Expanding Opportunities for Systems Thinking, Conceptual Learning, and Participation through Embodied and Computational Modeling
<p>Computational model with algae (producer), guppies (consumer), killifish (competitor), and cichlids (predator). The graph on the right represents populations of each organism, as well as particles of oxygen and carbon dioxide available in the ecosystem.</p> "> Figure 2
<p>Computational model code for guppies.</p> "> Figure 3
<p>Embodied model graphs, used to track populations of algae, guppies, killifish, and cichlids as students enacted the embodied models. The letters <b>a</b>–<b>e</b> were added to the figure to identify segments of the graph that students referenced in classroom discourse.</p> "> Figure A1
<p>Energy log given to students to track energy lost (from moving) and gained (from eating).</p> ">
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Computational and Embodied Modeling
1.2. Multimodal and Everyday Resources for Learning and Participation
1.3. Research Questions
- With each design cycle, how did the conceptual and epistemic relationship between the computational model and the embodied model shift?
- How did embodied modeling activities shape opportunities for learning and participation, particularly for students classified as ELs?
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants and Research Context
2.2. Initial Design
2.3. Data Sources and Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Quarter One: From a Rehearsal to a Reflective Medium
3.1.1. Conceptual/Epistemic Role of Embodied Modeling
3.1.2. Significance for Learning and Participation
- Ms. S:
- I saw someone eat some algae. So, did you check—did you put the extra two points by it, Sofia?
- Katherine:
- So, you write down 11?
- Ms. S:
- Wait, stop, Katherine has a question for the class.
- Katherine:
- So, if you begin with 10 energy, and we’re on nine energy, if you eat algae is it plus two, 11 energy or 10 energy?
- Ms. S:
- Well remember, every time you take a step, what happens?
- Students:
- Lose one.
- Ms. S:
- Every time you touch algae, what happens?
- Students:
- Gain two.
- Ms. S:
- So there you go.
- Ms. S:
- What do you have on your sheet right now?
- Students (algae):
- Nothing!
- Reid:
- I have nine [energy].
- Ms. S:
- So, you’ve taken one step and you’ve eaten nothing?
- Reid:
- I’ve taken like four steps and I ate Christian.
- Ms. S:
- Okay who else can share theirs?
- Grace:
- I have 13.
- Ms. S:
- Okay tell us how that happened.
- Grace:
- I ate all of them [algae], and I subtracted it.
- Ms. S:
- What about your steps, did you subtract them? Nicely done. Jasmin what about you?
- Jasmin:
- I took steps and I ate Anthony.
- Ms. S:
- Very good!
- Ashlyn:
- Have we seen anybody die yet?
- David:
- I’m dead.
- Ashlyn:
- So, let’s keep going.
- Ms. S:
- Now I see people taking one step. What I don’t see is the turn. What is the first step [of the program]?
- Students:
- Turn left.
- Ms. S:
- You can, or you could—
- Students:
- Turn right.
- Ms. S:
- As long as you’re staying within that line, right? So, you could go negative 90 all the way, you could go anywhere in between negative and positive 90. So, I want to see that first.
- Ms. S:
- I see a lot of people moving but not turning.
- Student:
- Do we have to turn?
- Ms. S:
- You don’t have to. But go ahead and give it a shot because you—you may want —why would you want to turn? What would be the point in turning?
- Students:
- Predator. Eating.
- Ms. S:
- To not get eaten, or what?
- Students:
- To get food. Eating.
- Ms. S:
- Right, so you can look around to see if you’ve got anyone [representing “food”] close to you.
- Ms. S:
- The cichlid [predator] in the last one was able to only eat guppies, right? Now the cichlid can also eat killifish [competitor].
- David:
- Ooo—I get a balanced diet.
- Hunter:
- What?! He gets to survive so much longer!
- Ashlyn:
- Do we think that’s going to let the guppies live longer—
- Students:
- No!
- Ashlyn:
- Or shorter, or doesn’t matter?
- Ross:
- Uh, he’s going to be targeting killifish.
- Reid:
- It doesn’t matter, either way the killi—
- David:
- I’m gonna target everything.
- Hunter:
- It doesn’t matter either way.
- David:
- If it’s moving, I’m gonna eat it.
- Reid:
- It’s not going to affect anything.
- Ashlyn:
- Does anyone think that letting the cichlid eat the killifish will let the guppies live longer?
- Students:
- No.
- Ashlyn:
- Are there any killifish still alive?
- Jasmin:
- I’m still alive.
- Ashlyn:
- Just one. Last time both killifish made it to the end, now we have a killifish and two guppies still alive, so the guppies are making it longer than they did last time. Why are the guppies able to survive longer?
- Jasmin:
- Because the cichlids are eating the killifish.
- Hunter:
- OH WAIT! [Eating] the killifish does have an effect on it because now [the cichlids] have two prey and maybe they won’t go for the guppy as much.
- Ashlyn:
- And so what does that mean for the guppy as well?
- Hunter:
- That they will live longer. Some more [guppies] will live longer… since like David’s [cichlid] doing, he’s chasing her [killifish] which makes time for the guppies to just roam around and find food without getting eaten straight away because he’s distracted…then [guppies] get more food, they get more food because the killifish, there’s only one instead of like three eating all the algae.
3.2. Quarter Two: From Remixing to Leveraging Resources
3.2.1. Conceptual/Epistemic Role of Embodied Modeling
3.2.2. Significance for Learning and Participation
- Ashlyn:
- So, we’re about to switch [roles]—is there anything that we want to change that we think would make the guppies live longer?
- Lucy:
- Maybe they could…
- Virginia:
- If the algae…
- Porter:
- If they could talk to each other.
- Ms. S:
- Wait, wait, wait, we can only hear one at a time.
- Abby:
- If the algae grew back up maybe a little bit faster.
- Porter:
- If the guppies kind of communicate because I saw one thing—Ana and Virginia [guppies] were coming for the both of us [algae], but Virginia got both of us and Ana—and Virginia could have told her to go like find somewhere else because she was going to get us.
- Ashlyn:
- Okay so guppy communication could be a solution so they could strategize better, or algae re-growing faster could be a solution. Is there any other solution you guys could think of that would make the guppies live longer?
- Caleb:
- Reproduction faster.
- Virginia:
- Reproduction faster.
- Ashlyn:
- Yeah, we don’t have any reproduction right now. That would be hard to imagine in an in-person model, even harder to imagine than the computer model. Any other ways we could have the guppies stay alive longer?
- Lucy:
- They could like start with more energy.
- Ashlyn:
- They could start with more energy. Okay so let’s pick one thing that you’re allowed to change in your program. Who wants to nominate something? What do you think Abby?
- Abby:
- The algae reproduces faster.
- Ashlyn:
- Okay so let’s change in our program… let’s have the algae grow back after seven [ticks] next time. So, after seven ticks the algae will grow back. That’s the only thing we are going to change this time. We might change something else next time.
- Ms. S:
- So, the guppies are going to reproduce. So how do we do that? We’re going to program that. How do we make that happen in the class?
- Abby:
- After seven…
- Lucy:
- Stand back up.
- Abby:
- After five ticks the guppies stand up.
- Porter:
- Four—four [ticks], because they get low energy, because the guppies are moving too, so you lose energy when you’re moving… So I said four.
- Ms. S:
- So, reproduction is gonna happen kind of like that—respawning?
- Porter:
- Because we have to move, so we’re gonna lose energy…
- Ms. S:
- Okay so what’s the plan? So, you think after four you can stand back up? Is that what I’m hearing?
- Ana:
- So, when we stand back up, how much energy do we have? How much energy do we get back when we reproduce? Because I wasn’t sure if it was like 10, or half of it.
- Miles:
- 10, because you’re new.
- Virginia:
- It’s a new guppy, so it should be the same.
- Ashlyn:
- Okay, so another good question I heard was: can killifish also reproduce?
- Students:
- Yeah.
- Virginia:
- Yeah, but I think we should only have one killifish.
- Porter:
- But the cichlids should not reproduce.
- Ashlyn:
- Why did all of our guppies die?
- Virginia:
- Because all the algae was spread out.
- Ashlyn:
- Interesting, so why does all the algae being spread out mean that the guppies are more likely to die?
- Micah:
- Because they’re going for competition.
- Lucy:
- They have to move more.
- Caleb:
- Because they can’t move fast enough.
- Porter:
- And if they all go for one and somebody gets it then another person won’t get it.
- Ashlyn:
- What if we had set it up to where… we had a guppy over here, but all the algae was in the same place [across the room], would that guppy have made it?
- Students:
- Yeah—No, No.
- Ashlyn:
- Why not?
- Ana:
- Because it’s like, so far away, and they don’t have enough energy to like go all the way.
- Ms. S:
- What did anyone else notice with this change?
- Caleb:
- That the algae was like more crowded and didn’t reproduce as fast—like they were more crowded in one spot kind of.
- Ms. S:
- Okay, so how did that affect things?
- Caleb:
- The guppies got to eat more because like Miles (guppy), he—like, every time he ate her (algae), he would like, go over there to eat something or just stand over there to wait and then come back and then wait 15 ticks and then get her again.
