.46 *0.18−0.20−0.45 *0.18−0.19−0.44 *0.18−0.19
NFCgeneral 0.010.01.04−0.020.02−0.10
NFCstudy 0.04 *0.020.20
R2/R2korr0.038/0.0340.039/0.0320.061/0.050
R20.038 **0.0010.022 *
Student Sample 2 (N = 157)
PredictorModel 1Model 2Model 3
BSEβBSEβBSEβ
Constant4.35 ***0.21 4.09 ***0.25 4.12 ***0.25
Gender a0.62 **0.240.210.69 **0.240.230.69 **0.230.23
NFCgeneral 0.030.020.15−0.000.02−0.02
NFCstudy 0.05 *0.020.26
R2/R2korr0.043/0.0360.064/0.0510.103/0.085
R20.043 **0.0210.039 *
Employed Sample 1 (N = 159)
PredictorModel 1Model 2Model 3
BSEβBSEβBSEβ
Constant4.86 ***0.30 4.75 ***0.30 4.64 ***0.31
Age0.100.070.120.090.070.110.110.070.13
NFCgeneral 0.010.020.07−0.020.03−0.09
NFCjob 0.04 *0.020.22
R2/R2korr0.14/0.0080.019/0.0060.044/0.025
R20.0140.0040.025 *
Employed Sample 2 (N = 156)
PredictorModel 1Model 2
BSEβBSEβ
Constant4.78 ***0.19 4.76 ***0.19
NFCgeneral0.040.020.18−0.000.03−0.02
NFCjob 0.05 *0.020.26
R2/R2korr0.032/0.0260.059/0.046
R20.032 *0.027 *
Employed Sample 3 (N = 346)
PredictorModel 1Model 2
BSEβBSEβ
Constant4.22 ***0.14 4.30 ***0.14
NFCgeneral0.04 *0.020.14−0.020.03−0.08
NFCjob 0.06 **0.020.27
R2/R2korr0.020/0.0170.042/0.036
R20.020 **0.021 **
Note. NFCgeneral = domain-general Need for Cognition. NFCstudy = study-specific Need for Cognition. NFCjob = job-specific Need for Cognition. SE and significance of coefficients based on 1000 Bootstrap samples. Variance inflation factor: 1.7–3.3. a 0 = male, 1 = female. n = 156 because of omitting one diverse participant for regression analyses. * p < 0.05. ** p < 0.01. *** p < 0.001.
Table 8. Hierarchical regression of NFC predicting study-related satisfaction.
Table 8. Hierarchical regression of NFC predicting study-related satisfaction.
Satisfaction with Study Subject
Student Sample 1 (N = 258)
PredictorModel 1Model 2
BSEβBSEβ
Constant67.38 ***2.56 68.28 ***2.57
NFCgeneral0.99 ***0.280.24−0.130.42−0.03
NFCstudy 1.34 **0.360.39
R2/R2korr0.056/0.0520.137/0.130
R20.056 ***0.082 ***
Student Sample 2 (N = 157)
PredictorModel 1Model 2
BSEβBSEβ
Constant69.97 ***2.43 70.71 ***2.35
NFCgeneral0.460.300.12−0.390.44−0.10
NFCstudy 1.12 **0.380.35
R2/R2korr0.015/0.0080.086/0.074
R20.0150.071 ***
Coping with Study-Related Stress
Student Sample 2 (N = 157)
PredictorModel 1Model 2
BSEβBSEβ
Constant55.97 ***3.02 56.58 ***2.93
NFCgeneral0.610.400.13−0.080.55−0.02
NFCstudy 0.93 *0.470.24
R2/R2korr0.018/0.0110.051/0.038
R20.0180.033 *
Note. NFCgeneral = domain-general Need for Cognition. NFCstudy = study-specific Need for Cognition. Variance inflation factor: 1.7–2.1. SE and significance of coefficients based on 1000 Bootstrap samples. * p < 0.05. ** p < 0.01. *** p < 0.001.
Table 9. Hierarchical regression of NFC predicting job satisfaction.
Table 9. Hierarchical regression of NFC predicting job satisfaction.
Employed Sample 1 (N = 159)
PredictorModel 1Model 2Model 3
BSEβBSEβBSEβ
Constant27.21 ***1.62 27.29 ***1.75 26.15 ***1.74
Age0.80 *0.370.170.80 *0.370.170.94 *0.370.20
NFCgeneral −0.010.10−0.01−0.32 **0.11−0.27
NFCjob 0.41 ***0.090.38
R2/R2korr0.029/0.0230.029/0.0160.103/0.085
R20.029*0.0000.074 ***
Employed Sample 2 (N = 156)
PredictorModel 1Model 2
BSEβBSEβ
Constant28.62 ***0.78 28.58 ***0.78
