Abstract
Wood ants are excellent navigators, using a combination of innate and learnt navigational strategies to travel between their nest and feeding sites. Visual navigation in ants has been studied extensively, however, we have little direct evidence for the underlying neural mechanisms. Here, we perform lateralized mechanical lesions in the central complex (CX) of wood ants, a midline structure known to allow an insect to keep track of the direction of sensory cues relative to its own orientation and to control movement. We lesioned two groups of ants and observed their behaviour in an arena with a large visual landmark present. The first group of ants were naïve and when intact such ants show a clear innate attraction to the conspicuous landmark. The second group of ants were trained to aim to a food location to the side of the landmark. The general heading of naïve ants towards a visual cue was not altered by the lesions, but the heading of ants trained to a landmark adjacent food position was affected. Thus, CX lesions had a specific impact on learnt visual guidance. We also observed that lateralised lesions altered the fine details of turning with lesioned ants spending less time turning to the side ipsilateral of the lesion. The results confirm the role of the CX in turn control and highlight its important role in the implementation of learnt behaviours that rely on information from other brain regions.
Similar content being viewed by others
Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.
Introduction
Ant foragers travel diligently back and forth between their nest and feeding sites using a combination of innate and learnt navigational strategies (Buehlmann et al. 2020b; Knaden and Graham 2016). Innate strategies such as path integration (Collett and Collett 2000; Mueller and Wehner 1988; Wehner and Srinivasan 2003), pheromone trails (Harrison et al. 1989), attraction to food odours (Buehlmann et al. 2014), and/or innate responses to visual cues (Buehlmann and Graham 2022; Collett 2010; Graham et al. 2003) are vital during exploration of novel territory. Innate responses shape the ants’ paths and thus ultimately facilitate the learning of visual cues required for establishing habitual foraging routes to stable feeding sites (Collett 2010; Graham et al. 2003; Heusser and Wehner 2002). Visual navigation in ants has been studied extensively at the behavioural level (reviewed in Collett et al. 2013; Graham and Philippides 2017; Wehner et al. 2014; Zeil 2012). Direct evidence for which circuits underly ant behaviour is sparse, although there is rapidly increasing information from other insects about neural areas that are hypothesised to have a role. Recent lesion studies in ants (Buehlmann et al. 2020c; Kamhi et al. 2020) demonstrate the need for the mushroom bodies (MBs), as associative learning centres, to learn the valence of views that can be used for navigation. However, other brain regions are likely involved in the transformation of MB outputs to steering behaviour and MBs do not appear to be required in innate visual orientation (Buehlmann et al. 2020c).
The central complex (CX), a collection of neuropils located at the midline of the insect brain (Strausfeld 1999), receives sensory input allowing an insect to keep track of the direction of sensory cues relative to its own orientation (Seelig and Jayaraman 2015), and to directly control movement (Martin et al. 2015). Studies have shown that the CX plays a role in insect orientation behaviours that use celestial information (locusts: (Pegel et al. 2018; Vitzthum et al. 2002); dung beetles: (el Jundi et al. 2015); monarch butterfly: (Heinze et al. 2013; Heinze and Reppert 2011); bees: (Stone et al. 2017); crickets: (Sakura et al. 2008); fruit flies: (Giraldo et al. 2018)), and terrestrial visual information (fruit flies: (Green et al. 2019; Kuntz et al. 2012; Liu et al. 2006; Neuser et al. 2008; Ofstad et al. 2011; Seelig and Jayaraman 2013); cockroaches: (Ritzmann et al. 2008); locust: (Rosner and Homberg 2013)). While fundamental knowledge has been gained from electrophysiological studies in which animals are restrained, we also have a handful of examples where neural recordings in the CX were performed in moving animals (e.g., Beetz et al. 2022; Green et al. 2019; Wosnitza et al. 2022). In insects, the CX has a role in the control of speed, turning behaviour, and spatial orientation (reviewed in Fisher 2022; Honkanen et al. 2019; Pfeiffer and Homberg 2014; Varga et al. 2017). Maintaining a stable direction relative to celestial or terrestrial visual cues allows insects to move in a straight line into a chosen direction but this ability needs to be augmented to account for the flexible visual navigation behaviours seen in ants and other species.