- Ms. S:
- Do you think you could somehow look at that and think about animal behavior?
- Caleb:
- Maybe that it’s hungry?
- Ms. S:
- That’s good, I like that. Abby, you were about to say something?
- Abby:
- I mean I think it might, but it also—the animals, they can’t just stand still, they have to continue moving. So, I don’t know if it’s exactly correct, but I mean, I kind of moved between Maria, Ana, and Lucy, because they were all in a small cluster.
- Ms. S:
- Okay, Lucy?
- Lucy:
- So, if animals know like where the plants grow a lot, they might continue to go back to that place over time.
- Ms. S:
- Yes, think about—okay, when you guys did the invasive models in the science class, what was one of the things that happened? Did you notice any of the agents going to a particular area?
- Abby:
- Yeah, the zebra muss—the zebra clams or whatever, um they all went to sunny spots, and the sunny spots are where the algae would grow.
- Ms. S:
- So, these spots over here were—where the fish were just hanging out, that could have been a sunny spot, right? That’s really good.
3.3. Quarter Three: From Multimodal Resources to a Model System
3.3.1. Conceptual/Epistemic Role of Embodied Modeling
- Ms. S:
- What do you notice when they’re surviving long term?
- Jasper:
- Mine have been (gesture: wave gesture, oscillatory but rising) and up, up, up. Then oxygen and carbon dioxide go up and down.
3.3.2. Significance for Learning and Participation
- Ms. S:
- Did we see fluctuation?
- Students:
- Yes, I didn’t, Yes.
- Amanda:
- It basically just went like (gesture: line with negative slope, sound: high to low pitch).
- Sam:
- There was [fluctuation].
- Ms. S:
- There was? Talk to me about that, Sam.
- Sam:
- So, like with the algae, we started up higher, everyone was alive, and as we started to get tagged by the guppies, we went down. And after the 10 [ticks] we got back up…and the guppies, they were eating food and getting more and more…the guppies, as they were eating algae they were getting more and more energy, but then we [algae] died.
- Ms. S:
- And so that fluctuation, what did that look like, can you show me with your hand?
- Sam:
- (gesture: wave gesture)
- Ms. S:
- And what is that? Which line is that? Is that the guppy line, is that the algae line?
- Sam:
- Algae.
- Ms. S:
- Okay can you use both hands to show me the algae and the guppies?
- Sam:
- (gesture: wave gesture, oscillating—one hand high while the other is low)
- Ashlyn:
- Did we see that with guppies though? Did we see this shape for the guppies (gesture: wave gesture)?
- Students:
- No.
- Carlos:
- We [guppies] were high then we were low, low, low (gesture: stair-step gesture).
- Amanda:
- Because the guppies can’t reproduce.
- Nora:
- They need energy from the algae.
- Ashlyn:
- So, that’s interesting, so we can see fluctuating for the guppies in their energy—for each individual guppy there is fluctuating up and down—but for our guppies over all, we just saw it decrease, because when guppies died, did they get to reproduce?
- Students:
- No.
- Ashlyn:
- What would happen if the guppies got to reproduce?
- Eli:
- We [algae] would die more often then, because—
- Carlos:
- If there’s a new one that came back up—
- Eli:
- —every 10 ticks
- Carlos:
- and there’s no more plants, every 10 ticks, it’s gonna die too.
- Ashlyn:
- That’s so interesting, so you’re saying that letting guppies reproduce might actually be bad for the guppies?
- Jesús:
- Yeah, overpopulate.
- Carlos:
- Yeah, because it’s like if there’s four guppies alive and there’s someone [another guppy] that got back up, and there’s no more plants and all of them are saying that they’re dropped down, then they [guppies] wouldn’t have any food, and every time they’re going to have to move, and their energy would get too low.
- Ms. S:
- Alright so let’s look at our data. Does anyone want to describe what they see in the data right now?
- Eli:
- The guppies, I noticed, there was a steady state for about the first—I’m not sure how many ticks those are, but it was very steady (Figure 3a). But the algae had a rapid decline as the fish started—as the fish stayed steady, but it went back up when, I guess, one guppy died (Figure 3b). And, the—I guess it looks like two or three algae went up, and then it [algae] went right back down, after it [guppies] stayed steady (Figure 3c). So, it seemed as long as there was a guppy staying steady then the algae would go down. And then, it seemed as almost like it was very—just a little bit of change would affect it a lot, it looked like to me.
- Ms. S:
- What else Jasper?
- Jasper:
- It looks like this graph proves that the more predators, the less prey, and that the less predators, the more prey.
