Figures
Abstract
The insect central complex appears to encode and process spatial information through vector manipulation. Here, we draw on recent insights into circuit structure to fuse previous models of sensory-guided navigation, path integration and vector memory. Specifically, we propose that the allocentric encoding of location provided by path integration creates a spatially stable anchor for converging sensory signals that is relevant in multiple behavioural contexts. The allocentric reference frame given by path integration transforms a goal direction into a goal location and we demonstrate through modelling that it can enhance approach of a sensory target in noisy, cluttered environments or with temporally sparse stimuli. We further show the same circuit can improve performance in the more complex navigational task of route following. The model suggests specific functional roles for circuit elements of the central complex that helps explain their high preservation across insect species.
Author summary
Even tiny animals with reduced neuronal resources need to solve 2 dimension spatial problems. In this paper, we modelled a neural network, based on the central complex connectivity, that sustains insect visual-guided navigation both to a landmark and following a previously learned route. This combined different features that have been previously highlighted in the insect brain, (1) an inner compass, allowing an allocentric representation of their orientation, (2) a positioning system, inherited from their ability to integrate their path, (3) a long-term memory of relevant locations in the environment, that allows insect to revisit feeders repeatedly for example, and (4) a sensory guidance system that provides a stable goal direction when a rewarded signal is provided. We combined these different circuits in an complementary fashion, suggesting a crucial role for path integration in all insect navigation, beyond simply ensuring homing behaviour. Specifically, using the capability of the central complex neuronal circuit to store and manipulate navigational vectors, our implementation encodes the allocentric position of the navigation goal by combining a sensory-based vector, directed toward a goal, and a homing vector, directed toward a stable origin. We show this improves significantly the navigation in two visual-guidance paradigms, reaching a distant recognised target and following a route based on a panoramic memory.
Citation: Goulard R, Heinze S, Webb B (2023) Emergent spatial goals in an integrative model of the insect central complex. PLoS Comput Biol 19(12): e1011480. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1011480
Editor: Joseph Ayers, Northeastern University, UNITED STATES
Received: August 31, 2023; Accepted: December 1, 2023; Published: December 18, 2023
Copyright: © 2023 Goulard et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: The figures dataset and the model python code are available at the following address: https://github.com/RomanGoulard/CXModel_SpaceRep_Data.
Funding: RG and SH are funded by the European Research Council under the grant agreement 101044220 (Recipient: SH) - EvolvingCircuits. SH is funded by the European Union, Horizon Europe, Project 101046790 – InsectNeuroNano. Views and opinions expressed are however those of the author(s) only and do not necessarily reflect those of the European Union or the European Innovation Council. Neither the European Union nor the European Innovation Council can be held responsible for them. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
Mobile animals need to navigate through space for a diverse range of tasks: foraging for food or hunting prey, searching for mates, avoiding danger, returning to a nest. The nature of the neural encodings and computations that underlie spatial navigation have long been a central question in cognitive neuroscience. In mammals, the discovery of place, head direction and grid cells [1, 2] has been taken to support the existence of an internal “cognitive map” [3, 4]. In insects, despite some species displaying remarkable navigational capabilities, the possibility that their relatively small brains could support a cognitive map remains a point of debate [5–7]. One way this debate could be resolved is by elucidating the actual neural mechanisms that support navigation.
During the last decade, the discovery of an internal compass (resembling head direction cells from mammals) in the central complex (CX) of insects [8, 9] has lead to a rapid advance in understanding of how insect brains encode and process spatial information (Fig 1). The CX has a crossroad position in the brain between sensory inputs and motor outputs [10] and its structure is highly conserved across species [10, 11]; with many circuit properties interpretable as functional components of navigation [12]. This includes what appears to be a complete circuit to support path integration [13]—monitoring displacement (direction and distance) over time to estimate current position relative to an origin (maintaining a “home vector”) [14, 15], and guiding the animal back to the origin. In addition to this positioning system, the CX hosts circuits that effectively generate, transform and use navigation vectors (Fig 2): the projection geometry of intrinsic neurons appears well suited to compute vector rotation and vector addition. Consequently, vectorial operations allow the computation of some of the circuit properties, for example the virtual 180° shift observed in the FB subserves the transformation of the allocentric head direction into an egocentric representation of the insect’s traveling direction [16, 17]. The same principle applied to wind compass input allows Drosophila to reverse their up-wind/down-wind behaviour based on the odour context [18].
(A) Inputs to the CX from different other brain regions. We consider these inputs as three types. Sensory Compasses Insects are sensitive to various cues that give information about their allocentric orientation. Sensory compass pathways converge to the ellipsoid body (EB) where they connect with the EPGs, mostly through Ring Neurons (ER). Movement perception The CX receives information about self motion at the level of the Noduli (NO), mostly from visual (optic flow) and/or mechanosensory (proprioception) origin. Higher level neuropils The CX receives a large number of inputs from higher level regions that process multisensory information. Mushroom Bodies (MB) and Lateral Horns (LH), for example, mostly connect to the CX at the level of the fan-shaped body (FB) through FB tangential (FBt) neurons. (B) CX neuropils and the different modeled neuron type connectivity. The whole circuitry can roughly be segmented in three different functional parts, (1) the inner compass circuit (Fig D in S1 Text), represented on the top right by 4 cell types, EPG, PEG, PEN and Δ7, and projecting in-between the EB to the protocerebral bridge (PB), (2) the steering circuit (Fig 3), represented on the top left, is located between the PB, the FB and the lateral accessory lobes (LAL), including mainly 3 cell types, PFN, hΔ and PFL, and (3) a long-term vector memory circuit (Fig 4), represented by the parallel neural types FBt and DAN (Dopaminergic neurons) presenting a tangential projection pattern across the whole FB functional columns.
A-C Different model abstraction levels. (A) Vector operations underlying the transformation of the current and desired headings into a steering output signal. (C) Neurons population activity through the different CX neuropils involved in the steering circuit. (B) Mathematical principles of the transformation of the neuron population activities into a steering output signal. I-IV Steps of the transformation from a heading and desired heading to the steering output signal. (I) Compass level. The neuron activity has a bump at the level of the ellipsoid body that follows the allocentric orientation of the agent. This bump, as well as the bump representing the goal orientation, take the form of a sinusoid in the model, where the peak position and amplitude can be interpreted as respectively the direction and length of a vector. (II) Compass signal copy. At the level of the protocerebral bridge the compass signal is copied into 2 corresponding hemispheric signals. This allows idiothetic rotation cues to move the compass (see Fig D in S1 Text) and, downstream, a comparison of the compass with the goal direction signals. (III) Compass and goal direction comparison. At the level of the fan-shaped body, the two copies of the compass signal are compared with rightward and leftward shifted copies of the desired heading. This allows the identification of the required direction of rotation to align with the desired orientation. (IV) Left and right hemisphere comparison. The summation of resulting population activity in each hemisphere, at the level of the lateral accessory lobes, results in a differential signal (left-right) that can be used to generate the appropriate turn toward the desired orientation. Clipart(s) in the figure have been modified from https://openclipart.org/.
An important insight common to these models is that the CX provides a geometric representation of space [7] and processes and combines spatial information through vector manipulation [19, 20]. To explain some sophisticated abilities in bees, such as novel short-cuts between food sources, and development of efficient trap-lines, i.e. the visit of multiple feeders in a predictable and often optimised sequence, it has been hypothesised that the CX also supports vector memories [7, 20]. That is, at rewarded locations, the CX may store the current state of the path integrator and selectively reload this memory to drive movement back to the same location.
At the same time, the conserved nature of the CX circuit suggests it plays a role in the spatial behaviour of all insects, not just central place foragers, species like wasps, bees or ants inhabiting a nest, for which efficient relocation of the nest and food sources by individuals is crucial to survival [21]. Indeed, path integration has also been observed, over smaller spatial scales, in Drosophila which do not possess a fixed nest or home [22, 23]. The existence of homing in “nomadic” insects allows them to revisit a rewarded location, i.e. a food source, while exploring the environment in search for new ones [24], optimizing the exploitation of the resources. This has been supported by the behavioural observation in Drosophila of a reset of the path integration origin whenever an optogenetic simulation of food reward was experienced [25]. An even more basic role for the CX that we have suggested in previous work is to enhance localisation of innate or learned attractive stimuli by creating persistence in a given motion direction relative to other cues [26], which allows noisy, intermittently available, or currently invisible stimuli to be tracked more efficiently. Insects shows particular ability to maintain a straight route using a convergence of sensory information, so-called menotaxis, that have been related to the CX processing [27, 28]. The ability of the CX to generate and maintain navigation vectors to sustain an oriented behaviour even in absence of new sensory information is particularly adapted to support menotactic behaviours. It is, in addition, supported by the wide range of sensory streams converging to the CX, particularly in the fan-shaped body (FB) [15, 29–31]. This CX substructure has been proposed to be the centre of the comparison between the insect’s own orientation (compass) and its current goal orientation (Fig 2). The coexistence of multiple directional vectors, potentially representing competing goals, in the CX raises the question of their interactions together and with PI [32–34] to generate consistent and optimized behaviour [35].
In the current work we consider whether the anatomy of the central complex could support smooth interaction of these various forms of vectors to improve spatial navigation. More specifically, we explore whether visually guided navigation (to a visual target, or along a familiar route) can be enhanced by CX memory, using an anatomically plausible circuit model that incorporates new connectivity data. The key idea is that progress towards a goal in noisy, cluttered environments or with temporally sparse information can be scaffolded by memory of the PI location and heading direction when the insect last knew it was ‘on course’, which together produce an emergent internal goal. As such, this is a generic mechanism for enhancing spatial behaviour that could be common to a wide range of insects, yet easily co-opted to support the more sophisticated navigation of central place foragers.
Model
Overview and biological motivation
The CX model implemented for this paper builds on our previous work [26]. The model and its biological counterparts are shown in Fig 1. A core component of the model is constituted by the inner compass system in the insect brain [11]. This circuit in the CX (specifically in the EB, see Compass circuit section) is composed of four neuron subsets called EPGs, PEGs, PENs and Δ7 (see Table 1 for name correspondence with other insects species) constituting a so-called ring attractor [10], i.e. a circuit that generates a stable representation of a circular variable, here the immediate heading direction. This head direction circuit generates an activity ‘bump’, in the form of a sinusoidal activity pattern across the population of neurons (EPGs) that tracks the animal’s allocentric heading [9, 37].
Originally from Sayre et al., 2021 [36].
To generate the ‘bump’, we assume an accurate perception of orientation is provided by the multiplicity of sensory pathways in insects that detect directional cues [38–40] (Fig 1A). In insects these include visual cues, both from the sky (sun [41] and polarised light [42]) and the landscape [43], and also wind cues sensed by antennal displacement [44–46]. This information is carried to the EB by so-called ring neurons (ER), their topology forming plastic inhibitory synapses across the whole EB compartments [8, 43, 47]. The orientation of the different sensory compasses are dynamically imprinted, linking the egocentric orientation of a specific cue (visual, wind, …) with a stable inhibitory pattern, in a unified reference frame [48]. ER neurons modulate downstream EPG population activity to generate an integrative inner compass. In addition, the EB receives input, via the PENs, from sensing self-rotation through optic flow or proprioception, which can move the compass bump even in the absence of external orientation cues [49, 50].
This compass circuit then feeds into a steering circuit, which is based on a previous model of the CX as a circuit for path integration (PI) [13]. The steering circuit function is based on the phase-shift observed in the connectivity pattern of neuron groups projecting to or into the FB [16–18](Fig 1B). This particular geometry allows the comparison, at the PFL neurons level, of the current heading, carried by the direct inhibitory connection from Δ7 to PFL, with outputs from the PFN and hΔ, both excitatory, carrying the desired heading, e.g. goal direction (Fig 2). These two pathways can be used alternatively in the model to define this goal vector based either on the head direction circuit [26] or on the PI [13]. PFL project onto descending neurons directly involved in the motor control of turns in Drosophila [51]. The hypothesised ability of the CX circuit to compare heading and goal direction have been recently supported by functional neuroimaging and modeling studies [31, 52].