NFCgeneral0.27 **0.080.280.170.120.18
NFCjob 0.120.120.15
R2/R2korr0.077/0.0710.084/0.072
R20.077 ***0.007
Employed Sample 3 (N = 346)
PredictorModel 1Model 2
BSEβBSEβ
Constant26.50 ***0.64 27.10 ***0.62
NFCgeneral0.21 **0.070.18−0.27 *012−0.23
NFCjob 0.48 ***0.100.48
R2/R2korr0.031/0.0280.099/0.094
R20.031 **0.068 ***
Note. NFCgeneral = domain-general Need for Cognition. NFCjob = job-specific Need for Cognition. SE and significance of coefficients based on 1000 Bootstrap samples. Variance inflation factor: 1.9–3.3. * p < 0.05. ** p < 0.01. *** p < 0.001.
Table 10. Mediation analyses with self-regulatory processes as parallel mediators.
Table 10. Mediation analyses with self-regulatory processes as parallel mediators.
PredictorSampleIndirect EffectsDirect b
Total aSCS bReappraisal bSuppression b
Positive Affect
NFCgeneralSample 10.10 [0.05, 0.14] *xxxx
Sample 20.06 [0.01, 0.11] *nsxnsx
Sample 30.09 [0.04, 0.14] *xxnsx
NFCstudySample 10.07 [0.01, 0.13] *xxnsx
Sample 20.07 [−0.01, 0.16]xnsnsx
NFCjobSample 10.13 [0.05, 23] *xnsnsx
Sample 20.06 [−0.02, 0.15]nsxnsx
Sample 30.11 [0.06, 0.18] *xxnsx
Life Satisfaction
NFCgeneralSample 10.10 [0.06, 0.15] *xxxns
Sample 20.05 [−0.01, 0.09]nsxnsx
Sample 30.08 [0.03, 0.12] *xxnsns
NFCstudySample 10.07 [0.02, 0.13] *xxnsns
Sample 20.09 [0.01, 0.19] *xnsnsns
NFCjobSample 10.12 [0.04, 0.22] *xnsnsns
Sample 20.03 [−0.03, 0.10]nsnsnsx
Sample 30.09 [0.04, 0.14] *xxnsx
Study-Related Satisfaction: Subject-Related
NFCgeneralSample 10.01 [−0.03, 0.06]nsnsnsns
Sample 20.05 [−0.02, 0.12]nsnsnsns
NFCstudySample 10.04 [−0.01, 0.09]nsxnsx
Sample 20.07 [0.01, 0.14] *xnsnsx
Study-Related Satisfaction: Coping
NFCgeneralSample 10.01 [−0.04, 0.06]nsnsnsns
Sample 20.04 [−0.01, 0.12]nsnsnsns
NFCstudySample 10.01 [−0.03, 0.05]nsnsnsns
Sample 20.04 [−0.02, 0.12]nsnsnsx
Job Satisfaction
NFCgeneralSample 10.05 [0.00, 0.12] *nsxnsx
Sample 20.01 [−0.04, 0.07]nsnsnsx
Sample 30.03 [−0.00, 0.07]xnsnsx
NFCjobSample 10.06 [0.00, 0.13] *xnsnsns
Sample 20.01 [−0.04, 0.08]nsnsnsx
Sample 30.04 [0.01, 0.08] *xnsnsx
Note. N = 335–451. BCa 95% confidence intervals of 1000 Bootstrap samples. Significance of effects based on confidence intervals excluding 0. a Completely standardized. b x = significant effect. ns = non-significant. * Confidence interval excluding 0.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Grass, J.; Strobel, A. Cognitive Engagement and Subjective Well-Being in Adults: Exploring the Role of Domain-Specific Need for Cognition. J. Intell. 2024, 12, 110. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12110110

AMA Style

Grass J, Strobel A. Cognitive Engagement and Subjective Well-Being in Adults: Exploring the Role of Domain-Specific Need for Cognition. Journal of Intelligence. 2024; 12(11):110. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12110110

Chicago/Turabian Style

Grass, Julia, and Anja Strobel. 2024. "Cognitive Engagement and Subjective Well-Being in Adults: Exploring the Role of Domain-Specific Need for Cognition" Journal of Intelligence 12, no. 11: 110. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12110110

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop