Abstract
Background
Endophenotypes in genetic psychiatry may increase our understanding of the molecular mechanisms underlying disease risk and its manifestations. We sought to investigate the link between neuropsychological impairments and brain structural abnormalities associated with the COMT Val(158)Met polymorphism in patients with schizophrenia to improve understanding of the pathophysiology of this disorder.Methods
We performed a systematic review using studies identified in PubMed and MEDLINE (from the date of the first available article to July 2012). Our review examined evidence of an association between the COMT Val(158)Met polymorphism and both neuropsychological performance and brain structure in patients with psychosis, in their relatives and in healthy individuals (step 1). The review also explored whether the neuropsychological tasks and brain structures identified in step 1 met the criteria for an endophenotype (step 2). Then we evaluated evidence that the neuropsychological endophenotypes identified in step 2 are associated with the brain structure endophenotypes identified in that step (step 3). Finally, we propose a neurobiological interpretation for this evidence.Results
A poorer performance on the n-back task and the Continuous Performance Test (CPT) and smaller temporal and frontal brain areas were associated with the COMT Val allele in patients with schizophrenia and their relatives and met most of the criteria for an endophenotype. It is possible that the COMT Val(158)Met polymorphism therefore contributes to the development of these neuropsychological and brain structural endophenotypes of schizophrenia, in which the prefrontal cortex may represent the neural substrate underlying both n-back and CPT performances.Limitations
The association between a single genetic variant and an endophenotype does not necessarily imply a causal relationship between them.Conclusion
This evidence and the proposed interpretation contribute to explain, at least in part, the biological substrate of 4 important endophenotypes that characterize schizophrenia.Free full text
COMT, neuropsychological function and brain structure in schizophrenia: a systematic review and neurobiological interpretation
Abstract
Background
Endophenotypes in genetic psychiatry may increase our understanding of the molecular mechanisms underlying disease risk and its manifestations. We sought to investigate the link between neuropsychological impairments and brain structural abnormalities associated with the COMT Val158Met polymorphism in patients with schizophrenia to improve understanding of the pathophysiology of this disorder.
Methods
We performed a systematic review using studies identified in PubMed and MEDLINE (from the date of the first available article to July 2012). Our review examined evidence of an association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and both neuropsychological performance and brain structure in patients with psychosis, in their relatives and in healthy individuals (step 1). The review also explored whether the neuropsychological tasks and brain structures identified in step 1 met the criteria for an endophenotype (step 2). Then we evaluated evidence that the neuropsychological endophenotypes identified in step 2 are associated with the brain structure endophenotypes identified in that step (step 3). Finally, we propose a neurobiological interpretation for this evidence.
Results
A poorer performance on the n-back task and the Continuous Performance Test (CPT) and smaller temporal and frontal brain areas were associated with the COMT Val allele in patients with schizophrenia and their relatives and met most of the criteria for an endophenotype. It is possible that the COMT Val158Met polymorphism therefore contributes to the development of these neuropsychological and brain structural endophenotypes of schizophrenia, in which the prefrontal cortex may represent the neural substrate underlying both n-back and CPT performances.
Limitations
The association between a single genetic variant and an endophenotype does not necessarily imply a causal relationship between them.
Conclusion
This evidence and the proposed interpretation contribute to explain, at least in part, the biological substrate of 4 important endophenotypes that characterize schizophrenia.
Introduction
The catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene is one of the most investigated candidate genes for schizophrenia because of its role in the degradation of dopamine (DA). This gene contains a common functional variant, the Val158Met polymorphism, which impairs the thermostability of the mature protein, altering DA levels in several brain regions and specifically in the prefrontal cortex.1,2 To date, several meta-analyses have investigated the association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and schizophrenia: although the evidence is controversial, the most recent meta-analysis points to a weak association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and schizophrenia (from the Schizophrenia Research Forum, www.szgene.org/meta.asp?geneID=420).3–6
Interestingly, the dopaminergic system is also involved in some of the neuropsychological impairments frequently described in patients with schizophrenia.7 These impairments are thought to represent the functional correlate of abnormalities in the structure of the brain areas involved in these functions (for a review, see Antonova and colleagues8). Interestingly, there is evidence that the COMT Val allele itself may be associated with abnormalities in these areas in patients with schizophrenia.9–11
The COMT Val158Met polymorphism may therefore contribute to the risk for schizophrenia by influencing specific neuropsychological impairments and brain structural abnormalities, which have been proposed as endophenotypes for this disorder.12,13 Endophenotypes are heritable traits that possibly represent a causal link between genes and observable phenotypes.14 They have the advantage of being measurable using strategies that involve quantitative units of analysis, and of being amenable to assessment in the laboratory.15 Furthermore, the association between a specific endophenotype and certain genetic variants could be stronger than that with the illness itself.14 According to Gottesman and Gould,14 an endophenotype should
be associated with the illness in the population of interest,
be heritable,
be primarily state independent,
cosegregate with the illness within families, and
be present in nonaffected family members more frequently than in the general population.
Endophenotypes in genetic psychiatry may increase our understanding of the molecular mechanisms underlying disease risk and its manifestations. Thus, investigating the link between neuropsychological impairments and brain structural abnormalities associated with COMT Val158Met in schizophrenia may provide a better understanding of the pathophysiology of this disorder.
Although many studies have investigated the relationship between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and neuropsychological impairments or brain structural alterations in patients with schizophrenia, in their relatives and in healthy individuals, to our knowledge, no systematic review has critically appraised this evidence and explored the potential relationship between this polymorphism and neuropsychological impairments or the brain structural alterations. We sought to fill this gap with the present systematic review.
Methods
A flow chart of the methodology is presented in Figure 1. In step 1, we searched the PubMed and MEDLINE databases, from the date of the first available article to July 2012, to systematically identify the neuropsychological tasks and brain structural variations reported, as related to the COMT Val158Met polymorphism, across disorders in the psychosis spectrum. To investigate the association between COMT and neuropsychological tasks, we used the terms “catechol-O-methyltransferase” or “COMT” AND “cognitive” or “cognition” or “neuropsychological” or “neuropsychology” or “attention” or “memory” or “executive function” AND “schizophrenia” or “psychosis” or “schizoaffective” or “schizophreniform” or “healthy.” To investigate the association between COMT and brain structure we used the terms “catechol-O-methyltransferase” or “COMT”, AND “MRI” or “magnetic resonance imaging” or “imaging” or “voxel” or “brain structures” AND “schizophrenia” or “psychosis” or “schizoaffective” or “schizophreniform” or “healthy.” We also manually searched the reference lists of the articles identified in our search. Unpublished studies, conference abstracts and poster presentations were not included. One of us (E.I.) selected the articles, which 2 of us (P.D. and S.T.) then checked against our inclusion criteria.