Many goal-directed behaviours executed by insects require associative learning, and the formation of long-term memories. This is particularly important for insects that engage in central place foraging, because they must learn the location of the nest and be able to return to it from multiple foraging sites. The CX has been implicated in visual place learning in fruit flies (Ofstad et al. 2011). Also, an increase of the CX volume was shown in new ant foragers that were exposed to skylight information during the learning of navigationally relevant information at the offset of their foraging career, indicating its involvement in navigation (Grob et al. 2017). A number of computational models have shown, by reproducing behavioural observations, that the CX circuit is a plausible substrate for innate and learnt visual navigation in ants (Goulard et al. 2021; Sun et al. 2020, 2021; Wystrach et al. 2020).
We show here direct evidence for the involvement of the CX in innate and learnt visual navigation of freely moving wood ants.
Materials and methods
Ants
Experiments were performed with laboratory kept wood ants Formica rufa L. collected from Ashdown Forest, East Sussex, UK. Ants were kept in the laboratory under a 12 h light: 12 h darkness cycle at 25–27 °C. Ants were fed ad libitum with sucrose, dead crickets and water. During the experiments, food was limited to increase the ants’ foraging motivation, but water was permanently available. All experiments were undertaken with ethical approval from the University of Sussex.
Behavioural setup
The centre of the circular platform (120 cm in diameter) was placed within a cylindrical arena (diameter 3 m, height 1.8 m) with white walls (Fig. 1a; (Buehlmann et al. 2020c)). A 20° wide rectangle (height: 90 cm, width: 52 cm) was placed on the inner wall of the surrounding cylinder. To remove possible olfactory cues, the surface of the platform was covered with white paper, which was rotated between recordings. The centre of the platform consisted of a cylindrical release chamber of 6.5 cm diameter, which was remotely lowered to release the ant onto the platform. The ants’ position and body orientation were recorded every 20 ms using a tracking video camera (Trackit, SciTrackS GmbH).
To test the ants’ innate visual response, ants with no prior experience to this setup were released from the release chamber and their paths were recorded. To test the ants’ learnt visual response, other groups of ants underwent a training to learn to find a drop of sucrose on a microscope slide that, from the centre of the platform, was 30° to the right of the visual cue described above. During this training, individually marked ants were taken from the nests and released in the centre of the platform. Ants performed approximately 10 group training runs before being released and recorded individually. Once an ant had reached the feeder and started to feed, it was transferred into a feeding box and the next ant was released. Ants were considered to be reliable and accurate if they had approached the feeder directly on three consecutive training runs.
Lesion procedures
We made lateralised lesions in the CX in the brains of naïve ants with no prior experience of this behavioural setup as well as in the CX of ants that have learnt to navigate to the feeder location. These ants were immobilised on ice for 90 s and harnessed in a custom-made holder keeping their head fixed with plasticine while their body was free to move. Antennae were restrained with a pin. To access the CX, a small window was cut with a piece of razor blade below the medial ocellus. Mechanical lesions were made relative to landmarks (including tracheal branches) visible on the anterior surface of the brain, however, we did not control the depth. The bottom of the central tracheal branch, directly below the medial ocellus, was used as a superficial marker of the position of the CX. The bottom of it is approximately 53% down the midline of the brain (i.e., 218 µm down the midline that has a length of 409 µm in the brain shown in Fig. 1b). Other measurements made using a confocal microscope confirmed that this is within the range of the CX, the lower edge of which is approximately 58% down this midline (Fig. 1c). We made lateral CX lesions approximately 20 µm to the left or right of the midline. The width of the CX is around 200 µm at its maximum (Fig. 1c). These measurements ensured that we lesioned the CX. However, we did not visualise the lesion locations after ants were tested. This prevents us from localising the lesions precisely. Glass capillaries used for the mechanical lesions (Harvard Apparatus, Cambourne, UK; 30-0035; 1.00 mm outer diameter, 0.78 mm inner diameter, 10 mm length) were pulled with a P-97 Micropipette Puller (Sutter Instrument, Novato, California, USA) and then broken manually to a tip size of 10 µm and dipped in black ink to aid visibility of the capillary during the lesioning. After inserting the capillary into either the left (LLes) or right (RLes) side of the CX, the cuticle lid was put back and secured in place with a small drop of cyanoacrylate adhesive. Control ants (Contr) were handled and dissected in the same way as lesioned ants but the glass capillary was not inserted. After the CX lesion (or control) was performed, ants were placed into a box and allowed to rest for 10 min before their navigational performance was recorded on the platform.
