Nothing Special   »   [go: up one dir, main page]

We Are Intechopen, The World'S Leading Publisher of Open Access Books Built by Scientists, For Scientists

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

We are IntechOpen,

the world’s leading publisher of


Open Access books
Built by scientists, for scientists

6,900
Open access books available
186,000
International authors and editors
200M Downloads

Our authors are among the

154
Countries delivered to
TOP 1%
most cited scientists
12.2%
Contributors from top 500 universities

Selection of our books indexed in the Book Citation Index


in Web of Science™ Core Collection (BKCI)

Interested in publishing with us?


Contact book.department@intechopen.com
Numbers displayed above are based on latest data collected.
For more information visit www.intechopen.com
Chapter

A View from the Start: A Review


of Inhibitory Control Training in
Early Childhood
Erin Ruth Baker, Qingyang Liu and Rong Huang

Abstract

Young children’s capacity to monitor and control their thoughts and behaviors
is influenced largely by inhibitory control, which grows rapidly during this age
due to brain maturation. This capacity has important implications for children’s
development, including academic and social outcomes, and has been shown to be
influenced by culture and exposure to adverse life events such as poverty. Research
suggests that this capacity, importantly, may be largely trainable, with appropriate
training programs.

Keywords: early childhood, executive function, cross-cultural, low-income

1. Introduction

During the childhood years and into adolescence, the brain grows tremendously,
causing a significant change in cognitive capacities. In later years of childhood and
adolescence, many of the neurological changes correspond with advancements in
perspective taking and reasoning; however, evidence from the early childhood years
suggests that these changes more closely align with advancements in inhibitory
control and executive functions more broadly [1, 2]. However, there are distinct
developmental changes which inform our understanding of inhibitory control and
which merit further discussion. Regardless, these developmental changes have
profound impacts on children’s development overall, including academic and social
outcomes. It is important to recognize, however, that children’s capacities to inhibit
a prepotent response have been shown to vary by culture, as well as exposure to
early adverse life events, and therefore a consideration of environment should be
included when attempting to conduct research in this area or when making impor-
tant policy or curriculum decisions. Nevertheless, research which utilizes inhibitory
control (IC) training specifically within the early childhood ages demonstrates
positive results, with more intensive training yielding more promising results.

2. Nature of inhibitory control during the early childhood years

Research has consistently demonstrated that the preschool years are a develop-
mental time during which children experience profound growth in their ability to
inhibit an unwanted response [3]. Younger preschool-age children are more likely
to perseverate in their errors across multiple trials [4] by repeating a maladaptive

1
Inhibitory Control Training - A Multidisciplinary Approach

behavior—for instance, a child who continues to shout out in class instead of raising
their hand—whereas this pattern declines markedly by age 4. Similarly, 3-year-old
children demonstrate an ability to inhibit an automatic prepotent response on a
Simon Says task (e.g., Go/NoGo task [5]: children are trained to respond to one
stimulus and are trained not to respond to a similar stimulus; see Table 1) for
roughly one in four trials, in comparison to 4-year-old children who were success-
ful on roughly 9 out of 10 trials [6]. Moreover, the impacts of inhibitory control on
children’s cognitive capacities also seem to change as a function of age. For instance,
younger preschool-age children’s inhibitory control capacities strongly predict
their problem-solving strategy use and performance; however, older preschool-age
children’s problem solving is better explained by their working memory capaci-
ties (see Table 1) rather than their inhibitory control abilities [7]. Relatedly, the
development patterns of IC growth may not be limited to simply greater accuracy
on relatively straightforward tasks. Older preschool-age children perform with
greater success on more complicated tasks of IC than their younger peers [1], which
may indicate that using multiple, progressive tasks when assessing IC may reveal
important developmental patterns not captured by using a single task or by using

Key terms Definitions


Executive functions (EF) The constellation of foundation cognitive capacities, such as
inhibitory control, working memory, and attention, which allow for
later emergence of reasoning and problem solving

Inhibitory control (IC) The cognitive capacity to inhibit a prepotent, automatic behavioral
response

Working memory A cognitive system for temporarily storing and managing


information that is necessary for undertaking complex cognitive
tasks
Theory of mind (ToM) The understanding that others have mental states such as beliefs,
desires, etc., which can vary from person to person or within one
person over time

Go/NoGo task Children are trained to respond to one stimulus (e.g., “Go” stimuli)
and are trained not to respond to a similar stimulus (“NoGo”
stimuli). This task measures behavioral inhibition

Day/Night Stroop task Children are trained and must complete trials in which they say the
word “night” when presented with an image of a sun on a white
background and say “day” when presented with an image of a moon
on a dark background. This task involves both behavioral inhibition
and cognitive interference

Cognitive interference/interference It refers to attempts to suppress interference from competing


control stimuli. The response time of an IC task is usually considered as a
measure of cognitive interference

Behavioral inhibition It requires suppressing a behavioral response for a more optimal


response. Cognitive interference and behavioral inhibition are two
aspects of IC
Electroencephalogram (EEG) A neurological testing that allows researchers to precisely measure
brain activity during the behavioral tasks, which provides for a
more complete examination and consideration of IC as a cognitive
capacity
Inhibitory control training A designed intervention that includes a training process which
aimed at improving IC

Table 1.
A summary of definitions of the major concepts and techniques.