We included studies that fulfilled the following criteria. First, studies must have evaluated the association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and neuropsychological tasks in patients with diagnosed schizophrenia-spectrum disorders (i.e., schizophrenia, schizophreniform disorder, schizoaffective disorder) and/or in healthy controls and/or in relatives of patients with psychosis. We included only studies conducted using a standardized cognitive measure in adult participants. Behavioural data from functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies were also included when reported. Second, studies must have evaluated the association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and brain volumes using structural MRI in patients with diagnosed schizophrenia-spectrum disorders and/or in healthy controls and/or in relatives of patients with psychosis.
From the studies reviewed, we selected neuropsychological tasks and brain structures strongly associated with the COMT Val158Met polymorphism according to the following criteria. First, at least 3 studies must have evaluated the association between COMT and those neuropsychological tasks or brain structures in either patients or healthy controls. Second, at least 50% of these studies must have reported an association in patients.
In step 2, we considered each neuropsychological task and brain structure identified in step 1 to establish whether they satisfied the criteria for endophenotypes.
In step 3, we systematically searched the PubMed and MEDLINE databases, from the date of the first available article to July 2012, to verify whether the neuropsychological and the brain structural endophenotypes identified in step 2 were associated with each other. We used the name of each neuropsychological task as a search term combined with the terms “MRI” or “magnetic resonance imaging” or “imaging” or “voxel” or “brain structures” AND “schizophrenia” or “psychosis” or “schizoaffective” or “schizophreniform” or “healthy.” In addition, we manually searched the reference lists of the articles identified in our search. Unpublished studies, conference abstracts and poster presentations were not included.
Results
Step 1a: association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and neuropsychological tests
We identified 53 studies (Table 1) that explored the association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and various neuropsychological tests. We present the neuropsychological tests that satisfied our first criterion (i.e., at least 3 studies evaluated the association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and those neuropsychological tests in either patients or healthy controls).
Table 1
Study | No. patients | Diagnosis | No. controls (relatives) | Cognitive measures | Main results* |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Green et al.16 | — | 160 | MSIT, WASI (vocabulary, similarities, block design, matrix reasoning) |
| |
Gong et al.17 | — | 700 | Digital and spatial working memory spans | No association between COMT and digital and spatial working memory spans | |
Blanchard et al.18 | — | 291 | Spatial n-back task | No association between COMT and n-back task | |
Greenwood et al.19 | 87 | Schizophrenia | — | WCST, WAIS-R (digit span) |
|
Wishart et al.20 | — | 95 | TMT A–B, D-KEFS (trail-making subtest) |
| |
Stokes et al.21 | — | 50 | Spatial n-back, go/no-go, Tower of London tasks | No association between COMT and any cognitive measure | |
Rosa et al.22 | 67 | Schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder | 186 | Modified Stroop test (manipulating level of required cognitive stability) |
|
Solís-Ortiz et al.23 | — | 74 F | WCST, Stroop test, CPT (test single and AX task), verbal fluency test |
| |
Uçok et al.24 | 99 | Schizophrenia | — | WCST, CPT (ZA task) |
|
Wilkosc et al.25 | — | 200 | WCST | WCST (percentage of nonperseverative errors): Val/Val Val/met ↓ (in males only) | |
Wirgenes et al.26 | 315 | Schizophrenia-spectrum disorders
| 340 | Bergen n-back task, digit symbol coding test, CVLT, D-KEFS (colour-word interference test, verbal fluency test) |
|
Pomarol-Clotet et al.27 | 42 | Schizophrenia | 31 | N-back task (sequential letter version) | No association between COMT and n-back task |
Dennis et al.28 | — | 496 to 1218 | CANTAB (paired associates learning, spatial working memory, verbal recognition memory—immediate recall, ID/ED, rapid visual processing, spatial span, spatial recognition memory), Green’s story recall (immediate and delayed), TMT A–B, WAIS-III (forward and backward digit span, digit symbol, symbol search), COWAT, semantic fluency, Stroop colour-word interference | No association between COMT and any cognitive measures | |
Van den Bos et al.29 | — | 70 | IGT | Val/Val ↑ | |
Yue et al.30 | — | 21 | N-back task (sequential number version) | Val/Met ↓, Val/Val ↑ | |
Krug et al.31 | — | 80 | Verbal fluency | No association between COMT and verbal fluency | |
Neuhaus et al.32 | 111 | Schizophrenia | — | CPT-IP | Val/Val ↑ |
Prata et al.33 | 42 | Schizophrenia | 48 | Verbal fluency task |
|
Schmack et al.34 | — | 44 | Monetary incentive delay | No association between COMT and monetary incentive delay | |
Sheldrick et al.35 | — | 522 | D2 Test of Attention, MWT-B, LNS, TMT B, verbal fluency, Wechsler Memory Scale (spatial span) |
| |
Roussos et al.36 | — | 107 | SoC, IGT |
| |
Roffman et al.37 | 185 | Chronic schizophrenia | — | WCST |
|
Opgen-Rhein et al.38 | 63 | Schizophrenia | 40 | ANT | Schizophranie–controls: Val/Val ValMet ↓ reaction time and ↑ conflict effect scores |
Mata et al.39 | 130 | First-episode nonaffective psychosis | — |
| No association between COMT and any cognitive domain |
Diaz-Asper et al.40 | 325 | Schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder | 330 (359) | Spatial n-back task, WCST, CPT 1–9, WAIS-R (short form: arithmetic, similarities, picture completion and digit symbol), ID/ED | Patients, controls, relatives:
|
Bertolino et al.41 | — | 82 | Spatial n-back task, recognition memory paradigm | No association between COMT and any cognitive measures | |
Aguilera et al.42 | — | 521 | WCST, CPT-IP, WMS-R (backward visual span), WAIS III (LNS) |
| |
Woodward et al.43 | 86 | Schizophrenia | — | ACTT, WCST, WISC-R (mazes), BSRT, CIGT, COWAT, WAIS-R (digit symbol) |
|
MacDonald et al.44 | — | 464 | N-back task (sequential letter version), CPT (AX/DPX tasks) |
| |
Ehlis et al.45 | 56 | Schizophrenia-spectrum disorders | — | Stroop test, TMT A–B, verbal fluency test |
|
Dickerson et al.46 | 364 | Schizophrenia | — | RBANS: immediate memory (list learning and story memory tasks), visuospatial/constructional (figure copy and line orientation tasks), language (picture naming and semantic fluency tasks), attention (digit span and coding tasks) and delayed memory (list recall, story recall, figure recall and list recognition tasks) | No association between COMT and any of the RBANS index |
Caldú et al.47 | — | 75 | WCST, Conners’ CPT II, n-back task, WAIS (vocabulary) |
| |
Barnett et al.48 | 822 | Schizophrenia-spectrum disorders | 1088 | WCST |
|
Szöke et al.49 | 66 | Schizophrenia | 50 (57 SCZ) | TMT A–B, WCST | Patients, controls, relatives: no association between COMT and any cognitive measure |
Rybakowski et al.50 | 79 | Schizophrenia | — | WCST |
|
Krabbendam et al.51 | 23 | Schizophrenia | 21 (33) | CPT (Flanker version) | Patients, controls, relatives: Val/Val ↓ (correct responses) |