- Ashlyn:
- Can you explain more about that?
- Jasper:
- So, right here we have our highest amount of guppies (gesture: pointing to the graph, Figure 3a), but we also have our lowest amount of algae, and, but over here we have like a pretty high amount of algae, but we also have a low amount of guppies (Figure 3b). And this just shows that, um, there’s more competition for food when there’s more organisms that eat it.
- Ashlyn:
- And so all that competition from the organisms put pressure on the algae?
- Jasper:
- Yes.
- Ashlyn:
- The only thing that confuses me about that then is, like, we still have a high number of guppies here (gesture: points to graph, Figure 3b), like we went from five down to four, but that’s still pretty high, and we saw this big jump in algae. What do we think is happening there?
- Carlos:
- Most of the fish, from where they were at the beginning, kept eating all the algae and making them die (Figure 3a). Um so, every time they did it, and so on the ticks from right there—it went down when it was three ticks, it went down, and then the algae went “oh, I’m free to grow”, because all the other fish went to spread out and search for food, so then it was high (Figure 3b), went down, and went up again (Figure 3c).
- Ashlyn:
- And so, they had a chance to re-grow?
- Amanda:
- Also like, after like all of that decline, then there were like the 10 ticks, they started all growing—all coming back up (Figure 3b).
- Ashlyn:
- Does someone want to try and describe what happened there?
- Sean:
- I can see that the killifish, they were just the same throughout the whole thing—
- Driana:
- Which one is the killifish?
- Sean:
- The black line. The guppies didn’t do so well and also the algae was like the same with the fish, whenever it was like steady, it would drop, and then for like a second I think since also like the fish would be eating all the algae I think for, like, a second, they would all come back [algae re-grow, stand up] and then they would just be gone [eaten by guppies] after that (Figure 3d).
- Eli:
- I noticed that as, like, in the beginning, the guppies went down, but also so did the algae, so basically what happened was that the fish started to die because, there was a lot of algae but it just took a long time for them [algae] to reproduce, and they were so spread out, so that they died and then the killifish—no cichlid, died, sort of ending the populations, both populations (Figure 3d).
- Ashlyn:
- What happens to the algae after the guppies die (Figure 3e)?
- Sam:
- They start rising.
That was a really fun one, honestly, and it actually brought really good stuff at the end, because…with the cichlids there you could know how—how they like play—how they do stuff. So, like one step, minus one energy. It’s like in real life. You move places, you lose energy. You eat something, you gain energy…it showed different ideas because by the other models, the other models showed like just one predator, one prey—like that. That—that one had multiple—multiple preys and multiple algae things.
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A. Embodied Modeling Programs
Appendix A.1. Algae
- If you are tagged by a predator:
- You have been eaten (sit down).
- After 10 ticks, you can stand up (you have grown back as new algae).
Appendix A.2. Guppy
- Pick a number in between −90 degrees (full left turn) and 90 degrees (full right turn). Turn that amount.
- Take one step forward.
- Subtract one from your energy and on the next line write your new energy
- If your energy is zero:
- You have died (sit down).
- If you are within arm’s length of algae:
- Tag the algae
- Add two to your energy and on the same line write your new energy
- If you are tagged by a predator:
- You have died (sit down).
Appendix A.3. Killifish
- Pick a number in between −90 degrees (full left turn) and 90 degrees (full right turn). Turn that amount.
- Take one step forward.
- Subtract one from your energy and on the next line write your new energy
- If your energy is zero:
- You have died (sit down).
- If you are within arm’s length of algae:
- Tag the algae
- Add two to your energy and on the same line write your new energy
- If you are tagged by a predator:
- You have died (sit down).
Appendix A.4. Cichlid
- Pick a number in between −90 degrees (full left turn) and 90 degrees (full right turn). Turn that amount.
- Take one step forward.
- Subtract one from your energy and on the next line write your new energy
- If your energy is zero:
- You have died (sit down).
- If you are within arm’s length of a guppy:
- Tag the guppy
- Add two to your energy and on the same line write your new energy.
Appendix B. Interview Protocol
- Shows a picture of each of the students’ models: plant investigation, biosphere plan, biosphere model, food web, embodied model, computational model. For each model, ask:
- Can you tell me about making this model?
- What does this model show?
- Are any of the models related to each other? Why or why not?
- Which of the models were the most interesting or helpful to you?
- Which of the models were the least interesting or helpful to you?
- Did you use different languages [representations] in your models?
- If yes: does using different languages [representations] change how you think?
- If yes: can you tell me about a time when that happened?