Analysis
Paths were analysed in Matlab R2021b with custom written scripts. Trajectories were trimmed at r = 50 cm, where r is the distance from the platform centre, and the final heading directions determined at that point. Walking speed and path straightness (beeline/total path length) were calculated for trajectories from r = 3.25 cm to r = 50 cm. A more detailed analysis was performed for the ants’ turning behaviour. To determine turns, we took the angular velocity trace for each trajectory and smoothed it with a window of 50 frames to remove high frequency noise, we segregated this trace into turns by looking for zero crossings. Turns that were less than 0.2 s were not included in further analysis as these are often caused by periods of very low velocity or short straight path segments. Left and right turns were determined for each trajectory and duration and angle of those turns calculated. For each trajectory, we calculated the mean turn duration and mean turn angle for left and right turns, respectively. For both turn duration and angle, we then looked at turn balance by normalising the mean time or angle taken up by left turns (left mean/(left mean + right mean) for each ant.
Results
Overall path structure in naïve ants is not affected by CX lesions
CX lesions did not affect the ants’ innate preference for large dark objects, i.e., the innate visual orientation of CX-lesioned ants was not impaired (Fig. 2a–c). Control (Contr, n = 37 ants) and lesioned ants (left lesions, LLes, n = 24 ants; right lesion, RLes, n = 26 ants) were directed (Rayleigh test, all p < 0.001; Lengths of mean vector (r), Contr: 0.68, LLes: 0.75, RLes: 0.87). Ants from the three groups did not differ from each other in their path directions (Mardia–Watson Wheeler tests, all p > 0.05) and all three groups approached the visual cue, i.e., the 95% CI of the ants’ final heading direction encompassed the visual cue.
Furthermore, walking speed (Fig. 3a) and overall path straightness (Fig. 3b) did not differ between the control and the two lesioned groups (Kruskal Wallis with Dunn’s post hoc tests, all p > 0.05). In all three groups, the number of left and right turns per trajectory did not differ from each other (Wilcoxon tests, all p > 0.05). Hence, lesions in the CX had no discernable impact on the ants’ ability to move and their overall path structure.
CX lesions disrupt turning in innate visual orientation
Detailed path analysis revealed that the CX lesions disrupted the ants’ fine-scale turning behaviour. Duration and angle of all turns and means per ant for left and right turns, respectively, are shown in Online Resource 1. We analysed the turn balance by normalising the mean time taken up by left turns (left mean/(left mean + right mean) for each ant. We found that the two groups of lesioned ants were significantly different from each other (LLes vs. RLes, Kruskal Wallis with Dunn’s post hoc tests, p < 0.05). More specifically, lesioned ants had relatively shorter duration turns in the ipsilateral direction, i.e., LLes ants had relatively shorter turns to the left than RLes ants (Fig. 2d). The normalised left turn angles, however, were not significantly different between the three groups (Fig. 2e; Kruskal Wallis with Dunn’s post hoc tests, all p > 0.05), suggesting faster turns to the side ipsilateral to the lesion. As expected, we observed that the mean heading relative to the visual cue after a right and left turn differed significantly in all three conditions (Fig. 2f; Wilcoxon tests, all p < 0.01). At the end of a right turn, ants were oriented to the right relative to the centre of the visual cue, whereas at the end of a left turn, ants were oriented to the left of the centre of the visual cue.
CX lesions disrupt visual navigation along a learnt route
Innate visual behaviours shape foraging and thus navigation but are not sufficient for efficient navigation and so we also tested the impact of CX lesions on the ability to navigate along a learnt route. Ants learnt to navigate to a feeder and once they were reliably navigating to the feeding site, we performed the CX lesion and tested the ants’ route knowledge afterwards. Paths in all three groups were still directed (Fig. 4a–c; Rayleigh test, all p < 0.05; Lengths of mean vector (r), Contr: 0.74, LLes: 0.97, RLes: 0.59). In the control group, the ants’ headings did not differ between the training and test (Contr, n = 14 ants; Mardia–Watson Wheeler test, p > 0.05), hence, these ants were still able to follow the learnt route. In CX-lesioned ants, however, the ants’ headings were significantly different in training and tests and ants no longer accurately approached the learnt feeder location (LLes, n = 9 ants; RLes, n = 9 ants; Mardia–Watson Wheeler test, both p < 0.01).
In all three groups, the walking speed did not differ between the training and test (Fig. 3c; Wilcoxon tests, all p > 0.05). Overall path straightness, however, was significantly lower in the tests than training runs in all three groups (Fig. 3d; Wilcoxon tests, all p < 0.05). The number of left and right turns, however, did not differ from each other in any of the three groups (Wilcoxon test, all p > 0.05).