2
A View from the Start: A Review of Inhibitory Control Training in Early Childhood
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/intechopen.88700

multiple similar tasks. Overall, the findings on early childhood IC show robust and
dramatic growth, particularly during the childhood years.
Although researchers agree on the tremendous growth of IC during this devel-
opmental age, there persists disagreement as to the specific nature of IC, and execu-
tive functions, during this time. Executive functions (EF) refer to the constellation
of foundational cognitive capacities, such as inhibitory control, working memory,
and attention [1], which allow for later emergence of reasoning and problem solv-
ing. In middle childhood and beyond, these executive function capacities can be
considered as increasingly discrete processing mechanisms; however, during early
childhood, these patterns remain more nebulous, and studies using confirmatory
factor analysis have shown developmental differences in factor emergence and
persistent factor unification into the childhood years. For instance, during the pre-
school years (broadly, ages 3 years to 6 years), studies using multiple assessments of
inhibitory control, attention, and working memory yield a single unitary construct
of executive function or inhibition generally [8–12], whereas studies of middle
childhood have discerned multiple discrete factors, including working memory and
attention shifting [13], and this trend continues and expands into later childhood
and adolescence (see [14]).
The prevailing argument is that tasks of IC during the early childhood years
necessitate activation of multiple other components of EF. take, for instance, the
Day/Night Stroop task (see [15]; Table 1) in which a child is trained and must
complete trials in which they say the word “night” when presented with an image
of a sun on a white background and say “day” when presented with an image of
a moon on a dark background. In this task, IC is typically measured by accuracy,
with measurements of response time frequently included as well. This task clearly
requires the child to inhibit the automatic response of verbalizing the association
they have made between the sun and it being daytime, or between the moon being
present during nighttime, and thus is inarguably a task of IC. However, some argue
that this task measures additional facets of EF simply by the nature of the task. For
instance, a child must have sustained attention throughout the assessment, and if
the child loses focus for even a moment, the measurement of response time could
be conflated, leading some scholars to argue that the attentional component of EF
predicts IC [3, 16]. Similarly, the child must work to keep the rules of the moon/
day and sun/night matching in the forefront of their mind during the assessment,
and if they do not, then the accuracy measure could be conflated with working
memory. Many researchers have argued, therefore, that the various EF components
are highly integrated during the early childhood years and that these components
emerge as more distinct with age and experience [1, 14].
As shown by the previous example, it is difficult for researchers to disentangle
the various components of EF, from a measurement perspective, in early childhood.
Researchers’ understanding and measurement of inhibitory control during the early
childhood years should therefore be sensitive to the developmental nature of such
phenomena and should perhaps consider using indices of a variety of executive
function capacities. However, as described previously [14], a common conceptu-
alization of EF in the early childhood literature seems to imply that EF and IC are
analogous at this development time (e.g., [17]) or that IC developmentally precedes
other domains of EF (e.g., [16]). Although IC contributes largely to early childhood
EF, as demonstrated in the previous example, it may be problematic to consider these
as synonymous. One argument in support of this claim is that children’s task perfor-
mance on EF tasks most closely replicates issues of IC—that is, a child will persist in
making prepotent errors, a classic demonstration of immature IC, while also activat-
ing other areas of EF, such as attention, working memory, etc. Although several
arguments have been proposed to counter the position of equivocating EF with IC,

