The Effect of Correcting Neuromyths on Students’ and Teachers’ Later Reasoning
<p>The accuracy of evaluating neuromyths and scenarios in Experiment 1. <span class="html-italic">Note</span>. Error bars depict standard deviations. Accuracy was calculated as the average score recorded on the Likert scale (Likert scale 1–5).</p> "> Figure 2
<p>The accuracy of evaluating neuromyths and scenarios in Experiment 2. <span class="html-italic">Note</span>. Error bars depict standard deviations. Accuracy was calculated as the average score recorded on the Likert scale (Likert scale 1–7).</p> "> Figure 3
<p>The accuracy of evaluating neuromyths and scenarios in Experiment 3, one-week delay. <span class="html-italic">Note</span>. Error bars depict standard deviations. Accuracy was calculated as the average score recorded on the Likert scale (Likert scale 1–7).</p> ">
Abstract
:1. Experiment 1
1.1. Method
1.1.1. Participants
1.1.2. Design and Materials
1.1.3. Procedure
1.1.4. Analytic Plan
1.2. Results and Discussion
1.2.1. Initial Beliefs
1.2.2. Effect of the Corrective Feedback on Statement Accuracy
1.2.3. Effect of Corrective Feedback on Reasoning Accuracy
2. Experiment 2
2.1. Method
2.1.1. Participants
2.1.2. Design and Materials
2.1.3. Procedure
2.1.4. Analytic Plan
2.2. Results and Discussion
2.2.1. Initial Beliefs
2.2.2. Effect of the Corrective Feedback on Statement Accuracy
2.2.3. Effect of the Corrective Feedback on Reasoning
3. Experiment 3
3.1. Method
3.1.1. Participants
3.1.2. Design and Materials
3.1.3. Procedure
3.1.4. Analytic Plan
3.2. Results and Discussion
3.2.1. Initial Beliefs
3.2.2. Effect of Feedback on Statement Accuracy
3.2.3. Effect of Corrective Feedback on Reasoning Accuracy
4. General Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Anderson, Samantha F., and Ken Kelley. 2017. UCSS: Bias and Uncertainty Corrected Sample Size (Version 0.0.2) [R package]. Available online: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/BUCSS/index.html (accessed on 23 September 2024).
- Asterhan, Christa S. C., and Aviv Dotan. 2018. Feedback that corrects and contrasts students’ erroneous solutions with expert ones improves expository instruction for conceptual change. Instructional Science 46: 337–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bates, Douglas, Martin Mächler, Ben Bolker, and Steve Walker. 2014. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. arXiv arXiv:1406.5823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bensley, D. Alan, and Scott O. Lilienfeld. 2017. Psychological misconceptions: Recent scientific advances and unresolved issues. Current Directions in Psychological Science 26: 377–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bensley, D. Alan, Scott O. Lilienfeld, and Lauren. A. Powell. 2014. A new measure of psychological misconceptions: Relations with academic background, critical thinking, and acceptance of paranormal and pseudoscientific claims. Learning and Individual Differences 36: 9–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blanchette Sarrasin, Jérémie, Martin Riopel, and Steve Masson. 2019. Neuromyths and their origin among teachers in Quebec. Mind, Brain, and Education 13: 100–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blasiman, Rachael N., John Dunlosky, and Katherine A. Rawson. 2017. The what, how much, and when of study strategies: Comparing intended versus actual study behaviour. Memory 25: 784–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bransford, John D., Ann L. Brown, and RodneyR. Cocking. 2000. How People Learn. Washington, DC: National Academy Press, vol. 11. [Google Scholar]
- Busso, Daniel. S., and Courtney Pollack. 2015. No brain left behind: Consequences of neuroscience discourse for education. Learning, Media and Technology 40: 168–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cacioppo, John. T., Richar E. Petty, and Chuan Feng Kao. 1984. The efficient assessment of need for cognition. Journal of Personality Assessment 48: 306–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chan, Man-pui Sally, Christopher R. Jones, Kathleen Hall Jamieson, and Dolores Albarracín. 2017. Debunking: A meta-analysis of the psychological efficacy of messages countering misinformation. Psychological Science 28: 1531–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Corallis, Michael C. 2014. Left Brain, Right Brain: Facts and Fantasies. PLoS Biology 12: e1001767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dekker, Sanne, Nikki C. Lee, Paul Howard-Jones, and Jelle Jolles. 