Furthermore, we observe here the same trend for the normalised left turn duration as in naïve ants, however, this difference was not significant (Fig. 4d; Kruskal Wallis with Dunn’s post hoc tests, all p > 0.05). The three groups also did not differ from each other in the normalised left turn angles (Fig. 4e; Kruskal Wallis with Dunn’s post hoc tests, all p > 0.05). See Online Resource 2 for duration and angle of all turns and means per ant for left and right turns, respectively. In trained ants, we see a significant difference between the mean angles after a right and left turn in the LLes group (Fig. 4f; Wilcoxon test, p < 0.01) but not in the RLes and Contr groups (Wilcoxon test, both p > 0.05), but given the increase in sinuosity and small sample size these results seem broadly consistent with the results from naive ants. Altogether, we show here that CX-lesioned ants are no longer able to navigate towards a learnt feeder location.
Discussion
In recent years, the CX has received attention as a brain area crucial for orientation in insects (Fisher 2022; Honkanen et al. 2019; Pfeiffer 2022; Pfeiffer and Homberg 2014; Turner-Evans and Jayaraman 2016; Varga et al. 2017). The CX receives pre-processed sensory input that allows insects to orient themselves relative to external cues (Seelig and Jayaraman 2015). Furthermore, the CX can directly control movement (Bender et al. 2010). Keeping a stable direction relative to celestial or terrestrial visual cues is, however, not always sufficient for navigation and many goal-directed behaviours require associative learning, and the formation of long-term memories. We show here that CX lesions have an impact on innate and learnt visual navigation (Figs. 2 and 4). More specifically, we show here that lesions in the CX lead to a lateralised disruption in ants’ turning behaviour but that naïve ants with lateralised CX lesions can still control their overall orientation towards the visual cue (Fig. 2). Ants that learnt to navigate along a visually guided route, however, are no longer able to accurately approach the learnt feeder location when the CX is lesioned (Fig. 4).
Involvement of the CX in learnt visual navigation
Perhaps, the most striking result here is that lateralised CX lesions lead to a strong impact on learnt visual guidance. These ants reverted partially but not fully towards their innate visual response when the CX was lesioned.
A possible interpretation of our result is that the CX is the basis of visual memory (Ofstad et al. 2011) but we have previously observed that such visual guidance is disturbed by MB lesions that leave the CX intact (Buehlmann et al. 2020c; Kamhi et al. 2020). We, therefore, conclude that the loss in CX-lesioned ants reflects an inability to use this memory to drive steering. This seems to confirm that MBs are a structure that encodes the valence of sensory inputs rather than encoding any explicit spatial information directly (Collett and Collett 2018; Matheson et al. 2022; Sun et al. 2020) and that the influence of MB on navigation could be completely dependent on the way that the CX encodes spatial information and drives steering (Goulard et al. 2021).
Tangential neurons in the fan-shaped body of the CX have input neuron dendrites that are found in brain areas that are also innervated by MB output neurons (Heinze et al. 2013; Matheson et al. 2022; Young and Armstrong 2010). These MB output neurons encode the valence of sensory cues (Aso et al. 2014), hence, if a current view matches with a learnt view, the current head-direction activity pattern in the proto-cerebral-bridge region of the CX could be imprinted onto the orientation coding neurons (Honkanen et al. 2019). Consistent with this idea, we know from recent connectomic work on Drosophila that approximately half of the MB output neurons directly synapse onto tangential neurons of the fan-shaped body (Hulse et al. 2021; Li et al. 2020). Our results are consistent with the assumption that our lesions have disturbed this pathway, preventing the MB signal from influencing the steering circuit to drive approach in the learned direction.
Control of innate visual behaviour in the CX
Many insects show innate preferences, i.e., they perform fixed motor behaviours in response to specific visual stimuli. Tall objects are attractive for many insects, including ants (fruit flies: Gotz 1987; Strauss and Pichler 1998; Wehner 1972), locusts: (Wallace 1962), ladybirds: (Collett 1988), mantids: (Poteser and Kral 1995), ants: (Buehlmann and Graham 2022; Voss 1967), monarch butterflies: (Franzke et al. 2022)). Experiments in Drosophila have revealed that the CX is required for innate visual approaches (Bausenwein et al. 1994) and spatial learning paradigms in Drosophila revealed its importance for visual and spatial memory and directional decision making (Kuntz et al. 2012; Neuser et al. 2008; Ofstad et al. 2011; Seelig and Jayaraman 2013, 2015). Our lesion method allows us to perform lateralised lesions in the CX, however, we cannot get more detailed information about which part exactly was lesioned. Ring neurons in the ellipsoid body map visual cues spatially (Seelig and Jayaraman 2013) and computational simulations have revealed that the lateralised ellipsoid body input in Drosophila is suitable for innate responses to bar-like objects (Dewar et al. 2017; Wystrach et al. 2014). In addition, ellipsoid body neurones have been shown to be important for short-term spatial memory, during which they encode the position of a target (e.g., an attractive visual cue) and can guide movement towards it (Neuser et al. 2008) even when temporarily invisible. Given its conserved structure across insects, it is likely that the ellipsoid body plays a similar role in ants. With lateralised lesions, ants retained their innate attraction to the visual cue, suggesting that the wide field visual inputs from each visual hemisphere are sufficient for overall path control.