Han et al.52 | 132M | First-episode schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder | — | WAIS (information, digit span, vocabulary. arithmetic comprehension, similarity, picture, picture arrangement, block design, object assembly, digit symbol) |
|
Golimbet et al.53 | 124 | Schizophrenia | 116 (79) | Long-term memory, short-term memory, verbal fluency, selectivity of speech relations, calculating with switching over | No association between COMT and any cognitive measure |
Bertolino et al.54 | — | 27 | Recognition memory paradigm | Val/Val ↓ (accuracy at retrieval) | |
Stefanis et al.55 | — | 527 | CPT-IP version | Val/Val Val/Met: higher reaction time variability | |
Ho et al.56 | 159 | Schizophrenia | 84 | WCST, digit span backward, TMT A–B | No association between COMT and any cognitive measure |
Galderisi et al.57 | 106 | Schizophrenia | — | CPT (AX task), WCST | CPT (AX errors) and WSCT (perseverative errors): Val/Val ↓ |
Bruder et al.58 | — | 402 | SDR, WSPT, N-back (sequential letter version), LNS, WCST (n = 246) |
| |
Blasi et al.59 | — | 23 | Variable attentional control task | Val/Val Val/Met ↓ | |
Rosa et al.60 | 89 | Schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, psychotic mood disorder, schizophreniform disorder, brief psychotic disorder, delusional disorder, atypical psychosis | (89) | WCST |
|
Nolan et al.61 | 26 | Schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder | — | Competing programs task | Val/Val Val/Met ↓ cognitive stability and ↑ cognitive flexibility |
de Frias et al.62 | — | 286 M | Episodic memory (recall test, recognition test), semantic memory (knowledge test, fluency test) |
| |
Tsai et al.63 | — | 120 F | WCST | No association between COMT and WCST (perseverative errors) | |
Goldberg et al.64 | 74 | Schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder (depressed type) | 68 (108) | Spatial n-back task, CPT 1–9, WAIS-R (IQ similarities, arithmetic, picture completion and digit symbol) |
|
Malhotra et al.65 | — | 73 | WCST | WCST (perseverative errors): Val/Val Val/Met ↓ | |
Joober et al.66 | 94 | Schizophrenia | 31 | WCST |
|
Bilder1 | 58 | Schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder | — |
| Processing speed and attention domain: Val/Val ↓ |
Egan et al.67 | 175 | Schizophrenia | 55 (219) | WCST, WAIS-R, WRAT | Patients, controls, relatives: WSCT (perseverative errors): Val/Val ↓ |
ACTT = Auditory Consonant Trigram Test; ANT = Attention Network Test; BSRT = Buschke Selective Reminding Test; CANTAB = Cambridge Neuropsyhological Test Automated Battery; CIGT = Category Instance Generation Test; COMT = catechol-O-methyltransferase; COWAT = Controlled Oral Word Association Test; CPT = Continuous Performance Test; CPT-IP = Continuous Performance Test, Identical Pairs version; CVLT = California Verbal Learning Test; DAT = dopamine transporter; D-KEFS = Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System; F = female; DPX = Dot Pattern Expectancy task; ID/ED = intradimensional/extradimensional shift task; IGT = Iowa Gambling Task; LNS = letter and number sequencing; M = male; MSIT = Multi-Source Interference Task; MWT-B = Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest B; RBANS = Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status; SDR = Spatial Delayed Response; SoC = Stockings of Cambridge; TMT = Trail Making Test; WAIS = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; WAIS III = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III; WAIS-R = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised; WASI = Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence; WCST = Wisconsin Card sorting Test; WISC-R = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised; WMS-R = Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised; WRAT = Wide Range Achievement Test; WSPT = Word Serial Position Test.
N-back task
The n-back task is conceptualized as a measure of working memory and includes executive functions, such as encoding, updating, sequence maintenance, comparing and deciding.44 Eleven studies satisfied our inclusion criteria and together suggest that while the COMT Val158Met polymorphism seems to be associated with performance on the n-back task in patients with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder and in their siblings, this association is not present in healthy individuals. Three studies resported a significant or trend-level association between the Val/Val genotype and a worse performance (accuracy and reaction time) on the n-back task in patients with schizophrenia, schizophreniform disorder and schizoaffective disorder26,40,64 and in their relatives,40,64 whereas 1 study found no association.27 Interestingly, the 3 studies that detected a significant association used a version of the n-back task that requires a higher cognitive load, suggesting that the association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and performance on the n-back task in patients with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders may become apparent when a more challenging performance is required (for details about the different versions of the tasks used, see Appendix 1, Table S1, available at cma.ca/jpn). In healthy individuals, 3 studies found a significant association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and performance on the n-back task.30,40,64 In 2 of these studies the Val/Val genotype was associated with worse performance,40,64 whereas in the third one participants with the Val/Met genotype performed worse than those with the Met/Met or Val/Val genotype.30 Eight studies did not find a COMT Val158Met genotype effect on n-back task performance in healthy individuals.18,21,26,27,41,44,47,58
Continuous Performance Test (CPT)
The CPT is conceptualized as a behavioural assessment of attentional modulation; it can detect deficits in vigilance, selective attention and sustained attention.68 Ten studies satisfied our inclusion criteria, reporting inconsistent findings on the association between the CPT and the COMT Val158Met polymorphism in both patients with schizophrenia and healthy individuals. In patients, 3 studies found a significant association between the Val/Val genotype and a poorer performance in terms of number of errors57,69 and a better performance in terms of signal discrimination.32 In contrast, 2 studies found no association either in patients with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder or in their relatives.40,64 In healthy individuals, the Val/Val and Val/Met genotypes have been associated with worse performance in terms of commission errors,47 context processing44 and reaction time variability in some studies,55 whereas other studies reported no association.23,40,42,64 This inconsistency may be related to the use of different versions of the CPT across studies (for details about the different versions of the tasks used, see Appendix 1, Table S1). Interestingly, 1 study reported an interaction between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and the DAT 40 bp variable number tandem repeat; participants homozygous for both the COMT Val allele and the DAT 10 allele exhibited worse performance in terms of commission errors.47 This result suggests that the COMT Val158Met polymorphism may affect CPT performance by interacting with other genes, which may also explain inconsistencies in previous findings.