2012. Neuromyths in education: Prevalence and predictors of misconceptions among teachers. Frontiers in Psychology 3: 429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Deligiannidi, Karolina, and Paul A. Howard-Jones. 2015. The neuroscience literacy of teachers in Greece. Procedia—Social and Behavioral Sciences 174: 3909–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dechêne, Alice, Christoph Stahl, Jochim Hansen, and Michaela Wänke. 2010. The truth about the truth: A meta-analytic review of the truth effect. Personality and Social Psychology Review 14: 238–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dersch, Anna-Sophia, Alexander Renkl, and Alexander Eitel. 2022. Personalized refutation texts best stimulate teachers’ conceptual change about multimedia learning. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning 38: 977–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dirkx, Kim Josefina Hubertina, Gino Camp, Liesbeth Kester, and Paul Arthur Kirschner. 2019. Do secondary school students make use of effective study strategies when they study on their own? Applied Cognitive Psychology 33: 952–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dunlosky, John, Katherine A. Rawson, Elizabeth J. Marsh, Mitchell J. Nathan, and Daniel T. Willingham. 2013. Improving students’ learning with effective learning techniques: Promising directions from cognitive and educational psychology. Psychological Science in the Public Interest 14: 4–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dündar, Sefa, and Nazan Gündüz. 2016. Misconceptions regarding the brain: The neuromyths of preservice teachers. Mind, Brain, and Education 10: 212–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ecker, Ullrich KH, and Luke M. Antonio. 2021. Can you believe it? An investigation into the impact of retraction source credibility on the continued influence effect. Memory and Cognition 49: 631–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ecker, Ullrich KH, Stephan Lewandowsky, Briony Swire, and Darren Chang. 2011. Correcting false information in memory: Manipulating the strength of misinformation encoding and its retraction. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 18: 570–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferrero, Marta, Pablo Garaizar, and Miguel A. Vadillo. 2016. Neuromyths in education: Prevalence among spanish teachers and an exploration of cross-cultural variation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 10: 496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferrero, Marta, Tom E. Hardwicke, Emmanouil Konstantinidis, and Miguel A. Vadillo. 2020. The effectiveness of refutation texts to correct misconceptions among educators. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 26: 411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fazio, Lisa K., Nadia M. Brashier, B. Keith Payne, and Elizabeth J. Marsh. 2015. Knowledge does not protect against illusory truth. Journal of experimental psychology: General 144: 993–1002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gelman, Rochel, and Joan Lucariello. 2002. Role of learning in cognitive development. In Stevens’ Handbook of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Motivation, and Emotion. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 395–443. [Google Scholar]
- Gleichgerrcht, Ezequiel, Benjamin Lira Luttges, Florencia Salvarezza, and Anna Lucia Campos. 2015. Educational neuromyths among teachers in Latin America. Mind, Brain, and Education 9: 170–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goswami, Usha. 2006. Neuroscience and education: From research to practice? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 7: 406–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guillory, Jimmeka J., and Lisa Geraci. 2010. The persistence of inferences in memory for younger and older adults: Remembering facts and believing inferences. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 17: 73–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guillory, Jimmeka J., and Lisa Geraci. 2013. Correcting erroneous inferences in memory: The role of source credibility. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition 2: 201–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guillory, Jimmeka J., and Lisa Geraci. 2016. The persistence of erroneous information in memory: The effect of valence on the acceptance of corrected information. Applied Cognitive Psychology 30: 282–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Horvath, Jared Cooney, Gregory M. Donoghue, Alex J. Horton, Jason M. Lodge, and John AC Hattie. 