- In your science class, we used a computational model about zebra mussels. But we didn’t build that one—it was already built.
- If you think about the scientists that built that model, what kind of information do you think they needed to build it?
- How do you think they get that information?
- Do you think those scientists use other models to understand zebra mussels?
- If yes: which types of models? How would they use them?
- Is there anything we should definitely keep or definitely change about these projects for next quarter?
References
- Dickes, A.C.; Sengupta, P. Learning Natural Selection in 4th Grade with Multi-Agent-Based Computational Models. Res. Sci. Educ. 2013, 43, 921–953. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Papert, S. Mindstorms: Children, Computers, and Powerful Ideas; Basic Books, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1980. [Google Scholar]
- Wilensky, U.; Reisman, K. Thinking Like a Wolf, a Sheep, or a Firefly: Learning Biology Through Constructing and Testing Computational Theories—An Embodied Modeling Approach. Cogn. Instr. 2006, 24, 171–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dickes, A.C.; Sengupta, P.; Farris, A.V.; Basu, S. Development of Mechanistic Reasoning and Multilevel Explanations of Ecology in Third Grade Using Agent-Based Models. Sci. Educ. 2016, 100, 734–776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Danish, J.A. Applying an activity theory lens to designing instruction for learning about the structure, behavior, and function of a honeybee system. J. Learn. Sci. 2014, 23, 100–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Forrester, J.W. Industrial Dynamics; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1961. [Google Scholar]
- Wilensky, U.; Stroup, W.M. Learning through participatory simulations: Network-based design for systems learning in classrooms. In Proceedings of the Computer Supported Collaborative Learning (CSCL ’99), Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA, 12–15 December 1999. [Google Scholar]
- García, O.; Kleyn, T. Translanguaging with Multilingual Students: Learning from Classroom Moments; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Blackledge, A.; Creese, A. Translanguaging and the body. Int. J. Multiling. 2017, 14, 250–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wei, L. Translanguaging as a practical theory of language. Appl. Linguist. 2018, 39, 9–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- diSessa, A.; Hammer, D.; Sherin, B.; Kolpakowski, T. Inventing graphing: Meta-representational expertise in children. J. Math. Behav. 1991, 10, 117–160. [Google Scholar]
- Weintrop, D.; Beheshti, E.; Horn, M.; Orton, K.; Jona, K.; Trouille, L.; Wilensky, U. Defining computational thinking for mathematics and science classrooms. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 2016, 25, 127–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilensky, U.; Resnick, M. Thinking in Levels: A Dynamic Systems Approach to Making Sense of the World. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 1999, 8, 3–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilkerson-Jerde, M.H.; Gravel, B.E.; Macrander, C.A. Exploring shifts in middle school learners’ modeling activity while generating drawings, animations, and computational simulations of molecular diffusion. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 2015, 24, 396–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sengupta, P.; Kinnebrew, J.S.; Basu, S.; Biswas, G.; Clark, D. Integrating computational thinking with K-12 science education using agent-based computation: A theoretical framework. Educ. Inf. Technol. 2013, 18, 351–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lee, I.; Martin, F.; Denner, J.; Coulter, B.; Allan, W.; Inroads, J.E.A. Computational thinking for youth in practice. In Proceedings of the IDC ’14: Proceedings of the 2014 Conference on Interaction Design and Children, Aarhus, Denmark, 17–20 June 2014; Volume 2, pp. 32–37. [Google Scholar]
- Guo, Y.; Wagh, A.; Brady, C.; Levy, S.T.; Horn, M.S. Frogs to think with: Improving Students’ computational thinking and understanding of evolution in a code-first learning environment. In Proceedings of the IDC ’14, 2014 Conference on Interaction Design and Children, Manchester, UK, 21–24 June 2016; pp. 246–254. [Google Scholar]
- Horn, M.S.; Brady, C.; Hjorth, A.; Wagh, A.; Wilensky, U. Frog pond: A codefirst learning environment on evolution and natural selection. In Proceedings of the IDC ’14: Proceedings of the 2014 Conference on Interaction Design and Children, Aarhus, Denmark, 17–20 June 2014; pp. 357–360. [Google Scholar]