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST)
The WCST is regarded as a neuropsychological marker of the efficiency of executive functions.70 While the COMT gene appears to have no effect on WCST performance in patients with psychosis, it seems to have an effect in healthy individuals. A meta-analysis has been conducted to assess the effect of the COMT Val158Met polymorphism on WCST perseverative errors in patients with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders and in healthy controls;48 this included 11 studies that met our inclusion criteria.1,49,50,56–58,60,63,65–67 The meta-analysis revealed a small but significant association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and executive function in healthy individuals, with Met/Met carriers performing slightly better than Val/Val carriers. However, this association was not found in patients with schizophrenia.
Subsequent studies have partially confirmed these findings, reporting an association between Val/Val and Val/Met genotypes and a higher number of perseverative errors47 and nonperseverative errors25 and fewer commission errors23 in healthy individuals than in patients with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder.24,37,40 In contrast, other studies found the Val/Val genotype to be associated with worse performance in terms of perseverative errors43 and found the Val/Met genotype to be associated with lower number of categories achieved19 in patients with schizophrenia but not in healthy individuals42 or unaffected relatives.40 Moreover, an interaction between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and the MTHFR C677T mutation on WCST performance has been found in patients with schizophrenia, with COMT Val/Val carriers who had at least 1 copy of the MTHFR T allele making more perseverative errors.37
Trail Making Test (TMT) A and B
The TMT-A requires sequencing, psychomotor speed and visuoperceptual ability; part B also requires cognitive flexibility and working memory.71,72 Performance on the TMT-A has not been found to be associated with the COMT Val158Met polymorphism in patients with schizophrenia45,49,56 or healthy individuals.20,28,56 Worse performance on the TMT-B has been found to be associated with Val/Val and Val/Met genotypes in healthy individuals in 1 study20 but not in others,28,56 nor in those evaluating patients with schizophrenia.45,49,56
Interestingly, an association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and worse TMT-B performance has been reported in healthy individuals carrying both the COMT Val allele and the ANKK1 (rs1800497) T allele.20
Verbal fluency
Verbal fluency is considered to be an executive function task and a semantic memory measure.53 Both Val/Val and Val/Met carriers with schizophrenia have been reported to perform worse in 1 study,33 but this association has not been confirmed by other studies involving patients with schizophrenia,26,53 their relatives53 or healthy controls.23,28,33,53,62,73
Digit span
Composed by 2 parts, forward and backward, the digit span test is considered to be a short-term memory measure, with the digit span backward also involving working memory.56 A worse performance on digit span has been found to be related to the Val/Val genotype in patients with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder in 1 study,52 but not in another study involving patients with schizophrenia.19 Of note, the sample size of the former study52 was considerably larger than that in the latter study.19 Only the digit span backward has not been found to be associated with the COMT Val158Met polymorphism in patients with schizophrenia.74
According to our criteria, only the n-back task and the CPT can be considered strongly related to the COMT Val158Met polymorphism in patients with schizophrenia. In healthy controls, these associations seem to be weaker or absent. Interestingly, some fMRI studies that used an n-back paradigm detected a significant association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and the activation of the frontal cortex in patients with schizophrenia26 and healthy controls21,26,41,75 regardless of n-back task performance. Thus, fMRI may be a more sensitive approach to the evaluation of the neuronal circuitry affected by variation in the COMT gene and n-back task performance and when cognitive performance is not affected.
The n-back task and the CPT assess working memory and attention modulation, respectively. Interestingly, although the evidence available is not yet sufficient to make them satisfy our criteria of performance in other working memory tasks (e.g., letter and number sequencing, Auditory Consonant Trigram Test) and in tasks of attentional modulation (e.g., Attention Network Test, Variable Attentional Control Task), these tasks seem to be associated with the COMT Val158Met polymorphism in patients with schizophrenia. On the other hand, verbal fluency and digit span backward have not been found to be associated with the COMT Val158Met polymorphism in patients with schizophrenia or in healthy controls. However, this result may reflect the lack of association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and semantic and short-term memory, respectively, which are both required for performing these tasks.
The WCST, which assesses executive function, has been related to the COMT Val158Met polymorphism in healthy controls but not in patients with psychosis.48 It is plausible that other COMT polymorphisms or other genetic variants or nongenetic factors might influence the performance on this task in individuals with psychosis.
Step 1b: Association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and brain structure
We identified 13 studies (Table 2) exploring the association between brain structure (global or regional volumes of grey and white matter) and the COMT Val158Met polymorphism. We report the regions that satisfied the first criterion (i.e., at least 3 studies evaluated the association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and those brain areas in either patients or healthy controls).