2018. On the irrelevance of neuromyths to teacher effectiveness: Comparing neuro-literacy levels amongst award-winning and non-award winning teachers. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 1666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Howard-Jones, Paul A. 2014. Neuroscience and education: Myths and messages. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 15: 817–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hughes, Brenda, Karen A. Sullivan, and Linda Gilmore. 2020. Why do teachers believe educational neuromyths? Trends in Neuroscience and Education 21: 100145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, Hollyn M., and Colleen M. Seifert. 1994. Johnson, Hollyn M., and Colleen M. Seifert. 1994. Sources of the continued influence effect: When misinformation in memory affects later inferences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 20: 1420–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Judd, Charles M., Jacob Westfall, and David A. Kenny. 2017. Experiments with more than one random factor: Designs, analytic models, and statistical power. Annual Review of Psychology 68: 601–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Karakus, Ozge, Paul A. Howard-Jones, and Tim Jay. 2015. Primary and secondary school teachers’ knowledge and misconceptions about the brain in Turkey. Procedia—Social and Behavioral Sciences 174: 1933–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keith, Melissa G., Louis Tay, and Peter D. Harms. 2017. Systems perspective of Amazon Mechanical Turk for organizational research: Review and recommendations. Frontiers in Psychology 8: 1359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kendeou, Panayiota, and Edward J. O’Brien. 2014. The knowledge revision components (KReC) framework: Processes and mechanisms. In Processing Inaccurate Information: Theoretical and Applied Perspectives from Cognitive Science and the Educational Sciences. Edited by David N. Rapp and Jason L. G. Braasch. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 353–77. [Google Scholar]
- Khramova, Marina V., Tatyana V. Bukina, Nikita M. Smirnov, Semen A. Kurkin, and Alexander E. Hramov. 2023. Khramova, Marina V., Tatyana V. Bukina, Nikita M. Smirnov, Semen A. Kurkin, and Alexander E. Hramov. 2023. Prevalence of neuromyths among students and pre-service teachers. Humanities and Social Sciences Communications 10: 950. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kowalski, Patricia, and Annette Kujawski Taylor. 2017. Reducing students’ misconceptions with refutational teaching: For long-term retention, comprehension matters. Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Psychology 3: 90–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krammer, Georg, Stephan E. Vogel, and Roland H. Grabner. 2021. Believing in neuromyths makes neither a bad nor good student-teacher: The relationship between neuromyths and academic achievement in teacher education. Mind, Brain, and Education 15: 54–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lewandowsky, Stephan, Ullrich KH Ecker, Colleen M. Seifert, Norbert Schwarz, and John Cook. 2012. Misinformation and its correction: Continued influence and successful debiasing. Psychological Science in the Public Interest 13: 106–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lithander, Marcus. P. G., Lisa Geraci, Meltem Karaca, and Jason Rydberg. 2021. Correcting neuromyths: A comparison of different types of refutations. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition 10: 577–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Loibl, Katharina, and Nikol Rummel. 2014. Knowing what you don’t know makes failure productive. Learning and Instruction 34: 74–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lucariello, Joan, Michele T. Tine, and Colleen M. Ganley. 2014. A formative assessment of students’ algebraic variable misconceptions. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior 33: 30–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Macdonald, Kelly, Laura Germine, Alida Anderson, Joanna Christodoulou, and Lauren M. McGrath. 2017. Dispelling the myth: Training in education or neuroscience decreases but does not eliminate beliefs in neuromyths. Frontiers in Psychology 8: 1314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McDaniel, Mark A., and Gilles O. Einstein. 2020. Training learning strategies to promote self-regulation and transfer: The knowledge, belief, commitment, and planning framework. Perspectives on Psychological Science 15: 1363–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Montgomery, Jacob M., Brendan Nyhan, and Michelle Torres. 