Control of turning behaviour by the CX
The CX is also involved in fine-scale control of steering including the control of walking speed (Bender et al. 2010; Martin et al. 2015) and turning (Guo and Ritzmann 2013). The symmetrical features of the CX along the medial–lateral axis might suggest a lateralized role of the CX (see e.g., (Pfeiffer and Homberg 2014)). Our data suggest that ants had slightly faster turns in the ipsilateral direction, given that the mean duration of their left turns was shorter, while the angle turned was the same (Figs. 2 and 4). Faster movements have previously been described in ants that were not currently guided by visual information (Buehlmann et al. 2020a, 2018), hence, it could be that the faster turns are representative of reduced visual input during the control of ipsilateral turns in lesioned ants. Furthermore, Ridgel and co-workers showed in cockroaches that off-centre CX lesions resulted in lateralized navigational deficits (Ridgel et al. 2007). More specifically, lesioned cockroaches lost the ability to turn in the ipsilateral direction relative to the lesion (Guo and Ritzmann 2013; Ridgel et al. 2007). Furthermore, CX stimulation experiments showed evoked turning to the ipsilateral side, i.e., stimulating the left CX evoked left turns and vice versa (Guo and Ritzmann 2013; Martin et al. 2015). Our results in ants are consistent with the CX controlling steering in a lateralized way as shown in cockroaches.
It has previously been suggested that visual control of orientation in wood ants involves ‘correction points’ that are synchronised with the underlying path ‘wiggle’ or sinuosity (Collett et al. 2014; Lent et al. 2010). This suggests that there may be some independence between the control of underlying path sinuosity (perhaps from the lateral accessory lobes; (Clement et al. 2022; Steinbeck et al. 2020)) and of visual corrections. Lateralised lesions in ants might not, therefore, knock-out the control of all turns in one direction but might diminish the visual control of those turns. However, redundancy in the visual inputs that allow for innate visual orientation, mean that ants’ overall paths are still directed to the visual cue.
Data availability
The dataset generated during this study is available from the University of Sussex research repository (hosted by Figshare): https://doi.org/10.25377/sussex.22012652.
References
Aso Y, Sitaraman D, Ichinose T, Kaun KR, Vogt K, Belliart-Guérin G, Plaçais PY, Robie A, Yamagata N, Schnaitmann C, Rowell WJ, Johnston RM, Ngo T, Chen N, Korff W, Nitabach MN, Heberlein U, Preat T, Branson K, Tanimoto H, Rubin GM (2014) Mushroom body output neurons encode valence and guide memory-based action selection in Drosophila. Elife 3:e04580. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04580
Bausenwein B, Mueller NR, Heisenberg M (1994) Behavior-dependent activity labeling in the central complex of Drosophila during controlled visual stimulation. J Comp Neuro 340:255–268. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.903400210
Beetz MJ, Kraus C, Franzke M, Dreyer D, Strube-Bloss MF, Roessler W, Warrant EJ, Merlin C, el Jundi B (2022) Flight-induced compass representation in the monarch butterfly heading network. Curr Biol 32:338–349. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2021.11.009
Bender JA, Pollack AJ, Ritzmann RE (2010) Neural activity in the central complex of the insect brain is linked to locomotor changes. Curr Biol 20:921–926. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2010.03.054
Buehlmann C, Graham P (2022) Innate visual attraction in wood ants is a hardwired behavior seen across different motivational and ecological contexts. Insectes Soc 69:271–277. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00040-022-00867-3
Buehlmann C, Graham P, Hansson BS, Knaden M (2014) Desert ants locate food by combining high sensitivity to food odors with extensive crosswind runs. Curr Biol 24:960–964. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.02.056
Buehlmann C, Fernandes ASD, Graham P (2018) The interaction of path integration and terrestrial visual cues in navigating desert ants: what can we learn from path characteristics? J Exp Biol 221:jeb167304. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.167304
Buehlmann C, Aussel A, Graham P (2020a) Dynamic multimodal interactions in navigating wood ants: what do path details tell us about cue integration? J Exp Biol 223:jeb221036. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.221036
Buehlmann C, Mangan M, Graham P (2020b) Multimodal interactions in insect navigation. Anim Cogn 23:1129–1141. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-020-01383-2
Buehlmann C, Wozniak B, Goulard R, Webb B, Graham P, Niven J (2020c) Mushroom bodies are required for learnt visual navigation but not for innate visual behaviour in ants. Curr Biol 30:3438–3443. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2020.07.013