Table 2
Study | No. patients | Diagnosis | No. controls (relatives) | MRI analysis (Tesla) | Regions evaluated | Main results |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Barnes et al.76 | — | 82 | Automated (1.5 T) | Total grey matter volume, DLPFC | No association | |
Cerasa et al.77 | — | 149 | Automated (3 T) | Whole brain | Val/Val ↓ thickness of the right inferior prefrontal sulcus and right superior temporal sulcus | |
Ehrlich et al.9 | 98 | Schizophrenia | 114 | Automated (3 T and 1.5 T) | Whole brain, frontal lobe, hippocampal and amygdala volumes | Patients: Val/Val Val/Met ↓ bilateral amygdala and right hippocampal volumes; no association between COMT and whole brain and frontal regions Controls: Val/Val Val/Met ↓ bilateral amygdalae and right hippocampal volumes; no association between COMT and whole brain and frontal regions |
Honea et al.78 | — | 151 | Automated VBM/ROI (1.5 T) | Whole brain, DLPFC, hippocampal volumes | Val/Val Val/Met ↓ grey matter volume in hippocampus and parahippocampal gyrus; Val/Val ↑ (trend) grey matter volume in DLPFC; no association between COMT and whole brain | |
Dutt et al.79 | 128 | Schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, schizoaffective disorder, psychotic disorder NOS | 61 (194) | Automated (1.5 T) | Left hippocampal volume, right hippocampal volume and total lateral ventricular volume | No association |
Zinkstok et al.80 | 51 | Schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder | — | Automated VBM (1.5 T) | Regional grey and white matter density and total grey and white matter volume | No association between COMT and grey or white matter density; COMT x PRODH rs20086720: Val allele T allele ↑ white matter density in the left inferior frontal lobe |
Cerasa et al.81 | — | 57 | Automated VBM (1.5 T) | Hippocampus and prefrontal cortex volumes | Val/Val ↓ hippocampal grey matter volumes bilaterally; Val/Val Val/Met ↑ prefrontal cortex greater grey matter volume bilaterally and ↓ tissue volume of the hippocampus bilaterally | |
Taylor et al.82 | — | 31 | Automated (1.5 T) | Whole brain, hippcampus, amygdala, caudate and temporal lobe volumes | Val/Val ↓ temporal lobe (total and grey matter) and hippocampus volumes; no association between COMT and whole brain, caudate and amygdala volumes. | |
McIntosh et al.10 | — | High-risk for schizophrenia | 15 (78) | Automated VBM (1 T) | Prefrontal cortex | Val/Val Val/Met ↓ anterior cingulate cortex grey matter density |
Crespo-Facorro et al.83 | 75 | Schizophrenia, schizophreniform disorder, schizoaffective disorder, psychosis NOS, brief reactive psychosis | — | Automated (1.5 T) | Global and lobar volumes of grey matter and CSF | Met/Met Val/Met ↑ lateral ventricles, right lateral ventricle and left lateral ventricle |
Zinkstok et al.84 | — | 154 | Automated VBM (1.5 T) | Total and regional grey matter and white matter density | No association between COMT and grey or white matter volumes and densities | |
Ohnishi et al.11 | 47 | Schizophrenia | 76 | Automated TBM/whole brain and ROI (1.5 T) | Whole brain morphology, DLPFC | Controls: no association between COMT and any brain structure Patients: Val/Val ↓ left amygdala-uncus, bilateral anterior cingulate cortex, right middle temporal gyrus, left thalamus; no association between COMT and DLPFC |
Ho et al.56 | 100 | Schizophrenia | 49 | Automated (1.5 T) | Frontal lobe grey matter, white matter and CSF volumes | Controls: no association between COMT and frontal lobe morphology Patients: no association between COMT and frontal lobe volume |
COMT = catechol-O-methyltransferase; CSF = cerebrospinal fluid; DLPFC = dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; MRI = magnetic resonance imaging; NOS = not otherwise specified; ROI = region of interest; TBM = tensor-based morphometry; VBM = voxel-based morphometry.
Temporal regions
The most consistent finding has been that of an association with temporal areas. In patients with schizophrenia, a smaller volume of the hippocampus,9 amygdala-uncus9,11 and middle temporal gyrus11 has been associated with the Val/Val and Val/Met genotypes. Interestingly, this association has not been found in studies that included patients with any psychosis,79,80,83 indicating that the association may be specific to schizophrenia. In healthy individuals, smaller hippocampal,9,78,81,82 amygdalar,9 total and grey matter temporal lobe82 volumes as well as reduced thickness of the right superior temporal sulcus77 have been reported in Val/Val carriers. In contrast, other studies have failed to find a significant association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and temporal lobe volumes in healthy individuals or in unaffected relatives of patients with psychosis.11,79,84
Frontal areas
The Val/Val genotype has been associated with smaller grey matter density in the anterior cingulate cortex in a population at high risk for psychosis10 and with smaller grey matter volume in the left anterior cingulate cortex in patients with chronic schizophrenia.11 However, these findings have not been replicated by other studies.9,56,83 Interestingly, carriers of both the COMT Val allele and the PRODH GT or TT genotypes for rs20086720 have been found to have larger white matter volume of the inferior frontal area.80 This may suggest that the COMT Val158Met polymorphism affects frontal region morphology via an interaction with other genes, which may help explain the inconsistency found in the association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and frontal volumes. In healthy individuals, both the Val/Val and Val/Met genotypes have been associated with greater grey matter volume of the prefrontal,81 and (albeit at trend level) dorsolateral prefrontal cortex78 and with a reduced thickness of the right inferior prefrontal sulcus;77 however, these findings have not been confirmed by other studies.9,11,56,76,84
Lateral ventricles
In patients with first-episode schizophrenia with nonaffective psychoses, a significant enlargement of lateral ventricles has been reported in both Met/Met and Val/Met carriers.83 This finding has not been replicated in studies involving patients with any psychosis,11,79 in unaffected relatives79 or in healthy individuals.11,79,84
Thalamus
A reduced volume of the left thalamus has been associated with the COMT Val allele in patients with chronic schizophrenia,11 but not in healthy controls.11,84 Subsequent studies have not confirmed this association in patients.80,83
According to our criteria, only temporal and frontal volumes can be considered to correlate with the COMT Val158Met polymorphism in schizophrenia, with temporal volumes also associated with this genotype in healthy controls. Interestingly, the association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and temporal and frontal areas has also been explored in fMRI studies. The Val allele has been reported to be associated with a reduced activation of the medial temporal lobe during an episodic memory task in healthy controls,54,85 whereas in schizophrenia the Met allele has been reported to be associated with a reduced activation in this area.86 Moreover, a recent meta-analysis87 that evaluated 20 studies involving mostly healthy individuals showed a significant association between the Val allele and increased prefrontal activation. This finding has also been reported in patients with schizophrenia and in their relatives.27,67
Step 2a: Can the neuropsychological tasks identified (n-back and CPT) be considered endophenotypes?