To form the homing vector in the model, we assume that sensory information about self-translation (from optic flow [13, 53, 54] or proprioception [55, 56]) is projected via the noduli to PFN[36]. Similarly to [13], we assume accumulation of this signal across a set of directionally tuned neurons forms the home vector memory, however, in contrast to the previous model, here we locate this accumulation in the hΔ neurons. As yet, there is no direct evidence for such an accumulation property in either the PFN or hΔ neurons, and the previous suggestion [13] that recurrent connections within a CX column might support integration has been challenged by cold anesthesia experiments on ants [57] and dung beetles [58], which observed a memory impairment of the PI distance only, while on bumblebees no effect was observed at smaller scale [59]. Nevertheless, the FB remains the most likely location of PI.
Finally, we implement a long-term vector memory based on a previous model of the CX as a vector-based navigation center [20]. This is consistent with the projection pattern of a set of neurons, FBt, that are known to receive inputs from different sensory/memory streams outside the CX (including from the mushroom bodies) and project across the whole columns of the FB [36, 60]. Moreover, we assume these neurons are modulated by context and valence, and by connecting to either PFN or hΔ, the system as a whole can dynamically adjust its navigation behaviour to changing needs or motivation. Recently, a complete pathway has been unravelled from the MB to the FB and functionally shown to control upwind behaviour in the presence of a specific odor [18], supporting the existence of several context-dependent goals and justifying the position of the FB as a modulator of them.
Implementation
The model was implemented in Python 3.6. Each neuron is represented by a simple firing rate model [61]. The neuron activation rule can be of two types, either a linear function of the input (linear simple perceptron) capped between 0 and 1 or a logic function (active or inactive based on a threshold). The input function is the sum of the activity rate of the pre-synaptic neurons. The specific connectivity between neurons is defined in a connectivity matrix (Fig C in S1 Text) and the activity level at each time step is calculated by the multiplication of the activity vector of the neurons on the previous time step by this matrix: (1) With Nact a vector containing the rate activity of all the neurons of the CX model and MCX a matrix of connectivity between these neurons. The connectivity matrix is derived from known connectomic data of the main CX neuron groups (Fig 1). We particularly make use of the repeated columnar organisation of the different neuron types across the CX neuropils (EB ↔ PB ↔ FB). Although interspecific differences exist in the CX connections, we aimed here at unraveling general function and therefore did not target a specific species connectome but rather use representative projection patterns.
Inputs.
Sensory compasses. Insects brain rely on a variety of sensory inputs to generate the activity bump in the EB [8, 9] which acts as an inner compass able to sustain navigation [62]. In this model, we simplify the contribution of the different sensory compasses by having the EPGs receive input from Compass Units (, for Input from the Compasses to the EB, n = 8) with activity directly based on the agent’s orientation in space (Fig D in S1 Text). Each unit has a preferred orientation, homogeneously distributed every 45° to cover the entire 360° around the agent. Their activity is calculated following a winner-take-all rule, such that only the unit with its preferred orientation closest to the agent orientation is active (1) while all the other are inactive (0). Note that, with adjustment of the gain between EPG and Δ7 (), the circuit also supports a compass input as a form of a sinusoid-shaped signal (Fig E in S1 Text).
Self-motion perception. Insects, and animals in general, keep track of their movements, both rotations and translations, in space for different purposes [63, 64] through sensory pathways that are fairly conserved in the insect brain [65]. In this model, we represented the perception of self-motion in the insect through two independent pathways based on the agent’s translational and rotational speed respectively. In practice, the agent is moving at constant speed in its heading direction, so the translational speed pathway consists of a single neuron (, for Inputs of the Translational Speed to the Noduli) with a constant activity, which projects to both noduli and synapses with the PFNs (). The rotational speed is variable and is segmented laterally into two inputs, one for each hemisphere (Fig D in S1 Text). The activity of the neurons (ITRNO, for Inputs of the Rotational Speed to the Noduli) is binary, set to 1 (turn) in the left or right hemisphere in response to right or left turns respectively. The binary nature of these inputs might impact the ability of the compass to be maintained accurately in absence of compass inputs. As we did not simulate any disturbance in the compass inputs here, the role of the PEN is not crucial and have been included only to match the complete head direction circuit.
Contextual inputs. In addition to current sensory and self-motion inputs, the model assumes that the behavioural context provides internal states that influence the CX through tangential neuron inputs to the FB (Fig 1). We treat these inputs as two independent signals: FBt which controls the navigational goal (e.g. home or food); and DANs which signal reward in a given context and control the formation of vector memory.
Compass circuit.
The compass circuit is designed to transform external information, acquired by diverse sensory pathways described in insects, into an inner sinusoidal function representing the immediate orientation of the agent at any time. It is similar to the one we used in our previous model work [26] and is composed of 4 different cell types, EPG, PEG, PEN and Δ7 (Fig D in S1 Text). The EPG layer receives external inputs from the sensory compass (Fig D in S1 Text). The activity rate of the different neuron types is calculated by the following set of equations: (2) Where indicates the different gain parameters regulating the strength of inputs from the cell type i to the cell type j (see Table A in S1 Text for the value of the different k parameters). All together, this circuit transforms the signal from a single activity bump, inherited from the sensory compass inputs into a temporally smoothed sinusodial signal at the level of the Δ7 neurons (Fig D in S1 Text), that persists after the input is removed and can be moved by rotational self-motion perception. This inner allocentric compass is then transmitted further in the CX to the steering circuit, the PFNs and the PFLs layers by PB intrinsic neurons called Δ7 (Fig 1B).
Steering circuit.
The steering circuit is primarily designed to compute the error between the compass and goal vectors [31, 52] and generates an asymmetrical signal that is decoded as the turning force for the agent (Fig 2). In addition, it holds the two navigation vectors, the heading and homing vectors, that are used to generate the behaviour through the vector memory (see next section). The circuit is largely inspired by the PI circuit previously described by Stone et al. [13] and is composed here of 3 neuron types (Fig 3A), PFN, hΔ, and PFL. Both PFNs and PFLs receive inhibitory inputs from the compass via Δ7. In addition, PFNs receive translational self-motion inputs from the noduli, consequently building a copy of the current compass orientation vector, modulated in amplitude by the translational speed. Note that in our model, the velocity of the agent being constant and without holonomic component, this scaling is simply a constant factor, equal on both brain hemispheres. We note that a more realistic model with holonomic motion and offset optic flow preferences [13], PFN activity could differ in each hemisphere and the circuit might need some adjustment to function correctly. PFNs connect downstream to PFL via two pathways (Fig 3B). The direct pathway implements a one column shift in the projection pattern. The indirect pathway, formed by hΔ neurons, receives inputs from PFN and gives outputs to PFL with a projection pattern producing a 180° phase-shift [16–18]. In addition, hΔ integrate their input, i.e. the compass orientation inherited from PFNs, over time, supporting a homing vector [13]. As a result PFL receive inputs from the compass (Δ7) which is compared both with the current heading vector delayed by 1 simulation step (PFN) and the homing vector (hΔ). The composition of the complete PI circuit is represented in Fig 3A. The activity rate of the different neurons of the circuit is calculated by the following set of equations: (3)
(A) Circuit diagram incorporating the direct PFN − to − PFL and the indirect PFN − to − hΔ − to − PFL pathways. The activity rate of hΔ is updated continuously based on the PFN inputs to retain the PI memory. (B) Detailed connectivity diagram of the PI circuit, from the compass circuit output (Δ7) to the steering generator (Comparison of summed outputs of the PFL from both hemispheres). (C) Vectors encoded by the PI circuit over the path of the agent. The direct PFN − PFL pathway encodes a vector of constant length and with the immediate orientation of the agent. Note that, because we did not use a purely theoretical sinusoidal signal to represent the inner compass, the PFN population signal inherit some variability, in amplitude and shape, that can modify the length of this vector in a relative small magnitude. The indirect PFN − hΔ-PFL pathway encodes the integrated homing vector that points to the starting location of the path (nest/home). Clipart(s) in the figure have been modified from https://openclipart.org/.
With the translational self-motion inputs from the noduli, α a free parameter controlling the PI memory rate, , kPFN and khΔ parameters that control the relative influence of each pathway on the steering (kPFN = 1 − khΔ = 0.5 by default), ksteer a parameter that scales the steering command and ϵ a gaussian motor noise (σϵ = 10°). σPFN is the median activity of the whole array of PFNs, and is used to make the average contribution of PFN activity to the PI integration zero. That is, addition to the PI sum in the heading direction is always balanced by subtraction in the opposite direction. To support this symmetric positive-negative update of the PI, the initial activity rate of the hΔ is set to 0.5 and limited to the range [0 1]. The outcome of the whole circuit is the encoding of two vectors (Fig 3B), one instantaneous orientation vector from the PFN − PFL pathway, with a virtual length determined by the PFN activity, itself determined by the (constant in our model), and another continuously updated homing vector from the PFN − hΔ − PFL pathway.
Vector memory circuit.
The vector memory circuit is designed to store memory of either hΔ, carrying the PI homing vector, or PFN activity rate, carrying the immediate head direction vector. A reward signal induces the copying of one or both of these vectors into memory by modulating the synaptic weight of FBt inputs to the corresponding neuron type (hΔ or PFN). The memory can then be used later, under the control of an associated contextual signal that gates FBt activity.
The circuit. We implement a similar circuit to that proposed by Le Moël et al, 2019 [20] and generalise it to support visual navigation. The model circuit is composed of two neuron types, FBt and DANCX, so-called for its hypothetical dopaminergic neurotransmitter. Both have a similar projection pattern across the functional columns of the FB and interact in similar regions of the axons of both PFN and hΔ cells. FBt cells comprise two subtypes that form inhibitory synapses with either PFN (FBtPFN) or hΔ (FBthΔ). In contrast, a single DANCX neuron innervates both pathways simultaneously (Fig 4A) and triggers neuromodulation of the FBt − PFN and FBt − hΔ synapses (Fig 4A). Finally, each cell type receives different inputs, following the logic of their functionality. FBt receives input from contextual or state dependent signals of the agent, whereas DANCX receives input from a reward signal, also based on the agent’s current state. This circuit therefore modulates the connection from both PFN and hΔ to PFL, changing their update equations from Eq (3), to: (4) and represent the inputs to PFL cells from, respectively, PFN and hΔ. Constants are inherited from Eq (3). Sj represents the state j, which in the present model is limited to two different motivational states, exploration for food or returning to the nest.
(A) PI circuit with the addition of vector memory. The FBt pathways receive control from contextual/motivational signals based on the inner state (e.g. hunger). The DANs pathway receives inputs from reward signal(s) that define when a condition (dependent also on the context/task) is fulfilled and trigger a modulation of the FBt − PFN or FBt − hΔ synaptic strength at the level of PFN and/or hΔ axons to PFL. (B) Proposed mechanism for vector memory by synaptic modulation within the FBt − DAN − PFN/hΔ trios. Whenever a DAN is activated, any active FBt has its synaptic strength in every column modified proportionally to the activity rate of the corresponding PFN or the hΔ. The modulation of the synaptic weight could happen at the level of either the presynaptic partner (FBt), the postsynaptic level (PFN/hΔ), or both. An activity rate of PFN/hΔ greater than 0.5 (more active than inactive) induces an increase of the strength of the inhibitory FBt synapse; and an activity rate of PFN/hΔ lower than 0.5 (more inactive than active) induces a decrease of the synaptic strength. (C) Applied to all the functional columns, this mechanism stores a copy of the PFN and hΔ activity rates, at the time of reward via DANs, in the form of altered FBt synaptic strengths. The amplitude of the “copy” depends on the learning rate parameters, βPFN and βhΔ. Clipart(s) in the figure have been modified from https://openclipart.org/.
