Musical Collaboration in Rhythmic Improvisation
<p>Study flow. (<b>A</b>) Two participants sit facing against each other in a same room to create music together by improvising using drum pads while acoustically communicating with each other through headphones. (<b>B</b>) Sound amplitudes extracted (2 min × 2 sessions, excluding first 15 s with a backing track from each session). (<b>C</b>) Music recurrence plots created from sound amplitudes by symbolizing following ordinal patterns. Colored areas of a recurrent plot indicate recurrence of a symbol at time <span class="html-italic">t</span> and <span class="html-italic">s</span>, with colors representing which symbol recurred. Portion of the recurrence plot shown for clarity.</p> "> Figure 2
<p>Mean observed recurrence metrics of the music created by a pair against null distributions: music was characterized by rhythmic patterns, and players preferred some patterns over others. First session: (<b>A</b>) symbolic-recurrence rate and (<b>B</b>) entropy. Second session: (<b>C</b>) symbolic-recurrence rate and (<b>D</b>) entropy. Vertical red lines, observed means; grey areas, null distributions of means.</p> "> Figure 3
<p>Mean observed recurrence metrics of interaction within a pair against null distributions: process of musical collaboration is underpinned by information sharing and transfer between players. First session: (<b>A</b>) mutual information and (<b>B</b>) transfer entropy received from partners. Second session: (<b>C</b>) mutual information and (<b>D</b>) transfer entropy received from partners. Vertical red lines, observed means; grey areas, null distributions of means.</p> "> Figure 4
<p>Effects of pairwise traits in musical expertise on mutual information: in the first session, information sharing was favored by differences in experience in playing with others and similarities in duration of practicing music. First session: (<b>A</b>) experience in playing music with others and (<b>B</b>) duration of practicing music. Second session: (<b>C</b>) experience in playing music with others and (<b>D</b>) duration of practicing music.</p> "> Figure 5
<p>Effects of individual traits in musical expertise on transfer entropy (from partner to focal player): in the first session, information transfer was favored by differences in experience in playing with others and similarities in duration of practicing music. First session: (<b>A</b>) experience in playing music with others and (<b>B</b>) duration of practicing music. Second session: (<b>C</b>) experience in playing music with others and (<b>D</b>) duration of practicing music.</p> "> Figure A1
<p>Frequency of notes played by participants. Asterisks indicate significant difference at a 0.05 level.</p> "> Figure A2
<p>Diagonal lines of symbolic-recurrence plots. Mean length, maximal length, and determinism in (<b>A</b>–<b>C</b>) first and (<b>D</b>–<b>F</b>) second session. Vertical red lines, observed means; grey areas, null distributions of means.</p> "> Figure A3
<p>Vertical lines of symbolic-recurrence plots. Mean length, maximal length, and laminarity in (<b>A</b>–<b>C</b>) first and (<b>D</b>–<b>F</b>) second session. Vertical red lines, observed means; grey areas, null distributions of means.</p> "> Figure A4
<p>Mean observed recurrence metrics of music created by a pair and the interaction within the pair for a downsampling rate of 100 ms against null distributions. Symbolic-recurrence rate in (<b>A</b>) first and (<b>B</b>) second session, entropy in (<b>C</b>) first and (<b>D</b>) second session, mutual information in (<b>E</b>) first and (<b>F</b>) second session, and transfer entropy in (<b>G</b>) first and (<b>H</b>) second session. Vertical red lines, observed means; grey areas, null distributions of means.</p> "> Figure A5
<p>Correlations between the original recurrence metrics down-sampled at a 150-ms interval and those down-sampled at a 100-ms interval. (<b>A</b>) Symbolic-recurrence rate in the first session and (<b>B</b>) in the second session, (<b>C</b>) entropy in the first session and (<b>D</b>) in the second session, (<b>E</b>) mutual information in the first session and (<b>F</b>) in the second session, and (<b>G</b>) transfer entropy in the first session and (<b>H</b>) in the second session. Ellipses represent 95% confidence areas.</p> "> Figure A6
<p>Mean observed recurrence metrics of music created by a pair and the interaction within the pair symbolized with <math display="inline"><semantics> <mrow> <mi>m</mi> <mo>=</mo> <mn>2</mn> </mrow> </semantics></math>, against null distributions. Symbolic-recurrence rate in (<b>A</b>) first and (<b>B</b>) second session, entropy in (<b>C</b>) first and (<b>D</b>) second session, mutual information in (<b>E</b>) first and (<b>F</b>) second session, and transfer entropy in (<b>G</b>) first and (<b>H</b>) second session. Vertical red lines, observed means; grey areas, null distributions of means.</p> "> Figure A7