3
Inhibitory Control Training - A Multidisciplinary Approach

most pertinent to the current chapter may be that IC itself may be multidimensional.
Referring again to the aforementioned example, many researchers consider response
time in the Day-Night Stroop task to be a measure of cognitive interference (sometimes
referred to as interference control), which refers to attempts to suppress interference
from competing stimuli, in contrast to behavioral inhibition which requires suppress-
ing a behavioral response for a more optimal response [18, 19]. That is, the construct
of inhibitory control as it pertains to developmental changes during the early child-
hood years requires both the cognitive power to limit attention to distractor stimuli
and the behavioral power to engage an appropriate response.
Turning to developing an appreciation for the role of IC for holistic develop-
ment, the capacity for IC has important implications in terms of development
across a number of domains [20]. For instance, although IC has been shown to
predict children’s academic achievement generally throughout the childhood years
[21], strong IC consistently predicts more proficient mathematical knowledge
[16, 22–24] and numerical strategy use [25]. Moreover, IC has been implicated in
children’s emergent literacy proficiency [16, 24] and language development [26].
The development of IC during the early childhood years additionally has profound
implications for children’s social and emotional development [27], such as the
emergence and development of social perspective taking [28], problem solving
and emotional control [27], and suppressing disruptive behaviors and aggression
[11]. As such, IC should be considered by researchers and practitioners alike for the
implications this capacity may hold across areas of maladaptive academic and social
development.
Research methodologies employed for assessing IC during the early childhood
years can vary considerably, and each assessment offers a wealth of strengths yet,
as mentioned previously, may be incomplete on its own. Therefore, much of the
research studies in this area use more than one type of assessment or multiple
assessments with considerable methodological overlap. An important consideration
in measurement of IC, indeed of any cognitive faculty, during the early childhood
years is the developmental appropriateness of the task (see [8], for review). For
instance, children in this age range are often concurrently experiencing emerg-
ing literacy skills and are often not yet proficient readers; therefore, it would be
inappropriate to use a task which requires even low reading requirements, as such
a task would likely require a cognitive load too great to allow for successful task
completion. Moreover, such a task when used with an emerging reader would
result in contaminated measurement in that task performance may indicate a
lack of understanding the rules of the task, the lack of proficiency in reading, or
inhibitory control. Similarly, tasks to be used on a study of early childhood should
be rather straightforward, without overly complicated instructions or numerous
steps. Therefore, much of the research studies in the area of inhibitory control that
focus on early child development utilize tasks or games which require no reading,
with simple instructions provided to the child verbally and repeated if necessary,
and these tasks typically include a generous training time to ensure that the child
understands and can perform the task.
Many of the commonly employed tasks resemble that of the Day/Night Stroop
task [15, 29] and the Go/NoGo task [1, 5, 19, 30], both of which were described in
the previous paragraphs. Importantly, these two tasks differ in terms of cognitive
interference with regard to the expectations for children’s behavioral responses.
Specifically, the Day/Night Stroop task involves embedded rule use, thus requires
children to produce a verbal response to multiple stimuli, and therefore requires
that the children process and act on multiple rules (i.e., if moon, then “day,” but if
sun, then “night”), whereas the Go/NoGo task only requires a behavioral response
to a single stimulus (e.g., if “Simon Says,” then response; if not, then no response).

4
A View from the Start: A Review of Inhibitory Control Training in Early Childhood
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/intechopen.88700

This distinction has led some to argue that the Day/Night Stroop task may be more
complex particularly for younger children than other tasks, and therefore perfor-
mance on this and similar tasks may be indicative of greater IC capacities compared
with Go/NoGo tasks (see, e.g., [31, 32]).
Consistent with the recommendation noted previously regarding the need to
use multiple indices of IC when attempting to correctly assess children’s capacities,
many researchers employ the use of neurological testing, such as an electroencepha-
logram (EEG), in concert with a behavioral task, such as the Go/NoGo task (e.g.,
[30]). Using neurological measurement, such as EEG, allows researchers to pre-
cisely measure brain activity during the behavioral tasks, which provides for a more
complete examination and consideration of IC as a cognitive capacity particularly
from a developmental perspective. That is, as the brain is experiencing tremendous
growth during the early childhood years, it is important to capture how such physi-
cal growth corresponds with cognitive growth, and this is perhaps best done by
measuring neural activity during a cognitive task.
Overall, EF generally, and inhibitory control specifically, undergoes dramatic
growth during the preschool years, which has important implications for their
development overall [1, 2]. Although EF is discernable as more discrete constructs
in later ages of development, this has not been consistently demonstrated during
the early childhood years [8–12], and thus researchers should consider possibly
utilizing multiple tasks, including neurological assessments if possible [30], to pro-
vide a more comprehensive understanding of IC during the early childhood years.

2.1 Impacts of culture and environment on young children’s inhibitory control


development

Consistent with other cross-cultural research which shows variation in the


timing and emergence of children’s cognitive capacities (e.g., [33]), evidence of
IC development from non-Western societies is not entirely consistent with that of
Western societies, suggesting that children’s inhibitory control may be impacted by
cultural and environmental factors [34]. For instance, a variety of studies comparing
Chinese samples to Western samples may suggest that preschool-age children reared
in Chinese cultures outperform their US counterparts on tasks of IC [34, 35], which
has also been found in other non-Western cultures (e.g., Japan [36]). Importantly,
comparing samples of non-Western cultures from African and Latin American
communities [37] as well as cultures which share both Western and Eastern ideals
(i.e., Turkey [38]) to Western has not yielded differences by culture. Overall, the
cross-cultural research on IC development in early childhood may indicate that
although there is a large universality in terms of IC development, cultural and
societal mores may cause differences in children’s IC and development more broadly.
Moreover, in terms of implications of the cross-cultural research for child
development more broadly, in Western cultures IC has been consistently shown
to predict theory of mind (i.e., the understanding that others have mental states
such as beliefs, desires, etc., which can vary from person to person or within one
person over time [39]) particularly during the early childhood years [31], which
holds implications for children’s social competence during early childhood and
beyond; however, this predictive relation between IC and theory of mind has
not consistently been demonstrated in cross-cultural samples to the same degree
as in Western samples [40]. For example, a recent meta-analysis discussed that
although IC and EF generally did predict theory of mind and mental state under-
standing across cultures, the strength of this prediction was weaker among studies
assessing East Asian samples than several Western samples, including the USA,
Canada, and Europe [31].