N-back task
Association with the illness: Several studies suggest that patients with schizophrenia have worse n-back accuracy during high working memory load conditions and longer reaction time.40,64,88
Heritability: The n-back heritability seems high, at 72.9% for accuracy and 56.4% for reaction time, as found in healthy twins.89
State independence: Impairment in the n-back task has been reported in patients with schizophrenia at various illness stages,90–92 before and after antipsychotic treatment92 and independent of illness duration.91
Cosegregation with illness within family: Cosegregation is suggested by evidence that this impairment is present in relatives of patients with schizophrenia, who perform better than probands.40,64,88,93 Moreover, a significant influence of familial loading on visuospatial working memory has been detected in patients with schizophrenia but not in patients with schizoaffective disorder.94
Presence in unaffected relatives: With the exception of 2 studies,95,96 n-back performance has been found to be impaired in unaffected relatives of patients with schizophrenia in comparison to healthy controls.40,64,88,93
Continuous Performance Test
Association with the illness: Schizophrenia has been consistently associated with impairment in CPT discrimination accuracy and reaction time.12,40,64,97,98
Heritability: The heritability for d′ of the CPT, Identical Pairs version (CPT-IP), has been estimated at around 39% for verbal and 49% for spatial attention.99 For the X-CPT, the heritability has been reported to vary between 48% and 62%.100,101
State independence: It has been reported that at-risk offspring in whom a schizophrenia-spectrum disorder later developed had attentional deficits as early as age 12 years, with the impairment persisting over the following 15 years.102
Cosegregation with illness within family: CPT impairment may cosegregate with psychosis, as suggested by its presence among family members of patients with schizophrenia.12 Moreover, there is evidence that nonpsychotic siblings from multiplex families exhibit worse performance on a degraded CPT version as well as less proficiency in processing the perceptual load when compared with those from simplex families.103
Presence in unaffected relatives: Unaffected relatives of patients with schizophrenia show impairment in discrimination accuracy at the CPT-IP.12 However, studies that used different versions of the CPT have not reported impairment in discrimination accuracy.40,64 Relatives of patients with schizophrenia have also been found to have an impairment in hit reaction time in comparison to healthy controls.98
Both the n-back and the CPT can be considered useful endophenotypes for schizophrenia.
Step 2b: Can the previously identified brain structures (temporal and frontal areas) be considered endophenotypes?
Temporal areas
Association with the illness: Meta-analyses suggest that a reduction of total and medial temporal (amygdala–uncus–hyppocampus) volumes is present in patients with schizophrenia, particularly in the hippocampus.104–110
Heritability: A moderate heritability (40%–69%) for hippocampal volume has been reported in twin studies in healthy controls.111
State independence: Hippocampus reductions seem to be state-independent and have been reported in patients at both first episode and chronic illness stages.112
Cosegregation with illness within the family: Cosegregation is suggested by the presence of hippocampal volume reduction among relatives of patients with schizophrenia,113 particularly in multiplex families.114
Presence in unaffected relatives: Results from a meta-analysis indicate that amygdalar and hippocampal volume reductions are also present in relatives of patients with schizophrenia.113
Frontal areas
Association with the illness: Various meta-analyses have consistently reported volume reductions in medial prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices104–106,115 in patients with schizophrenia.
Heritability: The heritability of total frontal lobe volumes seems to be high (90%–95%); changes in ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate gyrus, superior frontal cortex and anterior cingulate volumes have also been found to be highly heritable.111 Moreover, a twin study has reported an association between smaller prefrontal grey matter volume and increasing genetic proximity to a patient with schizophrenia (monozygotic co-twins > dizygotic co-twins > control twins).116
State independence: Decreased prefrontal and cingulate grey matter volumes have been reported in high risk individuals who subsequently transition to psychosis117,118 as well as in patients with first-episode and chronic schizophrenia.104,110,115 Reductions in some regions of the frontal cortex seem to be a feature of high-risk people in whom schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder subsequently develops.119
Cosegregation with illness within family: Cosegregation of the smaller prefrontal cortex within schizophrenia families is suggested by evidence of smaller volumes in this region in family members, albeit to a lesser extent than in affected relatives.120,121 Moreover, there is evidence of a negative correlation between grey matter volume and increasing genetic risk for schizophrenia.122
Presence in unaffected relatives: Relatives of patients with schizophrenia show grey matter volume reductions in the total frontal lobe,120 prefrontal cortex123 and anterior cingulate gyrus110,115,121 in comparison to healthy individuals. However, this finding has not been replicated in other studies.124,125
Reduced grey matter volume in the medial temporal and prefrontal cortices can be considered a useful endophenotype for schizophrenia.
Step 3: Are the previously identified neuropsychological endophenotypes associated with these brain structural endophenotypes?
Six studies (Table 3) have examined the association between the n-back and CPT and the brain structural endophenotypes we identified in step 2 (grey matter volume of medial temporal and prefrontal cortices).
Table 3
Study | No. patients | Diagnosis | No. controls (relatives) | MRI analysis (Tesla) | Regions evaluated | Cognitive measures | Main results* |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Piras et al.130 | — | — | 181 | ROI (3 T) | Caudate, putamen, pallidum, thalamus, hippocampus and amygdala | N-back task | No association |
Crespo-Facorro et al.136 | 142 | Psychosis
| 83 | Automated segmentation global volumes and ROIs (1.5 T) | Volumes of whole brain, total grey matter, total white matter, cortical CSF and lateral ventricles, grey matter volumes of cortical (occipital, parietal, temporal and frontal lobes) and subcortical (caudate nucleus, thalamus and putamen) regions | CPT | Controls: ↑ CPT, ↓ lateral ventricle volume Patients: no association |
Laywer et al.135 | 71 | Schizophrenia | 65 | Semiautomated and fully manual (1.5 T) | Frontal, occipital, parietal, temporal and occipital lobes, subcortical region, CSF volume of the ventricles I, II and III, intracranial volume, corpus callosum, caudate, putamen, hippocampus, cerebellum, the posterior superior, posterior inferior and anterior vermis, and cerebellar tonsil | CPT | Controls/patients: association with putamen volumes and vermis regions |
Salgado-Pineda et al.134 | 13 | Antipsychotic-naive schizophrenia | 13 | Automated VBM and ROI (1.5 T) | Whole brain grey matter, white matter and CSF, thalamus | CPT | Controls: no association Patients: ↑ CPT (d′ scores), ↑ grey matter density in the left thalamic nucleus, left angular, supramarginal gyrus, left inferior frontal and postcentral gyri, grey matter density of the left and right thalamus |
Antonova et al.8 | 45 | Schizophrenia | 43 | Automated VBM (1.5 T) | Whole brain volume and grey matter | CPT | Controls/patients: no association |
Goldberg et al.137 | 14 | Schizophrenia | (14 co-twins) | Automated (1.5 T) | Hippocampus, third ventricle and a large section of the lateral ventricles | CPT | Patients/relatives: no association |
CPT = Continuous Performance Test; CSF = cerebrospinal fluid; MRI = magnetic resonance imaging; ROI = region of interest; VBM = voxel-based morphometry.