Synaptic plasticity memory. To generate a vector memory, and more generally a goal direction, associated with a specific context or motivation, the active DANs trigger the modulation of the synaptic weights between FBtPFN − PFN and FBthΔ-hΔ (Fig 4B). In each column, the modulation of the synaptic strength is proportional to the activity rate of the respective PFN or hΔ. Because the length of the vector encoded is dependent on the population sinusoid signal (Fig 2B), we use two gain parameters, respectively βPFN and βhΔ, to modulate the amplitude of the memory encoded at the level of the FBt synapses. These parameters can be set over a range of values to test their effect on the simulated behaviour. The modulation of the synaptic strength can be either positive, if the PFN/hΔ activity rate is greater than 0.5, or negative, if it is lower than 0.5. This results in a stable copy of the PFN/hΔ activity rate across the FB columns at the time of DANs activation (Fig 4C) that can be used to modify the PFN/hΔ inputs to the steering neurons PFL (Eq 4). To avoid any instability over time, the modulation of the memory at the synaptic level is made independent of the previous state of the synapses, i.e. the memory is flushed (the synapses are reset to their initial strength) and rewritten (the synaptic weight modulation is applied) every time the DAN is activated, as follow: (5) Where is the synaptic weight between each FBt and either PFN or hΔ and βPFN/hΔ is a free parameter representing the rate of the synaptic strength modulation. βPFN and βhΔ thus modulate the vector length kept in memory on each pathway. σPFN and σhΔ are the instant mean activity level of the population of respectively PFN and hΔ. Thus, the term that modulates the memory is centered around 0, positive for PFNs greater than the population mean activity and negative for PFNs lower. Consequently, the vector memory created is centered around its initial value, represented by the constant -0.5 (inhibitory) term in the equation. This implies that the creation of a new memory wipes any previous memory, i.e. the new FBt − PFN/hΔ synaptic weights are independent from the previous ones. Note that it also keeps the location memory (FBt − hΔ) consistent with the hΔ population activity (PI), centered on 0.5 in the model.
Simulations and results
To evaluate the model in different contexts, we defined different simulation paradigms in which we observed its navigation behaviour. The simulations are done in a custom made pyOpenGL environment (using the pyglet library). The virtual world used is composed of a blue sky, a brown ground and can either be empty or enriched by red or green objects such as vertical cylinders, cones and cubes depending on the paradigms (Fig A in S1 Text).
Setting the model: Replication of previous implementations
Navigation using PI and vector memory.
The first task we set consists in navigating from the nest to a known food source. It is a replication of the the work from Le Moël et al [20]. The main novelty of our implementation is the use of hΔ cells as the substrate for the PI. For this task we only focused on the vector memory of the PI and therefore omitted the direct connections from PFN to PFL in the model (Fig 5A.a). The simulations are composed of 3 parts, (1) the first outbound journey to find a food source and set the vector memory of this source, (2) the return to the nest based on the PI homing vector, and finally (3) a second outbound journey based on the interaction between the vector memory and the PI.
(A) Navigation using PI and vector memory, replicating [20]. a. The steering circuit in this simulation is only composed of the hΔ pathway, supporting path integration. When the agent is lacking food (food = 0) it promotes the exploration behaviour through the excitation of a specific FBt. When food is found, the DAN circuit triggers memory formation. The motivation then switches to the return state (food = 1) where the FBt is inhibited, leading to homing behaviour. b. Sequence of behaviour implemented: (1) the agent leaves the nest to explore (pre-determined zig-zag pattern). (2) When the agent reach 200l.u. from the nest, it is provided with a food reward, triggering the formation of the memory while simultaneously switching the motivation to the return mode. (3) Homing behaviour to the nest (catchment area of 20l.u. diameter) c. Reset of the motivation to the exploration mode (food=0). This time, the behaviour of the agent is left under the control of the steering circuit which should bring it back to the memorised location. See results in Fig 6. (B) Navigation using visual guidance and vector memory, replicating and extending [26]. a. For visual navigation without PI the steering circuit is only composed of the PFN to PFL pathway. The DAN reward circuit receives inputs from the sum of the target-detection ommatidia activity (see B.b), so the circuit forms a vector memory of its direction when facing the target. See results in Fig 7. b. Visual circuit used to control the recognition of a green object in the central visual field (Fig B in S1 Text). c. For visual navigation with PI, the circuit includes both PFN and hΔ pathways to the PFL. The agent thus forms a memory of both the direction of the visual target and the location from which it was seen, further improving its ability to locate the target. See results in Fig 8. Clipart(s) in the figure have been modified from https://openclipart.org/.
To simulate the first outbound journey (Fig 5A.b), we define a Z trajectory from the nest to the food source. The agent starts with a random direction and executes two random turns in opposite directions (of respectively 100 − 120° and 30 − 60°) at defined distances from the starting point (d = 80l.u. & d = 160l.u.), generating the zig-zag pattern. When the agent reaches 200l.u. from the nest, we then simulate an encounter with food. The first consequence is the activation of the DAN pathway. This induces the synaptic change at the FBt − hΔ level and forms a vector memory at the food location. The second consequence is the activation of the return pathway, leading to a complete inhibition of the FBt cells. This inhibition releases the hΔ pathway, which carries the PI, and thus generates the homing behaviour. Note that we could have used the FBt directly to generate an outbound journey by presetting a memory to a far away location, we decided to keep the zigzag pattern instead as this gave us the opportunity as well to check on the stability of the PI memory accumulation with non-linear trajectories.
The agent then returns to the nest and it is caught when it approaches within 20l.u. of the nest. Once the agent has reached the nest, the hΔs activity rate is homogeneously reset to its initial value (0.5), resetting the PI. The feeding state is also reset to 0, stimulating again the exploration FBt.
Finally, the second outbound journey is initiated. This time the inhibition by FBt of the hΔ inputs to PFL corresponds to the vector memory formed at the food location. The agent should thus be steered to the position where its PI (hΔ) activity and memory (FBt activity) cancel out, i.e. the food location. In order to evaluate whether a vector memory was created that included both the direction and the distance from the nest, the agent is not stopped when it reaches the 200l.u. limit, but rather left in exploration mode until the simulation reaches a fixed number of steps.
Fig 6A shows an example of the 2 first phases of the simulation (first outbound journey and return to the nest). The hΔ activity rate encodes correctly the home vector of the agent through the PI. At the location of the reward, the hΔ activity rate is efficiently copied in the synaptic weight from FBt to hΔ. Symmetric update of hΔ activity allows the activity to return near to its original level when it reaches the catchment area of the nest (20l.u.). Subsequently, the activated FBt leads the agent correctly toward the previously visited and rewarded location (Fig 6B). In addition, once it reaches the vicinity of the location, it clearly initiates a search behaviour from which we can calculate a search centre. To optimise the encoding of the vector memory, we varied the learning parameter βhΔ in different simulations (n = 200) and evaluate the distance between the search centre and the rewarded location to estimate the influence of βhΔ. The distance between the memorised location and the search pattern centre increases linearly when βhΔ is further from its perfect memory value (βhΔxIFBt ≈ 1).
(A) PI during the outbound journey (black path) and inbound journey (green path). The activity rate of the hΔ neurons is indicated for different location of both journeys; it encodes the home vector as a sine wave with amplitude corresponding to length and phase corresponding to angle. The agent is rewarded (finds food) when it reaches a set distance from the nest (200 l.u.) and triggers the creation of the synaptic long-term memory at the level of the FBt − to − hΔ axonal connections. (B) Retrieval outbound journey. The long-term FBt memory induces the steering circuit to drive the agent towards the location where the memory and PI cancel, i.e., the rewarded location. The distance between the search peak and target is used as a measure of precision in C. (C) Effect of varying the learning parameter βhΔ on the precision of retrieval journey: this modulates the vector length stored in memory. Each point represents one simulation with a fixed βhΔ randomly chosen in the [0 4] range. Note that because we modulated the motivational input to the FBt with a factor IFBt = 0.5, we corrected the value of βhΔ to verify that the best retrieval was achieve with a perfect memory (βhΔxIFBt ≈ 1).
Navigation using visual guidance.
The second task we investigated was whether the model could enhance guidance to a visual target. This replicates the task in [26]. The agent was placed in a 3D virtual world containing one target: a green vertical cylinder (Fig A in S1 Text). The key constraint on this task is that the visual system cannot estimate the target’s egocentric orientation directly to drive steering, but instead can only recognise when the target is in the frontal visual field. We use an insect-inspired eye model (Fig B in S1 Text) in which a subset of ommatidia oriented toward the front (azimuth ±10°) and above the horizon (elevation >0°), are set to be green-sensitive: any summed activity level above 0 is considered as an alignment with the target. Target detection is used as the input to the DAN reward pathway (Fig 5B). In this task, the desired vector memory is the current orientation of the agent, i.e., the direction it is facing when the target is seen. Hence, we only consider the PFN − PFL pathway and ignore the hΔ − PFL pathway (Fig 5B.a). Detection of the target triggers the creation of a vector memory at the level of the FBt to PFN axon synapses. The virtual length of this vector is determined arbitrarily by a learning rate parameter, βPFN.
Fig 7A shows 4 examples of paths using the visual guidance circuit. The model clearly succeeds in navigating in the direction of the target. However, in some cases when the target is missed the agent keeps heading in a constant direction, beyond the target (Fig 7A-top-right panel). The reward pathway, associated with the detection of the target in the frontal visual field, follows an intermittent pattern of activation, with a rate of activation between 0.1 to 0.2 (Fig 8B). The overall success rate to reach the target is relatively high (around 75%, Fig 7C) considering the simple target detection system. In addition, most of the unsuccessful simulations are observed with a low learning parameter βPFN (Fig 7D). It seems for any value of βPFN between 0.5 and 3 the success rate and time is similar (Fig 7D).
(A) Examples of trajectories from the nest to the target. (B) Left panel Example of reward input activity over time steps. Right panel Boxplot of the reward activity rate across all simulations. (C) Success rate and time to reach the target. Time is between first sighting the target and reaching it, normalised by the distance to the target at the first sighting. (D) Success to reach the target as a function of the learning parameter βPFN. (E) Time to reach the target as a function of the learning parameter βPFN. Clipart(s) in the figure have been modified from https://openclipart.org/.
Adding PI vector memory to visual guidance
In the previous simulations we separately examined the effects of PI and PI-vector memory (subserved by the hΔ circuit) and memory of the current heading (subserved by the PFN circuit). Here we examine the consequences of combining both circuits, with associated FBt memory, for the visual guidance task. A single DAN neuron triggers the synaptic modulation of both FBtΔ and FBtPFN synapses simultaneously (Fig 5D). Therefore, any detection of the target induces both the formation of a synaptic memory of the location (hΔ pathway) and of the heading direction (PFN pathway) at the last target sight. To ensure the location memory inherited from hΔ is reliable, we set the learning parameter βhΔ to its optimal value (βhΔxIFBt = 1.059, see Fig 6C), while βPFN is randomly assigned in the range [0.0 4.0] as previously.
Fig 8A shows 4 examples of simulated paths. The paths observed with this model do not strongly differ from the model without PI except, critically, it no longer proceeds in a constant direction beyond the target (Fig 7A). The reward activity rate, of similar magnitude ranging from 0.1 to 0.2, is slightly above the model without PI showing a better ability to observe the target (Fig 8B). However, the success rate climbs to 95% (Fig 8C), and the failed attempts only happened with a very low βPFN values (<0.5, Fig 8D). Failed attempt paths mostly show really high sinuosity but still a global movement toward the target (Fig 8A-top right panel), probably resulting from a too short vector memory formed at the FBtPFN level. Finally, the success rate and time is not drastically modified by the variations of the learning parameter βPFN, when it is above 0.5 (Fig 8E).