2018. How conditioning on posttreatment variables can ruin your experiment and what to do about it. American Journal of Political Science 62: 760–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morehead, Kayla, Matthew G. Rhodes, and Sarah DeLozier. 2016. Instructor and student knowledge of study strategies. Memory 24: 257–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Newton, Philip M., and Atharva Salvi. 2020. How common is belief in the learning styles neuromyth, and does it matter? A pragmatic systematic review. Frontiers in Education 5: 270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Newton, Philip M., and Mahallad Miah. 2017. Evidence-based higher education—Is the learning styles ‘myth’ important? Frontiers in Psychology 8: 444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nielsen, Jared A., Brandon A. Zielinski, Michael A. Ferguson, Janet E. Lainhart, and Jeffrey S. Anderson. 2013. An evaluation of the left-brain vs. Right-brain hypothesis with resting state functional connectivity magnetic resonance imaging. PLoS ONE 8: e71275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Papadatou-Pastou, Marietta, Eleni Haliou, and Filippos Vlachos. 2017. Brain knowledge and the prevalence of neuromyths among prospective teachers in Greece. Frontiers in Psychology 8: 804. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pashler, Harold, Mark McDaniel, Doug Rohrer, and Robert Bjork. 2008. Learning styles: Concepts and evidence. Psychological Science in the Public Interest 9: 105–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paynter, Jessica, Sarah Luskin-Saxby, Deb Keen, Kathryn Fordyce, Grace Frost, Christine Imms, Scott Miller, David Trembath, Madonna Tucker, and Ullrich Ecker. 2019. Evaluation of a template for countering misinformation—Real-world Autism treatment myth debunking. PLoS ONE 14: e0210746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pei, X., Paul A. Howard-Jones, S. Zhang, X. Liu, and Y. Jin. 2015. Teachers’ understanding about the brain in East China. Procedia—Social and Behavioral Sciences 174: 3681–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rich, Patrick R., Mariëtte H. Van Loon, John Dunlosky, and Maria S. Zaragoza. 2017. Belief in corrective feedback for common misconceptions: Implications for knowledge revision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 43: 492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rousseau, Luc. 2021. Interventions to dispel neuromyths in educational settings—A review. Frontiers in Psychology 12: 719692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Salovich, Nikita A., Amalia M. Donovan, Scott R. Hinze, and David N. Rapp. 2021. Can confidence help account for and redress the effects of reading inaccurate information? Memory & Cognition 49: 293–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwarz, Norbert, Eryn Newman, and William Leach. 2016. Making the truth stick & the myths fade: Lessons from cognitive psychology. Behavioral Science & Policy 2: 85–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sosu, Edward M. 2013. The development and psychometric validation of a Critical Thinking Disposition Scale. Thinking Skills and Creativity 9: 107–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Soto, Christopher J. 2019. Big five inventory--2: How replicable are links between personality traits and consequential life outcomes? The Life Outcomes of Personality Replication Project. Psychological Science 30: 711–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Swami, Viren, Ulrich S. Tran, Stefan Stieger, Jakob Pietschnig, Ingo W. Nader, and Martin Voracek. 2016. Who believes in the giant skeleton myth? An examination of individual difference correlates. SAGE Open 6: 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Swire, Briony, and Ullrich Ecker. 2018. Misinformation and its correction: Cognitive mechanisms and recommendations for mass communication. In Misinformation and Mass Audiences. Edited by Brian G. Southwell, Emily A. Thorson and Laura Sheble. Austin: University of Texas Press, pp. 195–211. [Google Scholar]