- Sengupta, P.; Dickes, A.; Farris, A.V.; Karan, A.; Martin, D.; Wright, M. Programming in K-12 Science Classrooms. Commun. ACM 2015, 58, 33–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brady, C.; Holbert, N.; Soylu, F.; Novak, M.; Wilensky, U. Sandboxes for model-based inquiry. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 2015, 24, 265–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klopfer, E. Technologies to support the creation of complex systems models—Using StarLogo software with students. Biosystems 2003, 71, 111–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Resnick, M. Turtles, Termites, and Traffic Jams: Explorations in Massively Parallel Microworlds; MIT press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Epstein, J.M.; Axtell, R. Growing Artificial Societies: Social Science from the Bottom up; Brookings Institution Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Yoon, S.A.; Anderson, E.; Koehler-Yom, J.; Evans, C.; Park, M.; Sheldon, J.; Schoenfeld, I.; Wendel, D.; Scheintaub, H.; Klopfer, E. Teaching about complex systems is no simple matter: Building effective professional development for computer-supported complex systems instruction. Instr. Sci. 2017, 45, 99–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Yoon, S.A.; Anderson, E.; Klopfer, E.; Koehler-Yom, J.; Sheldon, J.; Schoenfeld, I.; Wendel, D.; Scheintaub, H.; Oztok, M.; Evans, C.; et al. Designing Computer-Supported Complex Systems Curricula for the Next Generation Science Standards in High School Science Classrooms. Systems 2016, 4, 38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pierson, A.E.; Brady, C.E.; Clark, D.B. Balancing the Environment: Computational Models as Interactive Participants in a STEM Classroom. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 2020, 29, 101–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Enyedy, N.; Danish, J.A.; DeLiema, D. Constructing liminal blends in a collaborative augmented-reality learning environment. Int. J. Comput. Collab. Learn. 2015, 10, 7–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hall, R.; Nemirovsky, R. Introduction to the special issue: Modalities of body engagement in mathematical activity and learning. J. Learn. Sci. 2012, 21, 207–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kelton, M.L.; Ma, J.Y. Reconfiguring mathematical settings and activity through multi-party, whole-body collaboration. Educ. Stud. Math. 2018, 98, 177–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lindgren, R.; Johnson-Glenberg, M. Emboldened by embodiment: Six precepts for research on embodied learning and mixed reality. Educ. Res. 2013, 42, 445–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brady, C.; Orton, K.; Weintrop, D.; Anton, G.; Rodriguez, S.; Wilensky, U. All roads lead to computing: Making, participatory simulations, and social computing as pathways to computer science. IEEE Trans. Educ. 2016, 60, 59–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Colella, V. Participatory simulations: Building collaborative understanding through immersive dynamic modeling. J. Learn. Sci. 2000, 9, 471–500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Colella, V.; Borovoy, R.; Resnick, M. Participatory simulations: Using computational objects to learn about dynamic systems. In Proceedings of the CHI 98 Conference Summary on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Los Angeles, CA, USA, 18–23 April 1998; pp. 9–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klopfer, E.; Yoon, S.; Perry, J. Using palm technology in participatory simulations of complex systems: A new take on ubiquitous and accessible mobile computing. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 2005, 14, 285–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Resnick, M.; Wilensky, U. Diving into complexity: Developing probabilistic decentralized thinking through role-playing activities. J. Learn. Sci. 1998, 7, 153–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Danish, J.A.; Peppler, K.; Phelps, D. BeeSign: Designing to support mediated group inquiry of complex science by early elementary students. In Proceedings of the 9th International Conference on Interaction Design and Children, Barcelona, Spain, 9–12 June 2010; pp. 182–185. [Google Scholar]
- Danish, J.A. Designing for technology enhanced activity to support learning. J. Emerg. Learn. Des. 2013, 1, 1. [Google Scholar]
- Danish, J.A.; Enyedy, N.; Saleh, A.; Humburg, M. Learning in embodied activity framework: A sociocultural framework for embodied cognition. Int. J. Comput. Support. Collab. Learn. 2020, 15, 49–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reimers, J.; Brady, C. Theatrical Modeling as a Design for Perspectival Learning. In Proceedings of the International Conference of the Learning Sciences (ICLS ’20), Nashville, TN, USA, 20 June 2020; Volume 3, pp. 1817–1818. [Google Scholar]
- Bezemer, J.; Kress, G. Writing in multimodal texts: A social semiotic account of designs for learning. Writ. Commun. 2008, 25, 166–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lemke, J.L. Across the scales of time: Artifacts, activities, and meanings in ecosocial systems. Mind Cult. Act. 2000, 7, 273–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goodwin, C. Co-Operative Action, 1st ed.; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Feldman, L.B.; Aragon, C.R.; Chen, N.-C.; Kroll, J.F. Emoticons in text may function like gestures in spoken or signed communication. Behav. Brain Sci. 2017, 40, e55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oliveira, A.W.; Weinburgh, M.; McBride, E.; Bobowski, T.; Shea, R. Teaching Science to English Language Learners. In The Handbook of TESOL in K-12; de Oliveira, L.C., Ed.; Current Research and Practices in the Field of Science Education; John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.: Chichester, UK, 2019; Volume 12, pp. 277–290. [Google Scholar]