(A) Examples of trajectories from the nest to the target. (B) Left panel Example of a simulation reward input activity over time steps. Right panel Boxplot of the reward activity rate during the simulations (n = 200). (C) Success rate and time to reach the target. (D) Success to reach the target as a function of the learning parameter βPFN. Dots indicate individual simulation outcome. (E) Time to reach the target as a function of the learning parameter βPFN. (F) Conceptual comparison of the models with and without the hΔ/PI pathway for visual guidance. a. Without hΔ/PI pathway, the agent tries to correct its heading to be the same as the heading when the target was last sighted. b. With hΔ/PI pathway, the agent is steered towards the vector sum of the location where the target was last sighted, and the heading in which it was sighted, preventing overshoot. Clipart(s) in the figure have been modified from https://openclipart.org/.
To explain the improved performance after inclusion of PI memory in the model for visual guidance, we observe that the circuit effectively sums two vectors, such that the the visually-guided vector memory (FBtPFN) formed by the reward encounter is anchored at an allocentric reference point by the PI vector memory (FBthΔ) and this is maintained if there is no new observation of the target (Fig 8F). Thus, successive observations of the visual target (or of any cue characterising the goal) trigger the creation of spatial representations of successive aims/checkpoints around which the centre of the search pattern generated by the PI is moved. This generates more stable navigation that converges on the direct route between the last observation of the target and the target itself, whereas without the PI anchor, the visually-guided vector memory alone could quickly diverge without repeated observation of the target. Additionally, this stable spatial on-route anchor generated by the PI increases the chance to observe again the cue(s) associated with the goal.
Effect of target disappearance.
To verify the formation of a spatial representation of the aim by the PI and visual guidance integration, we ran simulations in which the target disappears when the agent gets within a certain distance of it(Fig 9A–9C). Therefore, the behaviour of the model in the absence of any new visual information to the CX (Fig 9C) will give us insight into the emergent spatial properties in the modeled CX circuit. We tested this scenario with both the model with visual guidance circuit alone (hΔ-PI pathway shutdown, Fig 9D–9G) and the model combining the PI memory (Fig 9H–9K).
(A-C) Simulation paradigm diagram. (A) The first phase corresponds to the previous simulation using visual-driven processing. (B) However, when the agent reach 50l.u. from the target, it disappears. (C) This agent is then left navigating without any new visual information modifying the FBt − PFN/hΔ guidance system in the CX. (D-G) Model without PI. (D) Example of a simulation path (black line). The location of the first sight of the target is indicated by the cyan dot and the last sight by a red dot. The target location is shown as a green plain circle and the origin location by a blue star. (E) Success rate (%) to reach the target location. (F) Success and failed attempts as a function of the βPFN learning parameter value. Success rate are calculated for every 0.25 section. (G) Mean distance to the target (blue dots) and the last sight (red dots) locations after the last sight event occurrence as a function of the βPFN learning parameter value. The lines show a 2nd degree polynomial fit for both location distances (blue for the target, red for the last sight). (H-K) Model with PI. Respectively identical to (D-G). Clipart(s) in the figure have been modified from https://openclipart.org/.
The visual guidance pathway alone (PFN − PFL) keeps track of the agent’s orientation at the last sight of of the target (Fig 9D). When it disappears, the agent is unable to stop. Combined with the motor noise in the simulation (σnoise = 10°), the probability to reach the target is decreased to around 40% (Fig 9E), compared with the baseline of around 75% (Fig 7C). The decrease of the success rate seems independent of the value of βPFN (Fig 9F).
In comparison, the addition of the PI vector memory to the model allows the agent to initiate search behaviour in the vicinity of the target even when it has disappeared (Fig 9H). It preserves the high overall success rate (Fig 9I), over a wide range of values of βPFN(Fig 9J). Finally, when comparing the mean distance to the target/last sight, after this last sight, we observe a strong dependence to the βPFN value. That is due to the behaviour converging to the end of the summed FBthΔ vector and FBtPFN vector (Fig 8F.b), whose length depends upon βPFN. These results support the creation of a spatial representation of the navigation goal in the CX based on the allocentric positioning system generated by the addition of PI.
Using a mushroom body model of visual long-term memory to navigate
In addition to navigating towards a conspicuous beacon, insects can also orient themselves based on visual memory [66, 67], aligning themselves in a particular direction with respect to surrounding landmarks or the panorama. This ability, which has been particularly associated with following familiar routes under conditions when PI information is removed, has been linked to the MB [68, 69]. The MB network, known for olfactory discrimination, can be used to discriminate on-route memorised views from off-route views [70]. However, as implemented, the MB model appear to encode valence of the visual scenery (familiar vs unfamiliar), and spatial information is encoded only implicitly in the use of retinotopic memories, such that familiarity is highest when aligned in the same direction as when the memory was stored. The quick degradation of valence with translation and rotation around a learned position [71] makes it challenging to derive robust navigation from MB output alone. If motor noise moves an agent just slightly off the route, the MB provides no useful information and it is lost. However, MB output neurons (MBONs) have been identified as strong partners for FBt neurons projecting into the CX in Drosophila [60], for example in the control of olfactory wind-guided behaviour [18]. We proposed previously a theoretical layout using the CX steering circuit to integrate the MB output, discriminate learned views oriented toward a feeder, and generate a robust navigation to this feeder [26] which is supported by the impact of CX lesions on learned visual navigation in ants [72].
Because the model we implemented in the current paper works very well with intermittent observation of the target (Fig 9), we tested its potential to integrate MB valence output. We implemented a similar MB circuit (Fig 10A) to the one we previously modeled [26]. Images from a simulated world cluttered with geometric objects are passed through an insect eye model, and lateral inhibition between blue channel ommatidia used to detect a skyline. This activates 1000 visual projection neurons (vPNs) which form random connections with the Kenyon Cells (KC, nKC = 20000) with a rate of 2–5 vPNs per KC. A sparse, binary activation pattern in the KCs is produced using a moving threshold to maintain the number of active KCs to 200 (1%), mimicking the inhibitory feedback regulating the KC activity found in numerous species [73]. Each of these KCs connects to a single MBON with an initial synaptic weight of 1. Finally, a single DAN neuron controls the learning process by stimulating neuromodulation at the level of every KC − MBON synapse.
(A) Mushroom body model. Visual input is based on the blue channel of an insect-inspired eye model (see Fig B in S1 Text), with lateral inhibition to encode edges in the layer of visual projection neurons (vPN) that provide input to the MB. (B) Left panel Diagram of the initiation of the MB “snapshot” memory following a straight route. Right panel Boxplot of the MB model performance index for the 2 versions of the model (see details in Fig H in S1 Text). (C) Simulation without PI. Left panel Path of 15 route following attempts for 1 single route learned. The red arrow indicates the learned route. Right panel Heatmap of 8 pooled simulations, realigned on the learned route direction (red arrow). (D) Simulation with PI, similar to (C) panels. (E) Boxplots of, from left to right, the average distance to the route (on the whole path and on the path detected on the route, see Fig G in S1 Text), the percentage of route traveled and the minimal distance to the end route. All the data have been filtered based on the MB performance index (B). (F) Success rate measured at different portions of the route for the model with (blue stars) or without (red triangles) PI. Individual simulations (1 single route learning and 15 route following attempts) are indicated in thin lines while pooled data are indicated in thick lines. Clipart(s) in the figure have been modified from https://openclipart.org/.
The MB model is trained on a straight route (starting at the origin [0,0], randomly oriented and of 100l.u. length). Memories are stored at regularly spaced locations (15 locations; every 6.7l.u.) by activating the DANMB which casuses the decrease to 0 of the KC − MBON synaptic weight of any currently active KC. After this training process, the agent is replaced near the origin of the learned route to observe its ability to detect and follow it. The model generates an MBON activity rate inversely proportional to the visual familiarity. This rate is normalised, using the fact that the active proportion of KCs is maintained constant, and binarized using an arbitrary threshold (TMBON = 0.01). To evaluate the performance of the MB model itself, we estimated a MB performance index, based on the alignment between the orientation of the agent when MBON was inactive (recognising on-route views) and the orientation at the nearest location on the route, and determined a threshold score above which the MB is considered to work better than random (i.e. appears to be more inactive when near the correct orientation) based on our data (Fig H in S1 Text). Note that we eliminated simulations with a score lower than this threshold as well as simulations where the MBON are never inactivated (meaning no views were recognised as on-route) from route following analysis.
The binarized MBON output produces a switching signal which can be used by the existing CX circuit. The MBON input to the CX DAN is set to be inhibitory, only releasing its activity when an on-route view is detected (reward). The CX model remains identical to the previous version, generating memories in FBt whenever the CX DAN is activated. As before we compare the model with the PFN pathway only (memory of direction) to a model with a combined memory of direction from the PFN pathway and PI location from the hΔ pathway.
The MB model performance index shows the route views are being detected with a score above the random threshold (see Fig H in S1 Text for the definition of this threshold) in both versions of the model. However, the MB model index ranges from -1 to 1 in simulation with the model without PI, while it is tightly constrained close to 1 in simulations with the model with PI. The integration of the MB model in the CX without PI (using the PFN pathway only) managed to globally orient in the region of the route orientation but failed to achieve properly the following of the learned straight route all the way to the end (Fig 10C). The combined heatmaps of 6 simulations (2D histogram corrected by the orientation of each learned route) only shows a weak tendency to converge to the beginning only of the learned route (Fig 10C, Right panel). In comparison, the addition of the PI (hΔ pathway) and the associated vector memory circuit successfully generated navigation behaviour converging on the learned route. Moreover, the combined heatmap of all simulations indicates a strong peak around the end of the learned route (Fig 10D). The PI model, compared with the model without PI, shows better performance in the average distance to the route, in the extent of the route followed (see Fig G in S1 Text), and the minimal distance to the end of the route (Fig 10E). Note that the PI model’s average distance to the route is kept low mostly by its ability to restrain the navigation in the vicinity of the route after losing it, whereas the model without PI diverges quickly when it loses the route (Fig 10E). Finally, the performance of route following decreases quicker in the model without PI along the route (Fig 10F). These results further demonstrate how the spatial representation of the navigation goal is enabled by the addition of PI to the sensory guidance circuit.
In addition, we tested the MB-guidance on more complex routes. We generated zig-zag routes similar to the first outbound journey in the vector-memory paradigm. While progressing along this route, the MB long-term memory was created continuously, during each step of the model (0.25lu.st−1), by maintaining DANMB active with a learning rate readjusted to 0.1. After the entire route has been learned, the agent was replaced around the starting location with a random orientation and the route following was tested. For each route learned, 15 tests were conducted, each with a different starting location and orientation.
Fig 11A shows the example of a full route following simulation, 1 route learning and 15 route following attempts, and 6 examples of individual route following attempts for the same route learned. Without additional components, the model shows capability in tortuous routes following. With both model versions, the MB performance index lies mostly under the random threshold, indicating it is a more difficult task to recognize views of combined route segments in different orientations (Fig 11C). Note that our method to define this index and the estimation of a nearest route location might add some discrepancies in the vicinity of the zigzag turns, where orientations vary quickly from one segment to another. Despite, while both models show an equivalent ability to approach the end of the route, the model with PI clearly shows an improvement in term of the route following and the average distance to the route (Fig 11D). The performance of both model is however strongly limited by the performance of the MB itself (Fig 11D). The addition of PI clearly increases the capability of the CX model to sustain route following over more complex routes (Fig 11E). Particularly, the performance of the model to follow the route in the vicinity of turns (20% and 60% of the route) diminishes linearly with the PI whereas on average, without PI the success rate drops more rapidly after each turn.