- Swire, Briony, Ullrich KH Ecker, and Stephan Lewandowsky. 2017. The role of familiarity in correcting inaccurate information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 43: 1948–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Swire-Thompson, Briony, Mitch Dobbs, Ayanna Thomas, and Joseph DeGutis. 2023. Memory failure predicts belief regression after the correction of misinformation. Cognition 230: 105276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taylor, Douglas J., and Keith E. Muller. 1996. Bias in linear model power and sample size calculations due to estimating noncentrality. Communications in Statistics: Theory and Methods 25: 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Torrijos-Muelas, Marta, Sixto González-Víllora, and Ana Rosa Bodoque-Osma. 2021. The persistence of neuromyths in the educational settings: A systematic review. Frontiers in Psychology 11: 591923. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tovazzi, Alice, Serena Giovannini, and Demis Basso. 2020. A new method for evaluating knowledge, beliefs, and neuromyths about the mind and brain among italian teachers. Mind, Brain, and Education 14: 187–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van Dijk, Wilhelmina, and Holly B. Lane. 2020. The brain and the US education system: Perpetuation of neuromyths. Exceptionality 28: 16–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Loon, Mariëtte H., John Dunlosky, Tamara Van Gog, Jeroen Jg Van Merriënboer, and Anique Bh De Bruin. 2015. Refutations in science texts lead to hypercorrection of misconceptions held with high confidence. Contemporary Educational Psychology 42: 39–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Walter, Nathan, and Riva Tukachinsky. 2020. A meta-analytic examination of the continued influence of misinformation in the face of correction: How powerful is it, why does it happen, and how to stop it? Communication Research 47: 155–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wells, Chris, Justin Reedy, John Gastil, and Carolyn Lee. 2009. Information distortion and voting choices: The origins and effects of factual beliefs in initiative elections. Political Psychology 30: 953–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilkes, A. L., and Mark Leatherbarrow. 1988. Editing episodic memory following the identification of error. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 40: 361–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Willingham, Daniel T., Elizabeth M. Hughes, and David G. Dobolyi. 2015. The scientific status of learning styles theories. Teaching of Psychology 42: 266–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Condition | Number of Participants (n) | Initial Test | Statements | Scenarios | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | ||
No Feedback | 33 | 0.40 | 0.14 | 0.35 | 0.20 | 0.43 | 0.29 |
Feedback Only | 34 | 0.41 | 0.16 | 0.73 | 0.18 | 0.66 | 0.32 |
Feedback and Explanation | 43 | 0.41 | 0.13 | 0.74 | 0.24 | 0.68 | 0.30 |
110 | 0.41 | 0.13 | 0.62 | 0.26 | 0.60 | 0.32 |
Outcome | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Predictors | Estimates | Odds Ratio | SE | p | z-Value |
Intercept | −1.22 | 0.30 | 0.27 | <0.001 | −4.54 |
Feedback-only (vs. control) | 2.13 | 8.40 | 0.30 | <0.001 | 7.17 |
Feedback–explanation (vs. control) | 2.34 | 10.34 | 0.29 | <0.001 | 8.17 |
Feedback–explanation (vs. feedback-only) | 0.21 | 1.23 | 0.28 | 0.46 | 0.74 |
Initial accuracy (statement) | 1.27 | 3.57 | 0.08 | <0.001 | 8.71 |
Initial confidence in statement evaluation | 0.06 | 1.07 | 0.76 | 0.45 | 0.76 |
Delayed confidence in statement evaluation | 0.38 | 1.46 | 5.47 | <0.001 | 5.47 |
Influenced behavior in life | −0.28 | 0.76 | −3.22 | <0.01 | −3.22 |
Random Effects | |||||
σ2 | 3.29 | ||||
τ00ID | 1.08 | ||||
τ00Question | 0.50 | ||||
ICC | 0.51 | ||||
NQuestion | 20 | ||||
NID | 110 | ||||
Total number of observations | 2200 |
Outcome | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Predictors | Estimates | SE | p | df |
Intercept | 3.45 | 0.20 | <0.001 | 84.39 |
Feedback-only (vs. control) | 1.25 | 0.22 | <0.001 | 105.80 |
Feedback–explanation (vs. control) | 1.39 | 0.20 | <0.001 | 105.53 |
Feedback–explanation (vs. feedback-only) | 0.14 | 0.20 | 0.48 | 105.07 |
Initial accuracy (statement) | 0.50 | 0.08 | <0.001 | 2089.28 |
Initial confidence in statement evaluation | 0.01 | 0.04 | 0.77 | 2179.87 |
Delayed confidence in scenario evaluation | 0.33 | 0.04 | <0.001 | 2109.66 |
Influenced behavior in life | −0.03 | 0.05 | 0.46 | 2163.09 |
Random Effects | ||||
σ2 | 3.01 | |||
τ00ID | 0.68 | |||
τ00Question | 0.29 | |||
ICC | 0.32 | |||
NQuestion | 20 | |||
NID | 110 | |||
Total number of observations | 2200 |
Condition | Number of Participants (n) | Initial Test | Statements | Scenarios | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | ||
No Feedback | 44 | 0.45 | 0.32 | 0.42 | 0.29 | 0.30 | 0.20 |
Feedback Only | 38 | 0.42 | 0.31 | 0.54 | 0.31 | 0.36 | 0.21 |
Feedback and Explanation | 40 | 0.45 | 0.32 | 0.63 | 0.30 | 0.41 | 0.21 |
122 | 0.44 | 0.32 | 0.53 | 0.32 | 0.35 | 0.21 |