Clement L, Schwarz S, Wystrach A (2022) An intrinsic oscillator underlies visual navigation in ants. bioRxiv. https://doi.org/10.1101/2022.04.22.489150
Collett T (1988) How ladybirds approach nearby stalks: a study of visual selectivity and attention. J Comp Physiol 163:355–363. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00604011
Collett M (2010) How desert ants use a visual landmark for guidance along a habitual route. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107:11638–11643. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1001401107
Collett M, Collett TS (2000) How do insects use path integration for their navigation? Biol Cybern 83:245–259. https://doi.org/10.1007/s004220000168
Collett M, Collett TS (2018) How does the insect central complex use mushroom body output for steering? Curr Biol 28:R733–R734. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.05.060
Collett M, Chittka L, Collett T (2013) Spatial memory in insect navigation. Curr Biol 23:R789–R800. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.020
Collett TS, Lent DD, Graham P (2014) Scene perception and the visual control of travel direction in navigating wood ants. Philos Trans R Soc B 369:20130035. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0035
Dewar ADM, Wystrach A, Philippides A, Graham P (2017) Neural coding in the visual system of Drosophila melanogaster: How do small neural populations support visually guided behaviours? PLoS Comp Biol 13:e1005735. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005735
el Jundi B, Warrant EJ, Byrne MJ, Khaldy L, Baird E, Smolka J, Dacke M (2015) Neural coding underlying the cue preference for celestial orientation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 112:11395–11400. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.150127211
Fisher YE (2022) Flexible navigational computations in the Drosophila central complex. Curr Opin Neurobiol 73:102514. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2021.12.001
Franzke M, Kraus C, Gayler M, Dreyer D, Pfeiffer K, el Jundi B (2022) Stimulus-dependent orientation strategies in monarch butterflies. J Exp Biol 225:jeb243687. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.243687
Giraldo YM, Leitch KJ, Ros IG, Warren TL, Weir PT, Dickinson MH (2018) Sun navigation requires compass neurons in Drosophila. Curr Biol 28:2845–2852. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.07.002
Gotz KG (1987) Course-control, metabolism and wing interference during ultralong tethered flight in Drosophila melanogaster. J Exp Biol 128:35–46. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.128.1.35
Goulard R, Buehlmann C, Niven J, Graham P, Webb B (2021) A unified mechanism for innate and learned visual landmark guidance in the insect central complex. PLoS Comp Biol 17:e1009383. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1009383
Graham P, Philippides A (2017) Vision for navigation: what can we learn from ants? Arthropod Struct Dev 46:718–722. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.asd.2017.07.001
Graham P, Fauria K, Collett TS (2003) The influence of beacon-aiming on the routes of wood ants. J Exp Biol 206:535–541. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.00115
Green J, Vijayan V, Pires PM, Adachi A, Maimon G (2019) A neural heading estimate is compared with an internal goal to guide oriented navigation. Nat Neurosci 22:1460–1468. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0444-x
Grob R, Fleischmann PN, Grubel K, Wehner R, Roessler W (2017) The role of celestial compass information in Cataglyphis ants during learning walks and for neuroplasticity in the central complex and mushroom bodies. Front Behav Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00226
Guo PY, Ritzmann RE (2013) Neural activity in the central complex of the cockroach brain is linked to turning behaviors. J Exp Biol 216:992–1002. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.080473
Harrison JF, Fewell JH, Stiller TM, Breed MD (1989) Effects of experience on use of orientation cues in the giant tropical ant. Anim Behav 37:869–871. https://doi.org/10.1016/0003-3472(89)90076-6
Heinze S, Reppert SM (2011) Sun compass integration of skylight cues in migratory monarch butterflies. Neuron 69:345–358. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.12.025
Heinze S, Florman J, Asokaraj S, el Jundi B, Reppert SM (2013) Anatomical basis of sun compass navigation II: The neuronal composition of the central complex of the monarch butterfly. J Comp Neurol 521:267–298. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.23214
Heusser D, Wehner R (2002) The visual centring response in desert ants, Cataglyphis fortis. J Exp Biol 205:585–590. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.205.5.585
Honkanen A, Adden A, Freitas JD, Heinze S (2019) The insect central complex and the neural basis of navigational strategies. J Exp Biol 222:jeb188854. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.188854