(A) Left panel Simulation example of a single route learning paradigm (red zigzag arrow) and route following trials (black lines, n = 15). The MB learning, only during the walk (with constant speed) along this route, is continuous and modulated by a learning rate of 0.1. All route following attempts are initiated at a random location situated around the starting point of the route (±20l.u.). Right panel Individual trial of route following. The route is shown as a black line and the route following attempt in blue. The red dots indicate the position at which the route has been considered lost (see Fig G in S1 Text for calculation details). (B) Estimation of the MB model performance scores for 15 attempts to follow a single learned route Left panel Probability density function of the MBON reward inactivity (on-route views) in relation to the orientation relative to the nearest route orientation. Right panel Boxplot of the MB evaluation scores (see Fig H in S1 Text for calculation details). (C) Left panel Boxplots of the MB performance score for the two different versions of the model (no PI: red, PI: blue) before (outside large boxplots) and after (inside small boxplots) the elimination of low MB performance score (see Fig H in S1 Text). Right panel Boxplot of the average distance to the route with both versions of the model calculated on the whole path or only before the route was considered lost (see Fig G in S1 Text). (D) From top to bottom, boxplots (red: without PI, blue: with PI) of the minimum distance from the route end, the percentage of route traveled and the average distance to the route in relationship with the MB performance score. Each boxplot is based on the pooled data of simulations with MB performance index value within a 0.1 range (grey and white bands). (E) Success rate of the model to reach different portion of the route. Data have been selected based on the MB performance score threshold. Clipart(s) in the figure have been modified from https://openclipart.org/.
Discussion
We propose an integrative model that follows the functional neuroanatomy of key identified neurons of the insect CX [12, 60], from the head direction to the steering circuits. Our main guideline to build the CX model was to follow closely the known projectomic/connectomic data in insects [36, 60]. However, we focused on the common patterns of projection and their functional aspects [12], rather than on the fine and species-dependent details. From this approach we aim to extract generic insight into CX processes that could guide our understanding of its function(s) as well as its notable conservation across insect evolution.
Specifically, we suggest that identified neurons (FBt) that convey signals from outside the CX and project across the FB serve the purpose (previously hypothesised without explicit neural grounding in [20]), of storing vector memories. This is merged with previous models that suggest integrative processes in intrinsic FB neurons (PFN and/or hΔ) can support path integration and homing to a nest [13] and persistence in approach to targets that provide a sensory-based,innate or learned, reward [26]. The connectivity between these components enables flexible switching between navigational modes. We show the model can support a range of behaviours including: revisiting a food source location; maintained navigation towards a visual target that disappears from view; and route following based on a familiarity signal from the mushroom bodies. We argue that this circuit function of creating allocentric spatial goals (not just heading goals) for immediate navigational control is relevant to a wide range of insect species and behaviours.
To our knowledge, no direct behavioural evidence exists as yet to support such a spatial representation in insects. However, we believe the development of experimental paradigms within a cluttered environment, forcing constant obstacle avoidance, and/or controlled disappearance of the sensory cues, mimicking our implementation (Fig 9), could provide insight to the actual existence of spatial representation of navigation goals. That is, we predict that an insect, after contouring an obstacle [74, 75], should set a course that reflects its last estimate of the goal position, or a point on route to the goal, not just the goal direction; in a similar manner to how ants have been observed, after deflection, to set a direct course to the nest [76] or to a known feeder [66].
Spatial vs. heading goals
The key insight from this integrative model is the emergence of an allocentric navigation vector—indicating a specific spatial location as the current goal to move towards—from the combination of both the directional (FBtPFN) and positional (FBthΔ) memory circuits. To date, the CX circuit has been most widely interpreted as a system that allows the insect to maintain a desired heading, supporting behaviours such as persistence in a direction when cues disappear [77], menotaxis, i.e. the ability to maintain a constant bearing in regard to a particular cue [27], or simply to walk straight [28]. More generally, in the navigation context, the CX has been highlighted as as neural circuit that compares the heading of the insect with its desired heading or goal direction [13, 26, 34, 78]. This has been supported by recent neuroimaging and behavioural data, demonstrating the mechanism that allows insects to steer based on the difference of headings (actual and desired) [31, 52, 79]. We have previously proposed that if the goal direction is determined by a ‘home vector’, obtained through path integration, then such a steering system can guide the insect to a particular location in space, specifically, the place where that vector is zero in length [13]. As such, the assumed primary function is to return to a point of origin such as a nest [15], but by introducing an opposing ‘vector memory’ the ‘zero point’ can be positioned at an alternative location relative to the nest, such as a recently experienced food source [20, 80]. Importantly, this system also produces an emergent search pattern around the target location without any additional mechanism or explicit switch to search behaviour required.
Here we suggest that PI and vector memory are not just specialised functions for central place foraging, but rather play a more general role, intrinsic to how the CX supports all spatial behaviour. The high degree of conservation in CX structure across insect species and the existence of PI in species such as Drosophila [22, 23] support this extension. For example, a simple modification of the circuit—abolition of the return inhibitory pathway—sustains navigation and exploitation of several food sources in a ‘nomadic’ fashion (Fig 12), reminiscent of Drosophila exploratory behaviour [81]. Essentially, we have shown that PI can provide an anchor, i.e. a stable reference/origin point in space, for any sensory-guidance pathway, extending its function from self-positioning to goal-positioning (we note that [82] similarly suggested that the fly may compare its vectorially encoded position to a vectorial goal, in 2D or 3D space, although not presenting a specific mechanism). In the example behaviours we have simulated, this ‘goal’ does not correspond to the exact location of the ultimate goal (such as the green cylinder target or the end of the learned route). Rather, the location temporarily imprinted in the memory is a way-point on route to this ultimate goal. This constrains movement to stay close to this route, by integration of deviation from the desired way-point, facilitating the (re-)observation of either the goal or any cue associated to it. Therefore, the behaviour innately converges, thanks to the innate PI central displacement, to a location that get closer and closer to the targeted location, maintaining a robust balance between exploration and sensory-guidance. Animals in the wild are confronted with cluttered environments and must deal with sparse sensory inputs and/or detours [75, 83]. Being able to keep a trace of a location (where you were headed when the goal was last seen) rather than just a direction (which compass direction you were headed when the goal was last seen) must therefore represent a significant advantage. For example, as we have shown, it can significantly improve a route following by an agent using visual familiarity learned in the MB [70] which otherwise can get quickly lost when small displacements or rotations make all views appear unfamiliar [71], and cannot recover the route using the heading memory alone. Other recent proposals to link the MB visual long-term memory to CX steering have used the bilateral MB to learn views corresponding to left and right turns in a visual homing task [78]. This extension would be compatible with our implementation and could further improve the ability of the model to follow tightly a route.
(A) Settler brain The CX model used in this simulation is similar to Fig 5B.c. Both sensory stimulations from the green visual pathway and food discovery are used as reward to the FBt − DAN circuit. The agent is placed at the nest and left to find food located at different location randomly assigned in a quadrant of 90° and at a distance of 100–400 l.u.. When a food source is found, it is fully consumed and disappears. Simultaneously, a memory is formed on the FBthΔ and the motivation is switched to trigger homing behaviour. When the agent reaches the nest, the motivation is switched again to promote further exploration. The simulation is stopped when the agent spends more than 5000 t.u. without finding a new food source or the nest and is considered lost. (B) Nomad brain The CX model used in this simulation is modified to replace any DAN stimulation by a re-zeroing of the hΔ activity, i.e. of the PI. The rest of the simulation is similar to (A). Clipart(s) in the figure have been modified from https://openclipart.org/.
The association of visual memories and PI, either to follow a route or to achieve homing, has been previously modelled as a problem of reconciling independent pathways [34], each one carrying a goal direction to follow. This approach is influenced by the description of insect navigation as a toolkit containing parallel functions [32] that should be optimally combined at output [7, 35]. By contrast we propose here a more integrated architecture, where different pathways contribute with complementary properties. Therefore, the outcome of the CX processing, illustrated here by the visual-driven and the route following behaviour, benefits simultaneously from the directional inputs of the compass, from the positional inputs of the PI and from the visual memory of the MB to achieve a more robust navigation than previous implementations [26, 70]. In addition to improving visual memory navigation, the same principles could also benefit other pathways connected to numerous FBts [60]. Note, however, that this integrated circuit does not explicitly associate a PI (location) to specific visual memories, as would be required to form a cognitive map [7]. Here, the spatial memory is created locally and only serves to navigate using the immediate sensory inputs. However, the storage of locations in the CX based on different sensory inputs could help explain cognitive-like ability observed in insect navigation behaviour. Additionally, the transformation of sensory/memory inputs to the CX from egocentric directional to allocentric positional representations could be crucial to unify modalities often presenting different spatio-temporal dynamics, allowing insects to more robustly navigate in their environment.
Comparison to alternative models
A number of other CX models have been proposed that utilise essentially the same neuron classes and connectivity as our model, but with somewhat different functional interpretations. Here we summarise the key similarities and differences, and relevant experimental evidence supporting each.
PFN—functional compass copy.
PFNs, in our implementation, serve to create a functional copy of the compass bump carried by Δ7, with their projection pattern producing a bilateral 45° shift of the bump to the left or right. In principle, the inhibitory Δ7 synapses are modulating, across the FB, an activation rate of the PFNs which is determined by the agent’s translational motion. In the current simulations, the speed is always set to a constant value, and we neglect possible holonomic motion, hence the baseline activation is always equal in left and right PFNs, and the activation pattern is simply the inverse of the Δ7 pattern, corresponding to a sinusoid-like function with a phase centered on the EPG bump of activity. In a previous model [13] the left and right PFNs are activated from neurons in the noduli tuned to optic flow at ±45° forming an orthogonal basis that allows egocentric translational inputs to be transformed to an allocentric estimate of motion that is accumulated to encode PI memory. More recent models, supported by neurophysiological measurements in flies, suggest that different subtypes, PFNv and PFNd, between them form a full orthogonal basis for the translational motion of the insect in allocentric coordinates, which is integrated by hΔB cells into an allocentric vector of motion [16, 17], see also [60]. These studies do not find evidence for accumulation of the translational input in PFNv and PFNd; although the existence, in bumblebees, of a pathway in the cap region of the noduli targeting specific PFNc neurons [36] that are not present in flies, leaves the possibility open that PFN could support PI in some insect species. In [84], PFNa are shown to be sensitive to wind from ±45°, and in the model presented in [18] this is assumed to support an analogous allocentric estimate of the wind direction in hΔA cells, which can contribute to upwind steering (as described further below). In brief, the interpretation of PFN neurons as not just copying and shifting the compass signal, but as transforming egocentric self-motion into an allocentric vector remains consistent with our model’s assumption that this vector provides a (temporary) goal (“maintain recent heading”). However we note that introducing holonomic motion in our model would require a compensatory mechanism (similar to that included in the model in [13]) to deal with the right-left imbalance in PFN activation this would produce, which would otherwise bias the memory processes (PI and vector memory) and the steering control.
hΔ—180° phase-shift and integration.
hΔ are essential to our model. They allow a 180° rotation of the compass and, by integrating the PFN inputs over time, form the PI homing vector, which then becomes a potential goal for the steering circuit. Previously, in [13], where it was assumed that PFN accumulated the PI vector, the analog of hΔ in bees (the pontine neurons) were only used to normalise the bilateral signal from PFN reaching the analog of PFL (CPU1). As indicated above, recent models in Drosophila suggest that by unifying the PFNv and PFNd in the same reference frame, hΔb allow the transformation of the heading orientation, acquired from external cues (compass), into an allocentric travelled orientation, based on the forward-backward and the 45° shifted optic-flow velocity estimation on both hemisphere [16, 17, 60]. This property remains consistent with our model, as this is indeed the velocity vector that should be accumulated for PI. Interestingly, [60] suggest that recurrent connections between hΔ pairs might support PI, but as yet there is no direct evidence for this. We note that using hΔ instead of PFN for PI allows the model to represent simultaneously the “recent heading” and “home” directions, whereas using PFN as the substrate (as in [13]) only allows “home” and “anti-home” directions. In the model proposed in [18], where hΔc are assumed to represent allocentric wind direction, an additional functional role of these cells is to integrate contextual control signals for the presence of odour, in which FBt neurons gate the hΔc output to PFL to drive up-wind steering.
FBt—Dynamic goal direction.