5
Inhibitory Control Training - A Multidisciplinary Approach

Other environmental factors, such as exposure to poverty or low socioeconomic


opportunity, have also been shown to impact children’s cognitive development,
including the development of EF during the early childhood years, with children from
low-income families generally underperforming their more affluent peers [41, 42];
however, the recent work has turned to focus on the adaptive strengths of children
raised in environments with higher rates of adversity [43]. For example, although
children from low-income families have demonstrated less accuracy on a Go/NoGo
task, they did not perform more slowly on the task [44]. Moreover, children from
low-income families performed less accurately on a simple working memory task
than with peers, but these group differences were eliminated when the task was made
to be more complex [44]. A similar finding has been shown for children who have
experienced familial trauma, and this may be true even when considering the impacts
of poverty exposure. Children who have been reported as experiencing family trauma,
as assessed by indices of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), showed poorer global
EF than non-traumatized children [45]; however, the effect size was weaker for IC task
performance than other types of EF, such as working memory and processing speed.
This may suggest that children who experience familial trauma may develop adaptive
responses to their environments which allow them to inhibit prepotent responses as
indicated by the IC task performance.
Similar to the pattern of IC mediating the impact of culture on early childhood
competence, it may be that environmental exposure to poverty and adversity may
additionally impact other areas of children’s development. Academic achievement and
behavioral regulatory faculties are more strongly predicted by IC task performance for
children from more affluent family backgrounds, for example, than their less affluent
peers [46]. Additionally, among children attending a federally funded educational
program for low-income families and their children, children with stronger IC per-
formance were rated by their teachers as having better socio-emotional faculties and
showed fewer internalizing behaviors (e.g., indications of anxiety and depression)
than their low-income peers who performed less well on IC tasks [47, 48]. Children
who experience other types of environmental adversity, such as children who experi-
ence violence or maltreatment at home, show similar patterns of poorer academic
achievement and school adjustment, yet this relation is additionally explained by chil-
dren’s IC [49]. In sum, although children who experience adverse early life events, or
are raised in low-income families and neighborhoods, have shown to differ from more
traditional samples in terms of academic achievement and socio-emotional competen-
cies, these discrepancies may be explained by young children’s emerging IC faculties.
Therefore, these children may show marked improvements in EF capacities, as well as
other positive outcomes such as improved academic achievement, with IC training.

2.2 Inhibitory control training in early childhood and implications for


development

Efforts in establishing inhibitory control as an effective tool for cognitive


improvements have proven successful across the life span [50]. Moreover, as the
early childhood years are an important time for the development of EF generally,
and IC specifically, as previously discussed, this developmental age range is ideal
for examining the possible power of IC training. Several studies have examined
the impacts of IC training on child outcomes, and these studies consistently yield
positive findings [19, 21, 50–56].
Importantly, the outcomes of IC training have been shown to vary considerably
based on the types of training. Studies have shown, for example, the training of
more global EF capacities rather than IC specifically (e.g., [53, 55]) may be success-
ful in expanding cognitive performance across a wide range of tasks. This aligns

6
A View from the Start: A Review of Inhibitory Control Training in Early Childhood
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/intechopen.88700