N-back task
Although the association between n-back task and prefrontal brain function has been frequently explored with fMRI in patients with schizophrenia88,126–129 and in their relatives,95 the association between this test and prefrontal (or temporal) volume in patients with schizophrenia has not been explored. In healthy controls, only 1 study has been conducted, reporting no association between n-back performance and hippocampal and amygdalar volume.130 However, reduced prefrontal activation during the n-back task has been associated with a reduction in prefrontal volume in people at ultra-high risk for psychosis, suggesting that “these 2 fundamental pathophysiological features of the disorder are interrelated.”117 Moreover, 2 studies that adopted working memory tasks similar to the n-back task (the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale arithmetic subtest and digit span backward, respectively) reported a association between worse working memory performance and reduced frontal, prefrontal and temporal lobe volumes in patients with schizophrenia.131,132 Taken together, these data suggest that worse n-back performance is associated with reduced prefrontal and temporal lobe volumes. Interestingly, an impaired performance on the n-back task has also been described in individuals with focal prefrontal damage,133 suggesting that this deficit may be shared by other disorders that primarily affect the prefrontal cortex.
Continuous Performance Test
A worse CPT performance has been associated with smaller prefrontal volume in patients with schizophrenia,134 but not in studies that included patients with any psychosis8,135,136 or healthy individuals.8,134–136 Studies in patients with schizophrenia, their unaffected co-twins and healthy controls have found no association between CPT performance and hippocampal volume.8,134–137
These findings suggest that worse performance on the n-back task is associated with smaller prefrontal and medial temporal volumes and that worse CPT performance is associated with smaller prefrontal volume in patients with schizophrenia. Consistent with this evidence, a meta-analysis of fMRI studies reported that the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the anterior cingulate cortex were less activated during the n-back task in patients with schizophrenia.128 On the other hand, prefrontal cortex activation has been found to be associated with the CPT in patients with schizophrenia138–140 and in their relatives.141
Discussion
To our knowledge, this is the first systematic review to comprehensively evaluate evidence on the link between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and neuropsychological performance and brain structures in patients with schizophrenia, in their relatives and in healthy controls. We found that the n-back task and the CPT (a working memory task and an attentional modulation task, respectively) are the only 2 neuropsychological tasks strongly associated with the COMT Val158Met polymorphism in patients with schizophrenia. More specifically, among patients, the Val/Val carriers showed a worse performance in both the n-back task and the CPT, with a greater number of errors, but a better performance in signal discrimination. Interestingly, evidence from fMRI studies suggests that performance on these 2 tasks is positively correlated with lower prefrontal cortex activation in patients with schizophrenia. This suggests that the cognitive domains assessed by these tasks reflect “the operation of a common underlying cognitive control mechanism supported by the prefrontal cortex.”142 Moreover, this evidence is consistent with the tonic–phasic DA hypothesis,143 which suggests that tonic DA may stabilize and maintain relevant information (“cognitive stability”) principally via D1 receptors, while phasic DA is important for updating and manipulating information (“cognitive flexibility”), principally via D2 receptors.144 In the COMT Val158Met polymorphism, while the Val allele increases phasic DA transmission and is expected to improve cognitive flexibility, the Met158 allele increases tonic DA transmission, and is therefore expected to improve working memory and executive functions that require cognitive stability.145 Furthermore, since some neuropsychological tests require both cognitive stability and cognitive flexibility,145 the DA hypothesis may help explain the inconsistency in findings regarding the association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and neuropsychological performance.
With respect to brain structure, the evidence reviewed points to an association between smaller temporal areas (e.g., hippocampus, amygdala, middle temporal gyrus) and the Val/Val and Val/Met genotypes in patients with schizophrenia. Smaller frontal areas also seem to be associated with the Val/Val genotype in patients with schizophrenia and in people at high risk for psychosis. This is interesting since CPT performance is associated with prefrontal cortex volume in patients with schizophrenia. Although, to our knowledge, no study has been conducted to date using the n-back task and structural MRI, we speculate that this test may be associated with both prefrontal and medial temporal volumes in view of evidence of prefrontal activation during the task88,95,126–129 and of an association between worse working memory performance and smaller frontal, prefrontal and temporal lobe volumes.131,132 Moreover, both neuropsychological tasks (n-back task and CPT) and brain regional volumes (medial temporal and prefrontal cortex), which have been reported to be associated with the COMT Val158Met polymorphism, met most of the criteria for an endophenotype.
Taken together, these results suggest that the COMT Val158Met polymorphism may contribute to 2 neuropsychological and 2 brain structural endophenotypes that characterize schizophrenia, with the prefrontal cortex representing a common neural substrate underlying both n-back task and CPT performance. It is interesting to note that the association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and the endophenotypes detected (with the exception of the hippocampus) is more evident in patients with schizophrenia than in healthy controls. This suggests that this polymorphism alone is not sufficient to produce a variation at the neuropsychological and brain structural levels. The presence of these endophenotypes (e.g., working memory impairment, prefrontal cortex grey matter volume reduction) seems to result from an interaction between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and other genetic and nongenetic (e.g., environmental) factors (possibly present in excess in affected individuals), which together contribute to shape the neuropsychological and brain structural alterations that characterize schizophrenia.47,80,146 We speculate that alterations in the fine-tuning of cortical DA may be responsible for worse neuropsychological performance and for brain volume reductions. More specifically, the COMT Val carriers may have reduced DA levels in the prefrontal cortex, leading to a decrease in D1 receptor activation with subsequent impairment in cognitive tasks, such as working memory.147 In fact, working memory seems to be related to the activity of both D1 and D2 receptors: the D1 receptors would signal to prefrontal pyramidal neurons via N-methyl-D-aspartate postsynaptic receptors, maintaining and stabilizing task-relevant signals while reducing signal-to-noise ratio in the prefrontal cortex via GABAergic inhibitory effect on nonrelevant “noisy” signals. The D2 receptors would in turn allow the development of new informative signals.147 In addition, the presence of the COMT Val allele may indirectly influence the neurodevelopment and neuroplasticity processes, ultimately leading to changes in the volume of brain areas such as the prefrontal cortex. Since COMT is responsible for DA cortical availability,67 it may also be implicated in key neuronal processes that ultimately lead to brain morphogenesis and development. How this actually occurs remains to be established. Interestingly, a recent review has proposed that COMT influences neurodevelopment and neuroplasticity via an epistatic interaction with other genes, such as AKT1.148AKT1 and dopaminergic pathways have already been shown to be synergistically responsible for neuronal atrophy after chronic opiate exposure.148,149 Also, genetic variation in AKT1 has been linked to DA-associated prefrontal cortical structure and function in humans.148 It is possible that the combination of different risk alleles results in impaired prefrontal activity during challenging cognitive tasks and in changes in key neurodevelopmental pathways affecting neuronal morphology and development.150
Limitations
The main limitation to our interpretation of the literature is that the association between a single genetic variant and an endophenotype does not necessarily imply a causal relationship between them. In other words, we hypothesized that the COMT Val158Met polymorphism, which affects prefrontal DA levels, may contribute to both CPT and n-back impairment and reduced prefrontal and medial temporal volume. However, schizophrenia is characterized by a complex genetic architecture, with an intricate interplay among several genes. As mentioned previously, the COMT Val158Met polymorphism can interact with other genes in shaping the neuropsychological profile and the brain structural alterations that characterize schizophrenia (for example, see the studies by Caldú and colleagues,47 Zinkstok and colleagues,80 and Dickinson and colleagues146).