The key novel component of our implementation to generate a goal direction signal, and the most speculative, is the tandem FBt − DAN. While based on the neuroanatomy of the FBt in Drosophila [60], the exact connectivity of these cells remains speculative. The addition of the FBt − DANs updates our previous model, in which such tangential FB inputs only conveyed a reward signal from innate or learned visual pathways [26]. This signal modulated integration in the FB to create persistence in rewarded heading directions and it provided an anatomically grounding for the theoretical implementation of vector memory in [20]. Note here that in addition to axonal connections, used in our model to stably maintain the vector memory, FBts present projection to the dendritic region of hΔ subtypes [60] that are not considered in our model, due to the single linear unit neuron model. It remains to show what function(s) they could serve but we can speculate that dendritic projection would interfere with the inner computation of the neuron, and particularly the PI in the case of hΔ, as we hypothesise here. Therefore, introducing them in the model could be useful to set/reset the PI at a specific state (based on sensory/memory information, zeroing at the feeder in drosophila for example [25]) or eventually gating it (when the orientation/odometry become uncertain for example). In the current model we extend the function of the vector memory circuit to form a goal direction signal. Similarly, the neuromodulation of synaptic weights to store the goal heading in the FB has been also proposed to sustain a contextual control of saccade-fixation Drosophila behaviours in a negative reinforcement paradigm [85]. In all cases, the resulting goal direction is represented by a sinusoidal pattern in the FB, which allows its effective comparison with the equivalently shaped compass signal in the PB [12, 52]. Indeed, a goal representation in line with these theoretical predictions has been experimentally verified in Drosophila [31]. In those experiments the animal’s goal direction could be optogenetically controlled by the activation of FC2 neurons, columnar neurons with mixed input and output fibers in individual columns of the FB [31]. This concept of a goal direction population code differs from our implementation of a goal direction encoded in FBt output synaptic weights. However, it is still unknown if the sinusoidal FC2 activity pattern is generated within these cells or inherited from upstream neurons. As the intra-columnar projections of FC2 cells suggest local information flow within columns of the FB, it is conceivable that the goal representation does not originate in FC2 cells, but that these cells could carry the associated information from where it is stored to where it is used; i.e., in our implementation, from FBt output synapses to the location where PFN and hΔ neurons interface with PFL cells.
To date, the only other models to include FBt neurons address olfactory navigation behaviour in Drosophila [18, 86]. The existence of both olfactory and visual pathway in parallel influencing the CX navigation is supported by multiple FBts input to the FB in insect [60], potentially representing different sensory-based vectors, supporting the multimodal control of navigation [87]. The main projection pattern of FBt they propose is similar to ours and their upstream input—non-directional odour perception/recognition, potentially from the LH (innate) and the MB (learned)—can be interpreted as a reward input, i.e. matching the dopaminergic FBts (DAN) in our model. They propose that FBt gates the columnar neuron (specifically hΔc) outputs to generate different goal directions based on the olfactory context, resulting in upwind orientation when exposed to attractive odours. While similar in principle, this model differs in two ways from our new model. Firstly, it does not use PI, and second, there is no dynamical modulation of the goal direction. The latter means that the this model is limited to the two predefined alternatives of upwind and downwind flight direction. Although this binary choice is sufficient for wind guided olfactory navigation when locating the source of an odor plume [88, 89], recent observations of continuous adjustment of goal direction signals in monarch butterflies after negative conditioning [79] support the capacity of the CX to perform dynamic readjustment of the goal direction based on sensory experiences—consistent with our implementation. Interestingly, in the event of a loss of the odour plume, insects show a stereotypical casting behaviour, sweeping from right to left in quick alternations to recover the odor plume [90]. We suggest this could correspond to the searching behaviour generated in our model, which is induced by the PI at the location indicated by the combination of the most recent vector memories (homing and sensory vectors). In the context of odour plume following this should correspond to the last place estimated on the route to the plume source, thus increasing the chance that the odour molecule can be detected again.
In summary, tangential inputs to the FB (FBt neurons) represent an efficient means to map non-direction inputs onto the directional system formed by columnar neurons [18, 26]. Our model follows this principle and additionally introduces positional inputs that strengthen the sensory-guided navigation by complementing it with the path integration at the same level. The synaptic modulation to represent dynamical changes in the goal direction memory is speculative, although consistent with the adaptability [79] and the persistence [77] of insect navigation behaviour. Additionally, such synaptic modulation is supported by the evidence for dopaminergic circuits in the FB [60], and the well-known function of dopamine in plasticity and learning, for example in the MB memory [91] or in the ER neurons plasticity [47]. Therefore we think our model provides a reasonable prediction of FBt function and could inform future experiments on the interplay between FBts and the FB pool of columnar neurons (PFN, hΔ [18] or FC [31]. However we note that the mechanism that sets the synaptic weights in the model is very abstracted, and lacks any obvious foundation in biophysical mechanisms of plasticity; this should be a focus for future work.
PFL—CX steering output.
The unilateral signals carried by the different component of the model (Δ7, PFN and hΔ) are then summed and compared, bilaterally, at the level of the PFL which compute the steering output of the CX model, as in previous models [13, 20]. Their role is supported by evidence of a strong downstream connectivity of PFL3 to descending neurons (DNa02) responsible for turning behaviour [51, 60], and has been recently verified by functional neuroimaging and modeling [31, 52]. However, the connectivity pattern we used, presenting a strict separation between hemispheres and offset by exactly one column, does not follow the precise details of the projection of either PFL1 and PFL3 reported in Drosophila [60], which have (respectively) a one (PFL1) or two (PFL3) column offset pattern that continues across the midline. All these patterns can reproduce the key function of creating a left-right difference in the PFL that reflects the difference between the compass and goal directions (Fig F in S1 Text), but incorporating the real connectivity in the future might allow a more subtle control of the steering. While it is generally accepted that relative PFL3 activity affects turning direction in the animal, different models have used this signal directly to set angular velocity (our model, [13, 18, 34, 52]), or to influence the relative probability of turning in each direction [85], or as input to an intrinsic oscillator based on the LAL circuit [92, 93]. The role of PFL1 could be simply to refine the PFL3 signal, but has also been speculated to instead contribute to head motion, or to controlling directional change through side-slip instead of body rotation [60]. Our model used a constant forward speed and so we did not include PFL2 neurons, which have a four column offset and thus combine information from both sides of the FB; in other models they are assumed to modulate forward speed or fixation duration in Drosophila. PFL2 activity has also been suggested to increase the angular velocity when facing the anti-goal [52], as the difference in PFL3 activity has a minimum at this point. However, we note that facing the anti-goal (unlike facing the goal) is an unstable state in the CX steering system. Hence the motor noise of (σϵ = 10°) used in our agent simulation (or alternatively, intrinsic oscillatory behaviour [94]) can turn the agent away from the exact anti-goal direction sufficiently that differential PFL3 activity will then continue to turn it back towards the goal direction.
Potential for temporal integration to improve the goal estimate
One limitation in the current implementation is the absence of any dynamics in the vector memory (goal) formation at the level of the FBt output synapses. The navigation is therefore based on a vector of arbitrary length (depending on βPFN) and completely rewritten with every reward event. A more realistic neuromodulation dynamic could induce a vector formation process that averages over events, and therefore space, to refine the positional goal gradually as more information is acquired. We showed in a previous implementation of the MB to CX pathway that the integration of the MB output signal over time could be used in this way to strengthen the navigation toward a learned feeder [26]. Additionally, observations in Drosophila are consistent with the assumption that they continuously adjust the zero-point of their PI to the statistical center of a optogenetically rewarded area [25]. Adding such a mechanism to the model in this paper could support a form of triangulation, relying on the positioning ability of the PI, that continuously improves the estimation of the navigation goal location every time an attractive/rewarding sensory combination is experienced.
Contextual control of navigation in insects
The recent deciphering of the FB circuit and its implementation in functional models [18, 31, 52] has strengthened the hypothesis that its key role is the comparison of the goal and current heading directions. The existence of a wide variety of converging sensory streams through a pool of specific FBts [60], should thus support multisensory integration, matching behavioural evidence of multisensory navigation in insects [40, 87]. Moreover, the mechanism we have highlighted, inducing a heading vector memory under the control of a contextual reward, is perfectly suited to allow the selection of action based on several vectors, either learned or based on immediate sensory inputs, in line with contextual gating in wind-guided behaviour in Drosophila [18]. Therefore, in addition to the integration of different goal orientations together [19], the FB could be involved in a dynamical selection of the different modality(ies) depending on the immediate information/context available. More specifically, our model’s ability to not only inherit a goal orientation from external sensory streams but rather to rebuild it plastically based on the inner compass orientation and on contextual valence signals, supports several sensory combination mechanisms, such as association, gating and/or summation [95]. The layering of the pool of FBts existing in insects [60] is particularly adapted to this selective activation/inhibition as well as the dynamical construction of the different sensory-based navigation vectors. Consequently, the FB appears as a structure combining a set of vectors, sensory- and/or memory-based, expressed in a common reference frame to achieve a context-dependant navigation task.
Analogy to vertebrate navigation
As discussed above, our model suggests that directional information from egocentric sensory cues and memory is transferred into an allocentric spatial framework. Importantly, this framework does not require the formation of a cognitive map to support successful navigation. Whereas the existence of a cognitive map in vertebrates has been supported by the discovery of a variety of spatially tuned cells [1, 2], an equivalent construct in insects remains questioned [5, 6]. So far, cellular evidence comparable to that in the vertebrate hippocampus and entorhinal cortex is absent in insects. However, a map-like representation that could be supported by the insect brain’s more moderate coding capacity might rely on an entirely different set of cell types. In principle, a map-like representation would imply that memories of sensory experiences in particular locations are associated to the geometric coordinates underpinned by PI [33, 96, 97]. This ability would allow an animal to recover PI vectors based on sensory and memory inputs, as well as to predict the sensory inputs based on the PI state [7]. Clearly, the model circuit we explored in this study is not sufficient to support a cognitive map representation. While it generates a long-term memory of a location based on the PI vector [20], it can neither recall these vectors based on memorized sensory information nor recall sensory memories (MB stored “views”) based on the PI state. Nevertheless, the proposed FBt vector memories in our model are based on both PI (FBt − hΔ) and innate and/or memorized sensory cues (FBt − PFN). They therefore could constitute a key circuit element to achieve bidirectional cross-talk between PI vector memories and memorized sensory cues. A circuit supporting a cognitive map should also involve neurons projecting from the CX back to upstream sensory and memory regions, for example FS neurons, columnar outputs from the FB identified in Drosophila [60], which project to several brain areas including the MB [98]. This prediction provides a unique starting point towards interrogating the feasibility of a cognitive map in the insect brain.
Supporting information
S1 Text. Supporting methods and figures.