with the aforementioned discussion of the entanglement of multiple components of


EF during early childhood (i.e., that many factor analytic studies have shown that in
the early childhood years the distinct components of EF, such as working memory
and IC, may not be as separately discernable as evidenced during the later years of
development [8–12]). To test this, some researchers have attempted to gauge the
effectiveness of IC training compared with training in other areas of EF, such as
working memory; for instance, children who received 5 weeks of computerized
IC training, compared with a group who received a parallel program of working
memory training, showed improvements in task performance for the task on which
they received training; however these improvements did not transfer to other tasks
of EF [55]. Limited transfer effects have been reported elsewhere as well, such as
children showing increased performance on methodologically and structurally simi-
lar tasks as to the training task, but not other tasks [51], although this increase in
performance was sustained over many months. However, other research has shown
considerable transfer effects, such as children showing enhanced reasoning abilities
after training on a Go/NoGo task [19] and children, adolescents, and adults showing
enhanced perspective-taking capacities after receiving IC training [50].
Other types of training have also shown to be successful in improving EF
performance. Having children engage in reflective metacognition regarding their
performance on difficult cognitive tasks, such as their performance in the Day/
Night Stroop task, has shown to be effective in enhancing their performance on
that task even compared with more traditional training procedures, such as practice
and corrective feedback [54]. Additionally, training on language skills can have
a positive impact on children’s IC, as such trainings require engaging in multiple
components of EF (e.g., sustained attention), as well as IC specifically [57] such
as by requiring the child to use the correct term for a specific item within a larger
category of items. This aligns with the espoused conceptualization of early child-
hood IC as profoundly entangled within EF and is consistent with other research
on EF trainings more broadly. For instance, children who received 12 sessions over
4 weeks of training that included working memory, IC, and cognitive flexibility
showed significant improvements across a range of EF capacities, and these effects
transferred to other areas of children’s cognition as well [58].
Additionally, outcomes of IC training may yield important changes at the level of
neuronal and brain activity [54], which may not necessarily correspond with immedi-
ate changes in behavior. For instance, studies have shown that children who received
an 8-week program of training which targeted children’s IC, working memory, and
planning ability found that, for children who received the training, neural activity
levels changed in the corresponding brain regions as expected; however children’s task
performance after the training did not differ significantly from their pre-training per-
formance [30]. Other studies have found that a single training session of metacogni-
tive reflection about controlling impulsive behaviors may lead to decreased activation
of the brain region responsible for processing conflicting information, which may
indicate a lessened likelihood to process the information as conflicting, and therefore
possibly indicating better adaptation in integrating new rule schemes [54].
Moreover, the positive impacts of receiving EF training have been found effec-
tive for children from low-income families as well. For instance, one study showed
that when classrooms serving low-income families implemented a full year of
an EF training that included a broad range of EF skills deeply integrated into the
classroom curriculum, compared with classrooms that had no such program,
children who received the training outperformed children in the control group on
both simple and advanced tasks [52]. Indeed, children in the control group showed
a decline in task performance for the more advanced tasks, which was not found
for children who received the training. What’s more, the program was so successful

7
Inhibitory Control Training - A Multidisciplinary Approach

that the control condition was not allowed to repeat, by request of the school, as
the teachers and parents noted such profound change in terms of academics and
student behavior that the school refused to continue the project without full imple-
mentation of the program in all classrooms. In concert with findings from more
traditional samples which included less intensive training, these outcomes would
indicate that children from low-income families might be an especially important
area for future research, given the overwhelming strength of the results.
Although studies using IC training with children of incarcerated parents, or
children who have experienced abuse or neglect, were not revealed during the
current literature search, the previously discussed research of IC training during
early childhood could be extended to these populations [45]. For instance, research
on treatment approaches for anxiety and depression (common outcomes of trauma)
with adults has shown that training individuals to develop better self-regulatory
executive processes, such as attention, have shown promising results (see [45] for
discussion). Given that children’s EF faculties are more nebulous and entangled
than adults, it stands to reason that such approaches would yield additionally
promising results during the early childhood years.
Moreover, such intervention approaches might be promising for researchers
working in clinical settings. For instance, mindfulness training approaches that
capitalize on EF processes, such as attentional focus, for children with anxiety have
proven effective in reducing anxiety symptomology [59]. Such training has proven
effective for children from low-income families as well [60], with positive effects
extending beyond advanced EF development to include positive socio-emotional
and behavioral changes as well. However, much of the research in this area has been
conducted with older children, and examinations of the promise of mindfulness
training during the early childhood years may not yet exist. This may be an impor-
tant area for future research, as the implications for positive outcomes may be more
robust with earlier intervention.
Overall, findings with regard to improving child outcomes during the early child-
hood years as they relate to IC training suggest that IC training may be effective to vary-
ing degrees. Although a few studies showed limited transfer effects, the most promising
findings come from studies which implemented a broader EF training program rather
than those which utilized specific IC training. Additionally, research studies with
longer training programs, such as the 1-year program discussed above, yield stronger
effects in terms of global child outcomes than did shorter programs. Studies involving
brain imaging have also shown positive outcomes in terms of changes in brain activity,
indicating that such training may be important for effecting long-term change.

3. Conclusions

The cognitive capacity to inhibit a prepotent, automatic response grows tre-


mendously during the early childhood years corresponding with and as a function
of profound brain development taking place at this time [1, 2]. At later ages, this
cognitive ability is rather distinct from other foundational cognitive capacities, such
as attention and working memory; however, considerable research suggests that
during early childhood these distinctions are less clear, leading many researchers to
consider and research EF as a more global function at this age [8–14].
Research which focuses on IC during the early childhood years typically utilizes
simple, game-like tasks which require brief or no verbal response, and researchers
typically utilize a variety of tasks which may assess various areas of EF, including
the Day/Night Stroop task [15, 29] and the Go/NoGo task [1, 5, 19, 30]. Importantly,
and with regard for the important growth occurring at this age in specific brain