Hulse BK, Haberkern H, Franconville R, Turner-Evans D, Takemura S, Wolff T, Noorman M, Dreher M, Dan C, Parekh R, Hermundstad GM, Jayaraman V (2021) A connectome of the Drosophila central complex reveals network motifs suitable for flexible navigation and context-dependent action selection. Elife 10:e66039. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.66039
Kamhi JF, Barron AB, Narendra A (2020) Vertical lobes of the mushroom bodies are essential for view-based navigation in Australian bull ants. Curr Biol 30:3432–3437. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2020.06.030
Knaden M, Graham P (2016) The sensory ecology of ant navigation: from natural environments to neural mechanisms. Annu Rev Entomol 61:63–76. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-ento-010715-023703
Kuntz S, Poeck B, Sokolowski MB, Strauss R (2012) The visual orientation memory of Drosophila requires Foraging (PKG) upstream of Ignorant (RSK2) in ring neurons of the central complex. Learn Memory 19:337–340. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.026369.112
Lent DD, Graham P, Collett TS (2010) Image-matching during ant navigation occurs through saccade-like body turns controlled by learned visual features. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107:16348–16353. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1006021107
Li F, Lindsey J, Marin EC, Otto N, Dreher M, Dempsey G, Stark I, Bates AS, Pleijzier MW, Schlegel P, Nern A, Takemura S, Eckstein N, Yang T, Francis A, Braun A, Parekh R, Costa M, Scheffer L, Aso Y, Jefferis G, Abbott LF, Litwin-Kumar A, Waddell S, Rubin GM (2020) The connectome of the adult Drosophila mushroom body provides insights into function. Elife 9:e62576. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.62576
Liu G, Seiler H, Wen A, Zars T, Ito K, Wolf R, Heisenberg M, Liu L (2006) Distinct memory traces for two visual features in the Drosophila brain. Nature 439:551–556. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature04381
Martin JP, Guo P, Mu L, Harley CM, Ritzmann RE (2015) Central-complex control of movement in the freely walking cockroach. Curr Biol 25:2795–2803. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.09.044
Matheson AMM, Lanz AJ, Medina AM, Licata A, Currier TA, Syed MH, Nagel KI (2022) A neural circuit for wind-guided olfactory navigation. Nat Commun 13:4613. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-022-32247-7
Mueller M, Wehner R (1988) Path integration in desert ants, Cataglyphis fortis. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 85:5287–5290. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.85.14.528
Neuser K, Triphan T, Mronz M, Poeck B, Strauss R (2008) Analysis of a spatial orientation memory in Drosophila. Nature 453:1244–1247. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature07003
Ofstad TA, Zuker CS, Reiser MB (2011) Visual place learning in Drosophila melanogaster. Nature 474:204–207. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature10131
Pegel U, Pfeiffer K, Homberg U (2018) Integration of celestial compass cues in the central complex of the locust brain. J Exp Biol 221:jeb171207. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.171207
Pfeiffer K (2022) The neuronal building blocks of the navigational toolkit in the central complex of insects. Curr Opin Insect Sci. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cois.2022.100972
Pfeiffer K, Homberg U (2014) Organization and functional roles of the central complex in the insect brain. Annu Rev Entomol 59:165–184. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-ento-011613-162031
Poteser M, Kral K (1995) Visual distance discrimination between stationary targets in praying mantids: an index of the use of motion parallax. J Exp Biol 198:2127–2137
Ridgel AL, Alexander BE, Ritzmann RE (2007) Descending control of turning behavior in the cockroach, Blaberus discoidalis. J Comp Physiol 193:385–402. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-006-0193-7
Ritzmann RE, Ridgel AL, Pollack AJ (2008) Multi-unit recording of antennal mechano-sensitive units in the central complex of the cockroach, Blaberus discoidalis. J Comp Physiol 194:341–360. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-007-0310-2
Rosner R, Homberg U (2013) Widespread sensitivity to looming stimuli and small moving objects in the central complex of an insect brain. J Neurosci 33:8122–8133. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5390-12.2013
Sakura M, Lambrinos D, Labhart T (2008) Polarized skylight navigation in insects: model and electrophysiology of e-vector coding by neurons in the central complex. J Neurophysiol 99:667–682. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00784.2007
Seelig JD, Jayaraman V (2013) Feature detection and orientation tuning in the Drosophila central complex. Nature 503:262–266. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature12601