Outcome | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Predictors | Estimates | SE | p | df |
Intercept | 2.25 | 0.18 | <0.001 | 103.49 |
Feedback-only (vs. control) | 0.81 | 0.18 | <0.001 | 118.08 |
Feedback–explanation (vs. control) | 1.28 | 0.17 | <0.001 | 118.06 |
Feedback–explanation (vs. feedback-only) | 0.47 | 0.18 | 0.01 | 117.68 |
Initial belief (statement) | 0.47 | 0.03 | <0.001 | 2256.15 |
Initial confidence in statement evaluation | 0.09 | 0.04 | 0.02 | 2430.29 |
Delayed confidence in statement evaluation | 0.08 | 0.04 | 0.03 | 2422.69 |
Influenced behavior in life | −0.02 | 0.04 | 0.67 | 2298.90 |
Random Effects | ||||
σ2 | 1.92 | |||
τ00ID | 0.21 | |||
τ00Question | 0.53 | |||
ICC | 0.28 | |||
NQuestion | 20 | |||
NID | 122 | |||
Total number of observations | 2440 |
Outcome | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Predictors | Estimates | SE | p | df |
Intercept | 2.18 | 0.13 | <0.001 | 95.86 |
Feedback-only (vs. control) | 0.40 | 0.13 | <0.001 | 117.87 |
Feedback–explanation (vs. control) | 0.65 | 0.13 | <0.01 | 117.79 |
Feedback–explanation (vs. feedback-only) | 0.25 | 0.13 | <0.01 | 117.42 |
Initial belief (statement) | 0.22 | 0.02 | <0.001 | 2331.56 |
Initial confidence in statement evaluation | −0.01 | 0.03 | 0.79 | 2429.90 |
Delayed confidence in scenario evaluation | 0.11 | 0.02 | <0.001 | 2417.42 |
Influenced behavior in life | −0.04 | 0.03 | 0.22 | 2352.60 |
Random Effects | ||||
σ2 | 0.93 | |||
τ00ID | 0.30 | |||
τ00Question | 0.13 | |||
ICC | 0.32 | |||
NQuestion | 20 | |||
NID | 122 | |||
Total number of observations | 2440 |
Condition | Number of Participants (n) | Initial Test | One-Week Delay Statements | One-Week Delay Scenarios | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | ||
No Feedback | 38 | 0.32 | 0.30 | 0.33 | 0.30 | 0.35 | 0.31 |
Feedback Only | 31 | 0.26 | 0.27 | 0.35 | 0.36 | 0.35 | 0.35 |
Feedback and Explanation | 35 | 0.32 | 0.30 | 0.41 | 0.36 | 0.41 | 0.35 |
104 | 0.30 | 0.29 | 0.36 | 0.34 | 0.37 | 0.34 |
Outcome | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Predictors | Estimates | SE | p | df |
Intercept | 1.62 | 0.14 | <0.001 | 136.78 |
Feedback-only (vs. control) | 0.24 | 0.18 | 0.19 | 95.12 |
Feedback–explanation (vs. control) | 0.36 | 0.17 | 0.04 | 94.43 |
Feedback–explanation (vs. feedback-only) | 0.12 | 0.18 | 0.51 | 95.32 |
Initial belief (statement) | 0.29 | 0.02 | <0.001 | 1988.39 |
Initial confidence in statement evaluation | −0.03 | 0.03 | 0.29 | 2038.18 |
Delayed confidence in statement evaluation | 0.05 | 0.03 | 0.07 | 2057.33 |
Influenced behavior in life | −0.14 | 0.04 | <0.001 | 2028.22 |
Random Effects | ||||
σ2 | 0.89 | |||
τ00ID | 0.51 | |||
τ00Question | 0.02 | |||
ICC | 0.47 | |||
NQuestion | 20 | |||
NID | 104 | |||
Total number of observations | 2067 |
Outcome | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Predictors | Estimates | SE | p | df |
Intercept | 2.06 | 0.15 | <0.001 | 139.68 |
Feedback-only (vs. control) | 0.08 | 0.19 | 0.87 | 95.13 |
Feedback–explanation (vs. control) | 0.25 | 0.19 | 0.21 | 94.59 |
Feedback–explanation (vs. feedback-only) | 0.17 | 0.20 | 0.39 | 95.21 |
Initial belief (statement) | 0.14 | 0.02 | <0.001 | 2005.41 |
Initial confidence in statement evaluation | −0.00 | 0.03 | 0.89 | 2034.58 |
Delayed confidence in scenario evaluation. | 0.06 | 0.03 | 0.03 | 2049.08 |
Influenced behavior in life | −0.16 | 0.04 | <0.001 | 2043.32 |
Random Effects | ||||
σ2 | 0.98 | |||
τ00ID | 0.59 | |||
τ00Question | 0.04 | |||
ICC | 0.39 | |||
NQuestion | 20 | |||
NID | 104 | |||
Total number of observations | 2067 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Lithander, M.P.G.; Geraci, L.; Karaca, M.; Hunsberger, R. The Effect of Correcting Neuromyths on Students’ and Teachers’ Later Reasoning. J. Intell. 2024, 12, 98. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12100098
Lithander MPG, Geraci L, Karaca M, Hunsberger R. The Effect of Correcting Neuromyths on Students’ and Teachers’ Later Reasoning. Journal of Intelligence. 2024; 12(10):98. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12100098
Chicago/Turabian StyleLithander, Marcus Per Gustaf, Lisa Geraci, Meltem Karaca, and Renee Hunsberger. 2024. "The Effect of Correcting Neuromyths on Students’ and Teachers’ Later Reasoning" Journal of Intelligence 12, no. 10: 98. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12100098
APA StyleLithander, M. P. G., Geraci, L., Karaca, M., & Hunsberger, R. (2024). The Effect of Correcting Neuromyths on Students’ and Teachers’ Later Reasoning. Journal of Intelligence, 12(10), 98. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12100098