8
A View from the Start: A Review of Inhibitory Control Training in Early Childhood
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/intechopen.88700

regions, many researchers use these tasks in combination with brain imaging [30],
providing important insights into developmental changes taking place in brain and
neuronal activity.
Importantly, the current chapter includes much literature on EF broadly rather
than focusing specifically on IC and IC training. Although this is consistent with
the current conceptualization of IC during this developmental time within the
literature, this may have resulted in certain findings and trainings being included in
the current discussion with which some researchers may disagree. Additionally, the
current chapter focuses narrowly on a specific developmental window and thus is
not representative of the research on IC across childhood.
For transparency, the literature review for the current chapter used the following
databases: Google Scholar, ScienceDirect, and Web of Science. The keywords were
executive function, inhibitory control (training), working memory, sustained atten-
tion, cognitive development, self-regulation, preschoolers, early childhood, children,
cross-cultural, and risk population. Specifically, inhibitory control is a broad term in
the research procedure, which has been expanded and is not limited to the executive
function and cognitive development [61]. For ease of understanding, an at-risk popu-
lation was defined as any potential risk factors (i.e., poverty or low income, neighbor-
hood violence, family violence, family maltreatment, low social status, low education
background, rural area). However, and as is the case with many reviews, the current
chapter does not include findings from unpublished works, and thus the positive
support for IC training as discussed here may be an artifact of publication bias.
Nevertheless, in creating IC training programs, much of the research has
shown positive outcomes across a variety of training programs; however, as is to
be expected, more promising and profound results accompany programs with
more intensive training with longer durations [50–58]. Such training programs
have proven effective across cultures and changes in the environment [52], includ-
ing children from low-income backgrounds and children who have experienced
profound early adverse life events. Although little research to date has examined
IC training during early childhood in clinical samples, extending from the work
discussed in this chapter, it would follow that IC training broadly, and perhaps
mindfulness training specifically, may yield positive outcomes across domains.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare there is no conflict of interest.

Author details

Erin Ruth Baker*, Qingyang Liu and Rong Huang


University at Albany, State University of New York, Albany, NY, United States

*Address all correspondence to: erbaker@albany.edu

© 2019 The Author(s). Licensee IntechOpen. This chapter is distributed under the terms
of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/
by/3.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium,
provided the original work is properly cited.

9
Inhibitory Control Training - A Multidisciplinary Approach

References

[1] Anderson PJ, Reidy N. Assessing Wager TD. The unity and diversity
executive function in preschoolers. of executive functions and their
Neuropsychology Review. 2012;22(4): contributions to complex “frontal
345-360 lobe” tasks: A latent variable
analysis. Cognitive Psychology.
[2] Munakata Y, Herd SA, Chatham CH, 2000;41(1):49-100
Depue BE, Banich MT, O’Reilly RC. A
unified framework for inhibitory [11] Raaijmakers MA, Smidts DP,
control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. Sergeant JA, Maassen GH,
2011;15(10):453-459 Posthumus JA, Van Engeland H, et al.
Executive functions in preschool
[3] Reck SG, Hund AM. Sustained children with aggressive behavior:
attention and age predict inhibitory Impairments in inhibitory control.
control during early childhood. Journal Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology.
of Experimental Child Psychology. 2008;36(7):1097
2011;108(3):504-512
[12] Wiebe SA, Espy KA, Charak D.
[4] Diamond A, Taylor C. Development Using confirmatory factor analysis
of an aspect of executive control: to understand executive control
Development of the abilities to in preschool children: I. Latent
remember what I said and to “Do structure. Developmental Psychology.
as I say, not as I do”. Developmental 2008;44(2):575
Psychobiology. 1996;29(4):315-334
[13] van der Sluis S, de Jong PF, van
[5] Luria AR. Higher Cortical Functions
der Leij A. Executive functioning
in Man. Berlin: Springer Science &
in children, and its relations with
Business Media; 2012
reasoning, reading, and arithmetic.
[6] Jones LB, Rothbart MK, Posner MI.
Intelligence. 2007;35(5):427-449
Development of executive attention
[14] Zelazo PD, Müller U. Executive
in preschool children. Developmental
Science. 2003;6(5):498-504 function in typical and atypical
development. In: Blackwell Handbook
[7] Senn TE, Espy KA, Kaufmann PM. of Childhood Cognitive Development.
Using path analysis to understand Oxford, England: Blackwell Publishers;
executive function organization in 2008. pp. 445-469
preschool children. Developmental
Neuropsychology. 2004;26(1):445-464 [15] Gerstadt CL, Hong YJ, Diamond A.
The relationship between cognition and
[8] Garon N, Bryson SE, Smith IM. action: performance of children 312-7
Executive function in preschoolers: a years old on a stroop-like day-night test.
review using an integrative framework. Cognition. 1994;53(2):129-153
Psychological Bulletin. 2008;134(1):31
[16] Blair C, Razza RP. Relating effortful
[9] Hughes C, Ensor R, Wilson A, control, executive function, and false
Graham A. Tracking executive function belief understanding to emerging math
across the transition to school: A latent and literacy ability in kindergarten.
variable approach. Developmental Child Development. 2007;78(2):647-663
Neuropsychology. 2009;35(1):20-36
[17] Kirkham NZ, Cruess L, Diamond A.
[10] Miyake A, Friedman NP, Helping children apply their
Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, knowledge to their behavior on a