- Karlsson, A.; Larsson, P.N.; Jakobsson, A. Multilingual students’ use of translanguaging in science classrooms. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2019, 41, 2049–2069. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Probyn, M. Pedagogical translanguaging: Bridging discourses in South African science classrooms. Int. J. Biling. Educ. Biling. 2015, 29, 218–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grapin, S. Multimodality in the new content standards era: Implications for English learners. TESOL Q. 2019, 53, 30–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Williams, M. Fifth graders’ use of gesture and models when translanguaging during a content and language integrated science class in Hong Kong. Int. J. Biling. Educ. Biling. 2020, 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lee, O.; Miller, E.C.; Januszyk, R. Next generation science standards: All standards, all students. J. Sci. Teach. Educ. 2014, 25, 223–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moschkovich, J.N. Academic literacy in mathematics for English Learners. J. Math. Behav. 2015, 40, 43–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nersessian, N.J. Creating Scientific Concepts; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Latour, B. Pandora’s Hope: Essays on the Reality of Science Studies; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Gooding, D.C. From phenomenology to field theory: Faraday’s visual reasoning. Perspect. Sci. 2006, 14, 40–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Radke, S.; Vogel, S.; Hoadley, C.; Ma, J. Representing percents and personas: Designing syncretic curricula for modeling and statistical reasoning. In Proceedings of the International Conference of the Learning Sciences (ICLS ’20), Nashville, TN, USA, 20 June 2020; Volume 3, pp. 1365–1372. [Google Scholar]
- Vogel, S.; Hoadley, C.; Ascenzi-Moreno, L.; Menken, K. The Role of Translanguaging in Computational Literacies: Documenting Middle School Bilinguals’ Practices in Computer Science Integrated Units. In Proceedings of the Computer Supported Collaborative Learning (CSCL ’19), Lyon, France, 17–21 June 2019; pp. 1164–1170. [Google Scholar]
- Cobb, P.; Confrey, J.; diSessa, A.; Lehrer, R.; Schauble, L. Design Experiments in Educational Research. Educ. Res. 2003, 32, 9–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Erickson, F. A history of qualitative inquiry in social and educational research. In The SAGE Handbook of Qualitative Research; Denzin, N.K., Lincoln, Y.S., Eds.; Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Rubin, H.J.; Rubin, I. Qualitative Interviewing; Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Sandoval, W. Conjecture mapping: An approach to systematic educational design research. J. Learn. Sci. 2014, 23, 18–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Erickson, F.; Schultz, J. When is a context? Some issues and methods in the analysis of social competence. In Mind, Culture, and Activity: Seminal Papers from the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition; Cole, M.D., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1997; Volume 22, p. 31. [Google Scholar]
- Charmaz, K. Constructing Grounded Theory: A Practical Guide through Qualitative Research; Sage Publications Ltd.: London, UK, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Strauss, A.; Corbin, J. Basics of Qualitative Research; Sage: Newbury Park, CA, USA, 1990; Volume 15. [Google Scholar]
- Brady, C.; Weintrop, D.; Anton, G.; Wilensky, U. Constructionist learning at the group level with programmable badges. In Proceedings of the 2020 Constructionism Conference, Bangkok, Thailand, 1–5 February 2016; pp. 61–68. [Google Scholar]
- Lehrer, R.; Schauble, L. The development of scientific thinking. In Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Ford, M.; Forman, A. Redefining Disciplinary Learning in Classroom Contexts. Rev. Res. Educ. 2006, 30, 1–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bazerman, C. Shaping Written Knowledge: The Genre and Activity of the Experimental Article in Science; University of Wisconsin Press: Madison, WI, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Flores, N.; Rosa, J. Undoing appropriateness: Raciolinguistic ideologies and language diversity in education. Harv. Educ. Rev. 2015, 85, 149–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hudicourt-Barnes, J. The use of argumentation in Haitian Creole science classrooms. Harv. Educ. Rev. 2003, 73, 73–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nasir, N.S.; Rosebery, A.S.; Warren, B.; Lee, C.D. Learning as a cultural process: Achieving equity through diversity. In The Cambridge Handbook of: The Learning Sciences; Sawyer, R.K., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2014; pp. 489–504. [Google Scholar]