Moreover, variables such as sex and age need to be taken into account. For example, estrogen reduces COMT enzymatic activity, and the production of estrogen varies across age.146 Finally, the COMT Val158Met polymorphism interacts with environmental factors like stress. This has been suggested by a recent study reporting that individuals homozygous for Met performed significantly worse than those homozygous for Val on the n-back task under stress.151 This highlights the importance of considering the complex interaction between genes and environment when evaluating neurocognitive performance.151
In view of this complexity, it is possible that the endophenotypes we found to be associated with the COMT Val158Met polymorphism are also related to other polymorphisms. Furthermore, there is little evidence on the association between neuropsychological tasks and brain structure in patients with schizophrenia, and the link we suggest in this review needs further investigation. With these caveats in mind, this may explain, at least in part, the biological substrate of 4 important endophenotypes that characterize schizophrenia.
Conclusion
Our review comprehensively evaluated evidence on the link between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and both neuropsychological performance and brain structure in patients with schizophrenia and their relatives, and in healthy controls. The review also summarized this evidence within a biological framework that may be used to advance our knowledge on the molecular mechanisms that underlie the pathophysiology of schizophrenia.
Footnotes
Competing interests: None declared for M. Zanoni and P. Dazzan. As below for E. Ira, M. Ruggeri and S. Tosato.
Contributors: S. Tosato designed the review. E. Ira acquired the data and analyzed it with M. Zanoni, M. Ruggeri and P. Dazzan. E. Ira and M. Zanoni wrote the article, which M. Ruggeri, P. Dazzan and S. Tosato reviewed. All authors approved its publication.
Funding: This study was supported by Veneto Region, Italy, with the FSE grant “Acquisizione nuove tecniche automatizzate di studio di immagini di risonanza magnetica nucleare cerebrali” to E. Ira and by Fondazione Cariverona with the grant “Disabilità cognitiva e comportamentale nelle demenze e nelle psicosi. Sotto-obiettivo A.9. Basi morfofunzionali cognitive e genetiche delle psicosi maggiori: uno studio integrato longitudinale” to M. Ruggeri and S. Tosato.
References
Articles from Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience : JPN are provided here courtesy of Canadian Medical Association
Full text links
Read article at publisher's site: https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.120178
Read article for free, from open access legal sources, via Unpaywall: http://jpn.ca/wp-content/uploads/2013/12/38-6-366.pdf
Citations & impact
Impact metrics
Citations of article over time
Smart citations by scite.ai
Explore citation contexts and check if this article has been
supported or disputed.
https://scite.ai/reports/10.1503/jpn.120178
Article citations
Meta-analysis of structural and functional brain abnormalities in early-onset schizophrenia.
Front Psychiatry, 15:1465758, 23 Aug 2024
Cited by: 0 articles | PMID: 39247615 | PMCID: PMC11377232
COMT Val158Met and BDNF Val66Met Single-Nucleotide Polymorphisms Are Not Associated With Emotional Distress One Year After Moderate-Severe Traumatic Brain Injury.
Neurotrauma Rep, 4(1):495-506, 07 Aug 2023
Cited by: 0 articles | PMID: 37636335 | PMCID: PMC10457651
Prognostic and immunomodulatory roles of schizophrenia-associated genes HTR2A, COMT, and PRODH in pan-cancer analysis and glioma survival prediction model.
Front Immunol, 14:1201252, 26 Jul 2023
Cited by: 3 articles | PMID: 37564635 | PMCID: PMC10411190
Early- and late-phase changes of brain activity and early-phase neuromodulation in the posttraumatic stress disorder rat model.
Neurobiol Stress, 26:100554, 22 Jul 2023
Cited by: 0 articles | PMID: 37576348 | PMCID: PMC10415797
Pharmacogenetic Guided Opioid Therapy Improves Chronic Pain Outcomes and Comorbid Mental Health: A Randomized, Double-Blind, Controlled Study.
Int J Mol Sci, 24(13):10754, 28 Jun 2023
Cited by: 2 articles | PMID: 37445931 | PMCID: PMC10341655
Go to all (40) article citations
Data
Data behind the article
This data has been text mined from the article, or deposited into data resources.
BioStudies: supplemental material and supporting data
SNPs
- (1 citation) dbSNP - rs1800497
Similar Articles
To arrive at the top five similar articles we use a word-weighted algorithm to compare words from the Title and Abstract of each citation.
Relationship of catechol-O-methyltransferase variants to brain structure and function in a population at high risk of psychosis.
Biol Psychiatry, 61(10):1127-1134, 02 Oct 2006
Cited by: 83 articles | PMID: 17014827
Executive subprocesses in working memory: relationship to catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met genotype and schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry, 60(9):889-896, 01 Sep 2003
Cited by: 406 articles | PMID: 12963670
Impact of catechol-O-methyltransferase on prefrontal brain functioning in schizophrenia spectrum disorders.
Neuropsychopharmacology, 32(1):162-170, 28 Jun 2006
Cited by: 42 articles | PMID: 16823382
[Schizophrenia, genetics and cognition].
Encephale, 37 Suppl 2:S127-32, 01 Dec 2011
Cited by: 0 articles | PMID: 22212842
Review