Seelig JD, Jayaraman V (2015) Neural dynamics for landmark orientation and angular path integration. Nature 521:186–191. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature14446
Steinbeck F, Adden A, Graham P (2020) Conecting brain to behaviour: a rol for general purpose steering circuits in insect orientation? J Exp Biol 223:jeb212332. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.212332
Stone T, Webb B, Adden A, Ben Weddig N, Honkanen A, Templin R, Wcislo W, Scimeca L, Warrant E, Heinze S (2017) An anatomically constrained model for path integration in the bee brain. Curr Biol 27:3069–3085. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.08.052
Strausfeld NJ (1999) A brain region in insects that supervises walking. Prog Brain Res 123:273–284. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(08)62863-0
Strauss R, Pichler J (1998) Persistence of orientation toward a temporarily invisible landmark in Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Physiol 182:411–423. https://doi.org/10.1007/s003590050190
Sun X, Yue S, Mangan M (2020) A decentralised neural model explaining optimal integration of navigational strategies in insects. Elife 9:e54026. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.54026
Sun XL, Yue SG, Mangan M (2021) How the insect central complex could coordinate multimodal navigation. Elife 10:e73077. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.73077
Turner-Evans DB, Jayaraman V (2016) The insect central complex. Curr Biol 26:R453–R457. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.04.006
Varga AG, Kathman ND, Martin JP, Guo PY, Ritzmann RE (2017) Spatial navigation and the central complex: sensory acquisition, orientation, and motor control. Front Behav Neurosci 11:4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00004
Vitzthum H, Muller M, Homberg U (2002) Neurons of the central complex of the locust Schistocerca gregaria are sensitive to polarized light. J Neurosci 22:1114–1125. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-03-01114.2002
Voss C (1967) Ueber das Formensehen der roten Waldameise (Formica rufa Gruppe). Z Vgl Physiol 55:225–254. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00349601
Wallace GK (1962) Experiments on visually controlled orientation in the desert locust, Schistocerca gregaria (Forskål). Anim Behav 10:361–369. https://doi.org/10.1016/0003-3472(62)90059-3
Wehner R (1972) Spontaneous pattern preferences of Drosophila melanogaster to black areas in various parts of the visual field. J Insect Physiol 18:1531–1543. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1910(72)90232-6
Wehner R, Srinivasan MV (2003) Path integration in insects. In: Jeffery KJ (ed) The neurobiology of spatial behaviour. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 9–30
Wehner R, Cheng K, Cruse H (2014) Visual navigation strategies in insects: lessons from desert ants. In: Werner JS, Chalupa LM (eds) The new visual neurosciences. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 1153–1163
Wosnitza A, Martin JP, Pollack AJ, Svenson GJ, Ritzmann RE (2022) The role of central complex neurons in prey detection and tracking in the freely moving praying mantis (Tenodera sinensis). Front Neural Circ 16:893004. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2022.893004
Wystrach A, Dewar ADM, Graham P (2014) Insect vision: emergence of pattern recognition from coarse encoding. Curr Biol 24:R78–R80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.11.054
Wystrach A, Le Moel F, Clement L, Schwarz S (2020) A lateralised design for the interaction of visual memories and heading representations in navigating ants. bioRxiv. https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.08.13.249193
Young JM, Armstrong JD (2010) Structure of the adult central complex in Drosophila: organization of distinct neuronal subsets. J Comp Neurol 518:1500–1524. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.22284
Zeil J (2012) Visual homing: an insect perspective. Curr Opin Neurobiol 22:285–293. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2011.12.008
Acknowledgements
The research was funded by the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (Grant number: BB/R005036/1). CB is currently funded by a Leverhulme Trust Grant (RPG-2019-232). We thank Yan Gu for help with confocal microscopy.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
CB, SD and ADP performed the experiments. CB, RG, JEN, BW and PG wrote the manuscript and all reviewed it.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Handling editors: Uwe Homberg, Wolfgang Rössler.
Supplementary Information
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Buehlmann, C., Dell-Cronin, S., Diyalagoda Pathirannahelage, A. et al. Impact of central complex lesions on innate and learnt visual navigation in ants. J Comp Physiol A 209, 737–746 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-023-01613-1
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-023-01613-1