Fig A. Virtual worlds used in simulations. Concentric circles on the ground do not appear during the simulations and are displayed here to show distance, 100l.u. separate consecutive circle radius. Vector memory replication of Le Moël et al [20] have been conducted in the empty environment. The sensory attraction to the innate green cylinder have been conducted in the single landmark. The MB route following simulation, both with a straight or zigzag route, have been conducted in both the enriched environment and the cluttered environment, without green landmarks in it. Finally, multiple source exploration simulations have been conducted in the cluttered environment, with randomly positioned green landmarks as food sources. Fig B. Eye model visual processing. We built an eye model with the aims to (A) reduce the resolution inherited from the raw simulation images and (B) represent the heterogeneity observed generally in insects eyes [99–101], i.e. the frontal and horizon part presenting often a higher resolution than the rest of the eye. (C) Each ommatidia is then assigned the set of pixel corresponding to its projection on the pyOpenGL rendering planes (4 orthogonally organized planes forming the panoramic view with a 160° vertical span. The activity rate of an ommatidia is calculated by the averaged light-level of all its assigned pixels. (D) The two color channels visible by insects (Green & Blue) are separated and can be used for different pathways. The Green channel is defined to create the frontal (above the horizon) visual field detection of green landmark (innate). Alternatively, the blue channel is used to input into the MB through the vPNs (learned). Fig C. CX model connectivity matrix. (A) Generic connectivity pattern used between individual neuron group. The ID of each neuron of a single type is based on the exitence of the functional columns observed in several CX sub-structure. (B) Overall connectivity matrix representing the whole CX model. Fig D. Compass circuit. The compass circuit consists of a ring attractor distributed between the EB and the PB that has been highlighted in recent neurophysiological studies in insects [9, 102, 103]. (A) Compass circuit diagram. The circuit is represented in a circular fashion to shed light on the columnar organization across EB and PB. The inter-neuron connectivity pattern is only shown for one functional column and repeated identically for every other. Note the intrinsic connectivity pattern across the PB of Δ7 (B) Compass orientation input to the EPG (EB). The orientation of the agent is compared with the preferred directions (with a 45° acceptance angle) of 8 orientation sensitive cells (Compass Neurons, CN), which could therefore correspond to mimic a sky polarization pathway. The cell that is sensitive to the current orientation is set with an activity rate of 1 while the others activity is set at 0. Each of this CN synapse to both EPGs of each wedge, one for each hemisphere, in the EB. (C) Compass rotational input to the PEN (PB). Left/Right turns alternatively excite PEN on one hemisphere of the PB allowing the rotation of the compass according to the movement of the insect. (D) Compass reformatting from the sensory input to the Δ7 layer and distribution to the PFN layer. Through the different layer of the compass circuit and particularly due to the Δ7 projection pattern, the single activity ‘bump’ inherited from the CN is transformed into a sinusoidal shape signal inherited by the PFNs. Fig E. Model with sinusoid compass inputs (A) Head direction signal process from the Compass Units, following a winner take all activity rule, to the PFNs. (B) Head direction signal process from the Compass Units, following a sinusoidal activity rule, to the PFNs. The activity of each units is calculated as the sinusoid of the difference between the orientation of the agent and of each compass unit preferred direction. is adjusted to 0.4 to deal with the increase in overall activity across EPGs. (C) Vector memory paradigm (Fig 4A) using the sinusoidal compass input. Fig F. Model with a realistic Δ7 − to − PFL3 connectivity pattern [31, 52]. (A) Default connectivity used in our model. No shift is applied from Δ7 to PFL as we used previously in [26]. (B) Connectivity pattern following a 2 columns shift from Δ7 to PFL3 as observed in Drosophila [31, 52, 60]. Note that connectivity pattern are presented without the additional 4 columns shift inherited from the Δ7 projection pattern across the PB for simplicity. Fig G. Route following detection routine. (A) Checkpoints are homogeneously distributed along the learned route. (B) For each route following trial, we calculated a minimal distance to the checkpoints, each line indicate a route following attempt (n = 15). Trials where the route is lost show typical escape line, which indicate a linear increase of the minimal distance to the checkpoints along the route whereas route following behaviour should be characterize by a constant (and lower) minimal distance to checkpoints. To identify this linear increase of the minimal distance to checkpoint we calculated its rate along checkpoints. We then defined a threshold based on the distance inter-checkpoints (Trf = 0.75Dck with Trf the threshold and Dck the distance between two consecutive checkpoints). Whenever the minimal distance variation between two consecutive checkpoint failed to stay under this threshold (Min(dCki) − Min(dCkj)>Trf; with dCk the distance to a checkpoint j = i+ 1), it indicates the agent never get closer than 0.75 times the distance inter-checkpoint than it was at the time it reach the minimal distance to the previous checkpoint, showing a lack of progress along the route. The checkpoint is then tagged as not visited and two consecutive checkpoints not visited are considered an end to the route following behaviour. The last checkpoint visited is therefore the further on-route location. Note that we did not consider any return to route following after it was consider out once. (C) Example of the route following end point detection by the routine described previously on 15 trials of a simulation. Red dots indicate both the further checkpoint reached on the learned route (green line) and the closest location to this checkpoint on the retrieval attempt (black line). (D) Boxplot of the percentage of route traveled based on the estimation of the last on-route checkpoint visited. (E) Success rate (%) at different portion of the route. Note that the learned route turns always happen at 20% and 60% of the “zigzag” routes. Fig H. Calculation of the MB model performance index. (A) To estimate the intrinsic performance of the MB model in route following simulations, we define a performance index based on the alignment of the agent at time where the MBON is active (views recognized as on the route) and the actual orientation of the nearest route location. (B) Alignment of these orientation indicate a good recognition and the cosinus of the orientation difference lie close to 1, whereas perpendicular orientations lie around 0 and opposed alignement around -1. The averaging of all these comparison () therefore range from -1 for an overall anti-alignment between the actual route and the recognized route, to 1 for a perfect alignment/recognition. (C) For each simulation we therefore estimate a score indicating the actual intrinsic performance of the MB model. (D) To estimate a limit/threshold indicating that the MB model was better than random we benefit from simulations where the MB was actually constantly active, indicating an unspecific recognition of on-route views. We then arbitrarily define a value just over the range of MB performance indexes estimated for these simulations to define our threshold. All simulations with a score lower are considered impaired by the MB model itself and excluded from analysis. Table A. List of neuron-to-neuron gain parameters used in simulations () Note that parameters are not set to reflect a biological reality but rather to ensure a stable function of the model. However, no automatic optimization process have been applied to define this particular set of parameters.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1011480.s001
(PDF)
Acknowledgments
We are very thankful to people from both the Insect Robotics group in Edinburgh and the Vision group in Lund for the fruitful discussion that helped improve the development and the description of the model.
References
- 1. Jeffery KJ. Distorting the metric fabric of the cognitive map. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2015;19(6):300–301. pmid:25882915
- 2. Grieves RM, Jeffery KJ. The representation of space in the brain. Behavioural processes. 2017;135:113–131. pmid:28034697
- 3. Tolman EC. Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological review. 1948;55(4):189. pmid:18870876
- 4. Jacobs LF. The evolution of the cognitive map. Brain, behavior and evolution. 2003;62(2):128–139. pmid:12937351
- 5. Cheung A, Collett M, Collett TS, Dewar A, Dyer F, Graham P, et al. Still no convincing evidence for cognitive map use by honeybees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2014;111(42):E4396–E4397. pmid:25277972
- 6. Cheeseman JF, Millar CD, Greggers U, Lehmann K, Pawley MD, Gallistel CR, et al. Reply to Cheung et. al.: The cognitive map hypothesis remains the best interpretation of the data in honeybee navigation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2014;111(42):E4398–E4398.
- 7. Webb B. The internal maps of insects. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2019;222(Suppl 1). pmid:30728234
- 8. Seelig JD, Jayaraman V. Feature detection and orientation tuning in the Drosophila central complex. Nature. 2013;503(7475):262–266. pmid:24107996
- 9. Seelig JD, Jayaraman V. Neural dynamics for landmark orientation and angular path integration. Nature. 2015;521(7551):186–191. pmid:25971509
- 10. Turner-Evans DB, Jayaraman V. The insect central complex. Current Biology. 2016;26(11):R453–R457. pmid:27269718
- 11. Pisokas I, Heinze S, Webb B. The head direction circuit of two insect species. eLife. 2020;9:e53985. pmid:32628112
- 12. Keram P. The neuronal building blocks of the navigational toolkit in the central complex of insects. Current Opinion in Insect Science. 2022;.
- 13. Stone T, Webb B, Adden A, Weddig NB, Honkanen A, Templin R, et al. An anatomically constrained model for path integration in the bee brain. Current Biology. 2017;27(20):3069–3085.e11. pmid:28988858
- 14. Wolf H. Odometry and insect navigation. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2011;214(10):1629–1641. pmid:21525309
- 15. Heinze S, Narendra A, Cheung A. Principles of insect path integration. Current Biology. 2018;28(17):R1043–R1058. pmid:30205054
- 16. Lyu C, Abbott L, Maimon G. Building an allocentric travelling direction signal via vector computation. Nature. 2022;601(7891):92–97. pmid:34912112
- 17. Lu J, Behbahani AH, Hamburg L, Westeinde EA, Dawson PM, Lyu C, et al. Transforming representations of movement from body-to world-centric space. Nature. 2022;601(7891):98–104. pmid:34912123
- 18. Matheson AM, Lanz AJ, Medina AM, Licata AM, Currier TA, Syed MH, et al. A neural circuit for wind-guided olfactory navigation. Nature Communications. 2022;13(1):1–21. pmid:35941114
- 19. Hoinville T, Wehner R. Optimal multiguidance integration in insect navigation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2018;115(11):2824–2829. pmid:29483254
- 20. Le Moël F, Stone T, Lihoreau M, Wystrach A, Webb B. The central complex as a potential substrate for vector based navigation. Frontiers in psychology. 2019;10:690. pmid:31024377
- 21. Webb B, Wystrach A. Neural mechanisms of insect navigation. Current Opinion in Insect Science. 2016;15:27–39. pmid:27436729
- 22. Kim IS, Dickinson MH. Idiothetic path integration in the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster. Current Biology. 2017;27(15):2227–2238. pmid:28736164
- 23. Titova AV, Kau BE, Tibor S, Mach J, Vo-Doan TT, Wittlinger M, et al. Displacement experiments provide evidence for path integration in Drosophila. bioRxiv. 2022;.
- 24. Corfas RA, Sharma T, Dickinson MH. Diverse food-sensing neurons trigger idiothetic local search in Drosophila. Current Biology. 2019;29(10):1660–1668. pmid:31056390
- 25. Behbahani AH, Palmer EH, Corfas RA, Dickinson MH. Drosophila re-zero their path integrator at the center of a fictive food patch. Current Biology. 2021;31(20):4534–4546. pmid:34450090
- 26. Goulard R, Buehlmann C, Niven E Jeremy, Graham P, Webb B. A unified mechanism for innate and learned visual landmark guidance in the insect central complex. PLOS Computational Biology. 2021;17(9):e1009383. pmid:34555013
- 27. Giraldo YM, Leitch KJ, Ros IG, Warren TL, Weir PT, Dickinson MH. Sun navigation requires compass neurons in Drosophila. Current Biology. 2018;28(17):2845–2852.e4. pmid:30174187
- 28. El Jundi B, Baird E, Byrne MJ, Dacke M. The brain behind straight-line orientation in dung beetles. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2019;222(Suppl 1).
- 29. Young JM, Armstrong JD. Structure of the adult central complex in Drosophila: Organization of distinct neuronal subsets. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2010;518(9):1500–1524. pmid:20187142
- 30. Heinze S, Florman J, Asokaraj S, Jundi Be, Reppert SM. Anatomical basis of sun compass navigation II: The neuronal composition of the central complex of the monarch butterfly. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2013;521(2):267–298. pmid:22886450
- 31. Pires PM, Abbott L, Maimon G. Converting an allocentric goal into an egocentric steering signal. bioRxiv. 2022;.
- 32. Wehner R. The architecture of the desert ant’s navigational toolkit (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Myrmecol News. 2009;12(September):85–96.
- 33. Collett M, Chittka L, Collett TS. Spatial memory in insect navigation. Current Biology. 2013;23(17):R789–R800. pmid:24028962
- 34. Sun X, Yue S, Mangan M. A decentralised neural model explaining optimal integration of navigational strategies in insects. Elife. 2020;9:e54026. pmid:32589143
- 35. Wystrach A, Mangan M, Webb B. Optimal cue integration in ants. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2015;282(1816):20151484. pmid:26400741
- 36. Sayre ME, Templin R, Chavez J, Kempenaers J, Heinze S. A projectome of the bumblebee central complex. Elife. 2021;10:e68911. pmid:34523418
- 37. Green J, Maimon G. Building a heading signal from anatomically defined neuron types in the Drosophila central complex. Current opinion in neurobiology. 2018;52:156–164. pmid:30029143
- 38. El Jundi B, Pfeiffer K, Heinze S, Homberg U. Integration of polarization and chromatic cues in the insect sky compass. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 2014;200(6):575–589. pmid:24589854
- 39. Dacke M, Bell AT, Foster JJ, Baird EJ, Strube-Bloss MF, Byrne MJ, et al. Multimodal cue integration in the dung beetle compass. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2019;116(28):14248–14253.