10
A View from the Start: A Review of Inhibitory Control Training in Early Childhood
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/intechopen.88700

dimension-switching task. Develop- from low-income homes: Contributions


mental Science. 2003;6(5): of inhibitory control. Developmental
449-467 Science. 2013;16(1):136-148

[18] Friedman NP, Miyake A. The [26] Barac R, Bialystok E. Bilingual


relations among inhibition and effects on cognitive and linguistic
interference control functions: a development: Role of language, cultural
latent-variable analysis. Journal of background, and education. Child
Experimental Psychology. General. Development. 2012;83(2):413-422
2004;133(1):101
[27] Fishburn FA, Hlutkowsky CO,
[19] Liu Q , Zhu X, Ziegler A, Shi J. The Bemis LM, Huppert TJ, Wakschlag LS,
effects of inhibitory control training Perlman SB. Irritability uniquely
for preschoolers on reasoning ability predicts prefrontal cortex activation
and neural activity. Scientific Reports. during preschool inhibitory control
2015;5:14200 among all temperament domains:
A LASSO approach. NeuroImage.
[20] Carlson SM. Developmentally 2019;184:68-77
sensitive measures of executive function
in preschool children. Developmental [28] Carlson SM, Moses LJ, Hix HR. The
Neuropsychology. 2005;28(2):595-616 role of inhibitory processes in young
children’s difficulties with deception
[21] Allan NP, Hume LE, Allan DM,
and false belief. Child Development.
Farrington AL, Lonigan CJ. Relations
1998;69(3):672-691
between inhibitory control and the
development of academic skills in
[29] Baker ER, Jensen CJ, Tisak MS. A
preschool and kindergarten: A meta-
closer examination of aggressive
analysis. Developmental Psychology.
subtypes in early childhood:
2014;50(10):2368
contributions of executive function
[22] Bull R, Scerif G. Executive
and single-parent status. Early Child
functioning as a predictor of children’s Development and Care. 2019;189(5):
mathematics ability: Inhibition, 733-746
switching, and working memory.
[30] Pietto ML, Giovannetti F,
Developmental Neuropsychology.
2001;19(3):273-293 Segretin MS, Belloli LM,
Lopez-Rosenfeld M, Goldin AP, et al.
[23] Espy KA, McDiarmid MM, Enhancement of inhibitory control in
Cwik MF, Stalets MM, Hamby A, a sample of preschoolers from poor
Senn TE. The contribution of executive homes after cognitive training in a
functions to emergent mathematic skills kindergarten setting: Cognitive and ERP
in preschool children. Developmental evidence. Trends in Neuroscience and
Neuropsychology. 2004;26(1):465-486 Education. 2018;13:34-42

[24] McClelland MM, Cameron CE, [31] Devine RT, Hughes C. Relations
Connor CM, Farris CL, Jewkes AM, between false belief understanding and
Morrison FJ. Links between behavioral executive function in early childhood:
regulation and preschoolers’ A meta-analysis. Child Development.
literacy, vocabulary, and math 2014;85(5):1777-1794
skills. Developmental Psychology.
2007;43(4):947 [32] Zelazo PD, Jacques S. Children’s
rule use: Representation, reflection,
[25] Fuhs MW, McNeil NM. ANS acuity and cognitive control. Annals of Child
and mathematics ability in preschoolers Development. 1997;12:119-176