- Lehrer, R. Designing to develop disciplinary dispositions: Modeling natural systems. Am. Psychol. 2009, 64, 759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miller, E.; Manz, E.; Russ, R.; Stroupe, D.; Berland, L. Addressing the epistemic elephant in the room: Epistemic agency and the next generation science standards. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2018, 55, 1053–1075. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holdway, J.; Hitchcock, C.H. Exploring ideological becoming in professional development for teachers of multilingual learners: Perspectives on translanguaging in the classroom. Teach. Teach. Educ. 2018, 75, 60–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pacheco, M.B.; Daniel, S.M.; Pray, L.C.; Jiménez, R.T. Translingual practice, strategic participation, and meaning-making. J. Lit. Res. 2019, 51, 75–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cole, M.W.; David, S.S.; Jiménez, R.T. Collaborative translation: Negotiating student investment in culturally responsive pedagogy. Lang. Arts 2016, 93, 430–443. [Google Scholar]
- Gutiérrez, K.D.; Bien, A.C.; Selland, M.K.; Pierce, D.M. Polylingual and polycultural learning ecologies: Mediating emergent academic literacies for dual language learners. J. Early Child. Lit. 2011, 11, 232–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- García, O.; Kleifgen, J.A. Translanguaging and literacies. Read. Res. Q. 2019. [CrossRef]
- Rosebery, A.S.; Ogonowski, M.; DiSchino, M.; Warren, B. “The coat traps all your body heat”: Heterogeneity as fundamental to learning. J. Learn. Sci. 2010, 19, 322–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gutiérrez, K.D.; Rogoff, B. Cultural ways of learning: Individual traits or repertoires of practice. Educ. Res. 2003, 32, 19–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Philip, T.M.; Bang, M.; Jackson, K. Articulating the “How,” the “for What,” the “for Whom,” and the ‘with Whom’ in Concert: A Call to Broaden the Benchmarks of Our Scholarship; Taylor & Francis: Abingdon, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Bang, M.; Brown, B.; Rosebery, A.S.; Warren, B. Toward more equitable learning in science. In Helping Students Make Sense of the World Using Next Generation Science and Engineering Practices; Schwarz, C.V., Passmore, C., Reiser, B.J., Eds.; NSTA Press: Arlington, VA, USA, 2016; pp. 33–58. [Google Scholar]
- Cobb, P.; Jackson, K.; Dunlap, C. Conducting design studies to investigate and support mathematics students’ and teachers’ learning. Compend. Res. Math. Educ. 2017, 208–233. [Google Scholar]
1 | All students entering this district complete a Home Language Survey which is used to determine if language proficiency evaluation is needed. Students are classified as ELs based on the IPT and ELDA assessments, which are used along with content-area state assessments to monitor their progress annually. Based on these assessments, the district determines whether a student’s English proficiency “does not enable them to succeed in school”. According to the district website, students are typically classified as ELs for four to seven years. |
Quarter | Initial/Revised Conjecture | Manifestation in Design | Revisions to Conjecture |
---|---|---|---|
1 | Embodied modeling helps students “read” code to understand agent actions and interactions as well as system-level effects, making computational modeling more accessible to students. | Students are given embodied code to enact that is similar to the code in their computational models of ecosystems. | Embodied modeling relies on all students’ insight and input. Embodied modeling can support learning about new aspects of phenomena. |
2 | Embodied modeling can offer students opportunities to understand computational models and real-world phenomena by inviting students’ questions and ideas. | Students are given code to enact and remix (modify) to ask and answer questions about ecosystems. | Students’ resources (e.g., social interaction) can be generative resources for conceptual learning through embodied modeling. |
3 | Embodied modeling can offer students opportunities to enrich their understanding by inviting students’ questions and ideas and by legitimizing and leveraging diverse multimodal resources. | Students are given code to enact and remix, and they are invited to used and create linked, multimodal representations to make sense of embodied modeling in relation to other models and real-world phenomena. | Embodied modeling can support more equitable learning and participation by positioning diversity as an asset in STEM. |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Pierson, A.E.; Brady, C.E. Expanding Opportunities for Systems Thinking, Conceptual Learning, and Participation through Embodied and Computational Modeling. Systems 2020, 8, 48. https://doi.org/10.3390/systems8040048
Pierson AE, Brady CE. Expanding Opportunities for Systems Thinking, Conceptual Learning, and Participation through Embodied and Computational Modeling. Systems. 2020; 8(4):48. https://doi.org/10.3390/systems8040048
Chicago/Turabian StylePierson, Ashlyn E., and Corey E. Brady. 2020. "Expanding Opportunities for Systems Thinking, Conceptual Learning, and Participation through Embodied and Computational Modeling" Systems 8, no. 4: 48. https://doi.org/10.3390/systems8040048