- 40. Shaverdian S, Dirlik E, Mitchell R, Tocco C, Webb B, Dacke M. Weighted cue integration for straight-line orientation. iScience. 2022; p. 105207. pmid:36274940
- 41. Homberg U. In search of the sky compass in the insect brain. Naturwissenschaften. 2004;91(5):199–208. pmid:15146265
- 42. Hardcastle BJ, Omoto JJ, Kandimalla P, Nguyen BCM, Keleş MF, Boyd NK, et al. A visual pathway for skylight polarization processing in Drosophila. Elife. 2021;10:e63225. pmid:33755020
- 43. Fisher YE. Flexible navigational computations in the Drosophila central complex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2022;73:102514. pmid:35196623
- 44. Grob R, Tritscher C, Grübel K, Stigloher C, Groh C, Fleischmann PN, et al. Johnston’s organ and its central projections in Cataglyphis desert ants. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2021;529(8):2138–2155. pmid:33258153
- 45. Okubo TS, Patella P, D’Alessandro I, Wilson RI. A neural network for wind-guided compass navigation. Neuron. 2020;107(5):924–940. pmid:32681825
- 46. El Jundi B, Dacke M. Insect Orientation: The Drosophila Wind Compass Pathway. Current Biology. 2021;31(2):R83–R85.
- 47. Fisher YE, Marquis M, D’Alessandro I, Wilson RI. Dopamine promotes head direction plasticity during orienting movements. Nature. 2022; p. 1–7. pmid:36450986
- 48. Mitchell R, Shaverdian S, Dacke M, Webb B. A model of cue integration as vector summation in the insect brain. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 2023;290(2001):20230767. pmid:37357865
- 49. Turner-Evans D, Wegener S, Rouault H, Franconville R, Wolff T, Seelig JD, et al. Angular velocity integration in a fly heading circuit. Elife. 2017;6:e23496. pmid:28530551
- 50. Green J, Adachi A, Shah KK, Hirokawa JD, Magani PS, Maimon G. A neural circuit architecture for angular integration in Drosophila. Nature. 2017;546(7656):101–106. pmid:28538731
- 51. Rayshubskiy A, Holtz SL, D’Alessandro I, Li AA, Vanderbeck QX, Haber IS, et al. Neural control of steering in walking Drosophila. bioRxiv. 2020; p. 2020.04.04.024703.
- 52. Westeinde EA, Kellogg E, Dawson PM, Lu J, Hamburg L, Midler B, et al. Transforming a head direction signal into a goal-oriented steering command. bioRxiv. 2022;.
- 53. Srinivasan MV. Going with the flow: a brief history of the study of the honeybee’s navigational ‘odometer’. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 2014;200(6):563–573.
- 54. Honkanen A, Hensgen R, Kannan K, Adden A, Warrant E, Wcislo W, et al. Parallel motion vision pathways in the brain of a tropical bee. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 2023; p. 1–29. pmid:37017717
- 55. Wittlinger M, Wehner R, Wolf H. The ant odometer: stepping on stilts and stumps. science. 2006;312(5782):1965–1967. pmid:16809544
- 56. Wittlinger M, Wehner R, Wolf H. The desert ant odometer: a stride integrator that accounts for stride length and walking speed. Journal of experimental Biology. 2007;210(2):198–207. pmid:17210957
- 57. Pisokas I, Rössler W, Webb B, Zeil J, Narendra A. Anesthesia disrupts distance, but not direction, of path integration memory. Current Biology. 2022;32(2):445–452. pmid:34852215
- 58. Yilmaz A, Gagnon Y, Byrne M, Baird E, Dacke M. Cold-induced anesthesia impairs path integration memory in dung beetles. Current Biology. 2022;32(2):438–444. pmid:34852216
- 59. Patel RN, Kempenaers J, Heinze S. Vector navigation in walking bumblebees. Current Biology. 2022;32(13):2871–2883. pmid:35640624
- 60.
Hulse BK, Haberkern H, Franconville R, Turner-Evans DB, Takemura S, Wolff T, et al. A connectome of the Drosophila central complex reveals network motifs suitable for flexible navigation and context-dependent action selection. bioRxiv. 2020;.
- 61.
Hertz JA. Introduction to the theory of neural computation. CRC Press; 2018.
- 62. Heinze S. Unraveling the neural basis of insect navigation. Current Opinion in Insect Science. 2017;24:58–67. pmid:29208224
- 63. Srinivasan M, Zhang S, Lehrer M, Collett T. Honeybee navigation en route to the goal: visual flight control and odometry. The Journal of experimental biology. 1996;199(1):237–244. pmid:9317712
- 64. Srinivasan MV, Poteser M, Kral K. Motion detection in insect orientation and navigation. Vision research. 1999;39(16):2749–2766. pmid:10492835
- 65. Taylor GK, Krapp HG. Sensory systems and flight stability: what do insects measure and why? Advances in insect physiology. 2007;34:231–316.
- 66. Collett TS, Collett M. Memory use in insect visual navigation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2002;3(7):542–552. pmid:12094210
- 67. Collett TS, Graham P, Durier V. Route learning by insects. Current opinion in neurobiology. 2003;13(6):718–725. pmid:14662374
- 68. Buehlmann C, Wozniak B, Goulard R, Webb B, Graham P, Niven JE. Mushroom bodies are required for learned visual navigation, but not for innate visual behavior, in ants. Current Biology. 2020;30(17):3438–3443.e2. pmid:32707069
- 69. Kamhi JF, Barron AB, Narendra A. Vertical lobes of the mushroom bodies are essential for view-based navigation in Australian Myrmecia ants. Current Biology. 2020;30(17):3432–3437.e3. pmid:32707060
- 70. Ardin P, Peng F, Mangan M, Lagogiannis K, Webb B. Using an insect mushroom body circuit to encode route memory in complex natural environments. PLOS Computational Biology. 2016;12(2):e1004683. pmid:26866692
- 71. Zeil J. Visual navigation: properties, acquisition and use of views. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 2022; p. 1–16. pmid:36515743
- 72. Buehlmann C, Dell-Cronin S, Diyalagoda Pathirannahelage A, Goulard R, Webb B, Niven JE, et al. Impact of central complex lesions on innate and learnt visual navigation in ants. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 2023; p. 1–10. pmid:36790487
- 73. Stopfer M. Central processing in the mushroom bodies. Current opinion in insect science. 2014;6:99–103. pmid:25621203
- 74. Wystrach A, Buehlmann C, Schwarz S, Cheng K, Graham P. Rapid aversive and memory trace learning during route navigation in desert ants. Current Biology. 2020;30(10):1927–1933.e2. pmid:32275874
- 75. Islam M, Deeti S, Mahmudah Z, Kamhi JF, Cheng K. Detouring while foraging up a tree: What bull ants (Myrmecia midas) learn and their reactions to novel sensory cues. Journal of Comparative Psychology. 2023;137(1). pmid:36931833
- 76. Collett TS, Collett M. Path integration in insects. Current opinion in neurobiology. 2000;10(6):757–762. pmid:11240286
- 77. Neuser K, Triphan T, Mronz M, Poeck B, Strauss R. Analysis of a spatial orientation memory in Drosophila. Nature. 2008;453(7199):1244–1247. pmid:18509336
- 78. Wystrach A, Moël FL, Clement L, Schwarz S. A lateralised design for the interaction of visual memories and heading representations in navigating ants. bioRxiv. 2020; p. 2020.08.13.249193.
- 79. Beetz MJ, Kraus C, el Jundi B. Neural representation of goal direction in the monarch butterfly brain. bioRxiv. 2022; p. 2022–10.
- 80. Collett M, Collett TS. How does the insect central complex use mushroom body output for steering? Current Biology. 2018;28(13):R733–R734. pmid:29990452
- 81. Corrales-Carvajal VM, Faisal AA, Ribeiro C. Internal states drive nutrient homeostasis by modulating exploration-exploitation trade-off. Elife. 2016;5:e19920. pmid:27770569
- 82. Green J, Vijayan V, Mussells Pires P, Adachi A, Maimon G. A neural heading estimate is compared with an internal goal to guide oriented navigation. Nature Neuroscience. 2019;22(9):1460–1468. pmid:31332373
- 83. Tarsitano M, Jackson RR. Jumping spiders make predatory detours requiring movement away from prey. Behaviour. 1994;131(1-2):65–73.
- 84. Currier TA, Matheson AM, Nagel KI. Encoding and control of orientation to airflow by a set of Drosophila fan-shaped body neurons. Elife. 2020;9:e61510. pmid:33377868
- 85. Dan C, Hulse BK, Jayaraman V, Hermundstad AM. Flexible control of behavioral variability mediated by an internal representation of head direction. bioRxiv. 2021; p. 2021–08.
- 86. Steele TJ, Lanz AJ, Nagel KI. Olfactory navigation in arthropods. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 2023; p. 1–22. pmid:36658447
- 87. Buehlmann C, Mangan M, Graham P. Multimodal interactions in insect navigation. Animal Cognition. 2020;23(6):1129–1141. pmid:32323027
- 88. Cardé RT, Willis MA. Navigational strategies used by insects to find distant, wind-borne sources of odor. Journal of chemical ecology. 2008;34(7):854–866. pmid:18581182
- 89. Baker KL, Dickinson M, Findley TM, Gire DH, Louis M, Suver MP, et al. Algorithms for olfactory search across species. Journal of Neuroscience. 2018;38(44):9383–9389. pmid:30381430
- 90. Kuenen L, Carde RT. Strategies for recontacting a lost pheromone plume: casting and upwind flight in the male gypsy moth. Physiological Entomology. 1994;19(1):15–29.
- 91. Perisse E, Burke C, Huetteroth W, Waddell S. Shocking revelations and saccharin sweetness in the study of Drosophila olfactory memory. Current Biology. 2013;23(17):R752–R763. pmid:24028959
- 92. Steinbeck F, Adden A, Graham P. Connecting brain to behaviour: a role for general purpose steering circuits in insect orientation? Journal of Experimental Biology. 2020;223(5):jeb212332. pmid:32161054
- 93. Adden A, Stewart TC, Webb B, Heinze S. A neural model for insect steering applied to olfaction and path integration. Neural computation. 2022;34(11):2205–2231. pmid:36112910
- 94. Clement L, Schwarz S, Wystrach A. An intrinsic oscillator underlies visual navigation in ants. Current Biology. 2022;. pmid:36538930
- 95. Currier TA, Nagel KI. Multisensory control of navigation in the fruit fly. Current opinion in neurobiology. 2020;64:10–16. pmid:31841944
- 96. Collett TS, Graham P. Animal navigation: path integration, visual landmarks and cognitive maps. Current Biology. 2004;14(12):R475–R477. pmid:15203020
- 97. Menzel R, Kirbach A, Haass WD, Fischer B, Fuchs J, Koblofsky M, et al. A common frame of reference for learned and communicated vectors in honeybee navigation. Current Biology. 2011;21(8):645–650. pmid:21474313
- 98. Li F, Lindsey JW, Marin EC, Otto N, Dreher M, Dempsey G, et al. The connectome of the adult Drosophila mushroom body provides insights into function. Elife. 2020;9:e62576. pmid:33315010
- 99. Land MF, Eckert H. Maps of the acute zones of fly eyes. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 1985;156:525–538.
- 100. Land MF. Visual acuity in insects. Annual review of entomology. 1997;42(1):147–177. pmid:15012311
- 101. Straw AD, Warrant EJ, O’Carroll DC. Abright zone’in male hoverfly (Eristalis tenax) eyes and associated faster motion detection and increased contrast sensitivity. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2006;209(21):4339–4354. pmid:17050849
- 102. Pfeiffer K, Homberg U. Organization and functional roles of the central complex in the insect brain. Annual Review of Entomology. 2014;59(1):165–184. pmid:24160424
- 103. Kim SS, Rouault H, Druckmann S, Jayaraman V. Ring attractor dynamics in the Drosophila central brain. Science. 2017;356(6340):849–853. pmid:28473639