11
Inhibitory Control Training - A Multidisciplinary Approach

[33] Shahaeian A. Sibling, family, and [40] Duh S, Paik JH, Miller PH, Gluck SC,
social influences on children’s theory Li H, Himelfarb I. Theory of mind and
of mind understanding: New evidence executive function in Chinese preschool
from diverse intracultural samples. children. Developmental Psychology.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2016;52(4):582
2015;46(6):805-820
[41] Farah MJ, Shera DM, Savage JH,
[34] Sabbagh MA, Xu F, Carlson SM, Betancourt L, Giannetta JM,
Moses LJ, Lee K. The development of Brodsky NL, et al. Childhood poverty:
executive functioning and theory of Specific associations with neuro-
mind: A comparison of Chinese and US cognitive development. Brain Research.
preschoolers. Psychological Science. 2006;1110(1):166-174
2006;17(1):74-81
[42] Molzhon A. Exploring the influence
[35] Lan X, Legare CH, Ponitz CC, of socioeconomic status on the
Li S, Morrison FJ. Investigating the executive function and theory of mind
links between the subcomponents skills of preschoolers [dissertation].
of executive function and academic Richmond, VA: Virginia Commonwealth
achievement: A cross-cultural analysis University; 2016
of Chinese and American preschoolers.
Journal of Experimental Child
[43] Ellis BJ. Toward an adaptation-based
Psychology. 2011;108(3):677-692
approach to resilience. In: The Biology
of Early Life Stress. Berlin: Springer;
[36] Kuwabara M, Smith LB. Cross-
2018. pp. 31-43
cultural differences in cognitive
development: Attention to relations and
[44] St. John AM, Kibbe M, Tarullo AR.
objects. Journal of Experimental Child
A systematic assessment of
Psychology. 2012;113(1):20-35
socioeconomic status and executive
functioning in early childhood. Journal
[37] Chasiotis A, Kiessling F,
of Experimental Child Psychology.
Hofer J, Campos D. Theory of mind
2019;178:352-368
and inhibitory control in three cultures:
Conflict inhibition predicts false belief
understanding in Germany, Costa [45] DePrince AP, Weinzierl KM,
Rica and Cameroon. International Combs MD. Executive function
Journal of Behavioral Development. performance and trauma exposure in a
2006;30(3):249-260 community sample of children. Child
Abuse and Neglect. 2009;33(6):353-361
[38] Orta IM, Corapci F, Yagmurlu B,
Aksan N. The mediational role of [46] Duncan RJ, McClelland MM,
effortful control and emotional Acock AC. Relations between executive
dysregulation in the link between function, behavioral regulation,
maternal responsiveness and Turkish and achievement: Moderation by
preschoolers’ social competency and family income. Journal of Applied
externalizing symptoms. Infant and Developmental Psychology. 2017;49:
Child Development. 2013;22(5):459-479 21-30

[39] Baker ER, Jensen CJ, Moeyaert M, [47] Liew J, Chen Q , Hughes JN. Child
Bordoff S. Socioeconomic status and effortful control, teacher–student
early childhood aggression: moderation relationships, and achievement in
by theory of mind for relational, but academically at-risk children: Additive
not physical, aggression. Early Child and interactive effects. Early Child
Development and Care. 2018:1-5 Research Quarterly. 2010;25(1):51-64

12
A View from the Start: A Review of Inhibitory Control Training in Early Childhood
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/intechopen.88700

[48] Rhoades BL, Greenberg MT, Developmental Science. 2009;12(1):


Domitrovich CE. The contribution 106-113
of inhibitory control to preschoolers’
social–emotional competence. Journal [56] Viterbori P, Usai MC, Traverso L,
of Applied Developmental Psychology. De Franchis V. How preschool executive
2009;30(3):310-320 functioning predicts several aspects
of math achievement in Grades 1
[49] Pears KC, Fisher PA, Bruce J, and 3: A longitudinal study. Journal
Kim HK, Yoerger K. Early elementary of Experimental Child Psychology.
school adjustment of maltreated 2015;140:38-55
children in foster care: The roles of
inhibitory control and caregiver [57] Kray J, Ferdinand NK. How
involvement. Child Development. to improve cognitive control in
2010;81(5):1550-1564 development during childhood:
potentials and limits of cognitive
[50] Karbach J, Kray J. How useful interventions. Child Development
is executive control training? Age Perspectives. 2013;7(2):121-125
differences in near and far transfer of
task-switching training. Developmental [58] Traverso L, Viterbori P, Usai MC.
Science. 2009;12(6):978-990 Improving executive function in
childhood: evaluation of a training
[51] Blakey E, Carroll DJ. A short intervention for 5-year-old children.
executive function training program Frontiers in Psychology. 2015;6:525
improves preschoolers’ working
memory. Frontiers in Psychology. [59] Semple RJ, Reid EF, Miller L.
2015;6:1827 Treating anxiety with mindfulness: An
open trial of mindfulness training for
[52] Diamond A, Barnett WS, Thomas J, anxious children. Journal of Cognitive
Munro S. Preschool program Psychotherapy. 2005;19(4):379
improves cognitive control. Science.
2007;318(5855):1387-1388 [60] Semple RJ, Lee J, Rosa D, Miller LF.
A randomized trial of mindfulness-
[53] Dowsett SM, Livesey DJ. The based cognitive therapy for children:
development of inhibitory control in Promoting mindful attention to enhance
preschool children: Effects of “executive social-emotional resiliency in children.
skills” training. Developmental Journal of Child and Family Studies.
Psychobiology: The Journal of the 2010;19(2):218-229
International Society for Developmental
Psychobiology. 2000;36(2):161-174 [61] Beveridge M, Jarrold C, Pettit E. An
experimental approach to executive
[54] Espinet SD, Anderson JE, fingerprinting in young children.
Zelazo PD. N2 amplitude as a neural Infant and Child Development: An
marker of executive function in young International Journal of Research and
children: an ERP study of children Practice. 2002;11(2):107-123
who switch versus perseverate on
the dimensional change card sort.
Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.
2012;2:S49-S58

[55] Thorell LB, Lindqvist S, Bergman


Nutley S, Bohlin G, Klingberg T. Training
and transfer effects of executive
functions in preschool children.

13

You might also like