<p>Correlations between original recurrence metrics symbolized with <math display="inline"><semantics> <mrow> <mi>m</mi> <mo>=</mo> <mn>3</mn> </mrow> </semantics></math> and those with <math display="inline"><semantics> <mrow> <mi>m</mi> <mo>=</mo> <mn>2</mn> </mrow> </semantics></math>. Symbolic-recurrence rate in (<b>A</b>) first and (<b>B</b>) second session, entropy in (<b>C</b>) first and (<b>D</b>) second session, mutual information in (<b>E</b>) first and (<b>F</b>) second session, and transfer entropy in (<b>G</b>) first and (<b>H</b>) second session. Ellipses represent 95% confidence areas.</p> ">
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Experiment Setup
2.2. Data Collection
2.3. Symbolic-Recurrence Quantification
2.4. Analysis
3. Results and Discussion
3.1. Symbolic-Recurrence Quantification of Music
3.2. Information Sharing and Transfer on Symbolic Recurrence
3.3. Effects of Pair and Individual Traits on Information Sharing and Transfer
4. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Appendix A.1. Preference of Notes Played by Participants
Appendix A.2. Other Recurrence Metrics on Music
Appendix A.3. Effect of Downsampling Rate on Recurrence Metrics
Appendix A.4. Effect of m on Recurrence Metrics
References
- Wallin, N.L.; Merker, B.; Brown, S. The Origins of Music; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Bowman, W.D. Philosophical Perspectives on Music; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Craft, R. Conversations with Igor Stravinsky; Faber & Faber: London, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Lendvai, E. Béla Bartók: An Analysis of His Music; Stanmore Press: London, UK, 1971. [Google Scholar]
- Foote, J. Visualizing music and audio using self-similarity. In Proceedings of the Seventh ACM International Conference on Multimedia, Orlando, FL, USA, 30 October–5 November 1999; ACM Multimedia: New York, NY, USA, 1999; pp. 77–80. [Google Scholar]
- Serrà, J.; Carlos, A.; Andrzejak, R.G. Nonlinear audio recurrence analysis with application to genre classification. In Proceedings of the 2011 IEEE International Conference on Acoustics, Speech and Signal Processing (ICASSP), Prague, Czech Republic, 22–27 May 2011; pp. 169–172. [Google Scholar]
- Fukino, M.; Hirata, Y.; Aihara, K. Coarse-graining time series data: Recurrence plot of recurrence plots and its application for music. Chaos 2016, 26, 023116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liu, X.F.; Chi, K.T.; Small, M. Complex network structure of musical compositions: Algorithmic generation of appealing music. Phys. A 2010, 389, 126–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moore, J.M.; Corrêa, D.C.; Small, M. Is Bach’s brain a Markov chain? Recurrence quantification to assess Markov order for short, symbolic, musical compositions. Chaos 2018, 28, 085715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Eck, D.; Schmidhuber, J. Finding temporal structure in music: Blues improvisation with LSTM recurrent networks. In Proceedings of the 12th IEEE Workshop on Neural Networks for Signal Processing, Martigny, Switzerland, 6 September 2002; pp. 747–756. [Google Scholar]
- Dubnov, S.; Assayag, G.; Lartillot, O.; Bejerano, G. Using machine-learning methods for musical style modeling. Computer 2003, 36, 73–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weinberg, G.; Driscoll, S. Toward robotic musicianship. Comput. Music J. 2006, 30, 28–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bello, J.P. Measuring structural similarity in music. IEEE Trans. Audio Speech Lang. Process. 2011, 19, 2013–2025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Walton, A.E.; Washburn, A.; Langland-Hassan, P.; Chemero, A.; Kloos, H.; Richardson, M.J. Creating time: Social collaboration in music improvisation. Top. Cogn. Sci. 2018, 10, 95–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Acebrón, J.A.; Bonilla, L.L.; Vicente, C.J.P.; Ritort, F.; Spigler, R. The Kuramoto model: A simple paradigm for synchronization phenomena. Rev. Mod. Phys. 2005, 77, 137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Néda, Z.; Ravasz, E.; Vicsek, T.; Brechet, Y.; Barabási, A.L. Physics of the rhythmic applause. Phys. Rev. E 2000, 61, 6987. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Canonne, C.; Garnier, N. Individual decisions and perceived form in collective free improvisation. J. New Music Res. 2015, 44, 145–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson-Laird, P.N. How jazz musicians improvise. Music Percept. 2002, 19, 415–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kenny, B.J.; Gellrich, M. Improvisation. In The Science and Psychology of Music Performance; Parncutt, R., Gary, M., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2002; pp. 117–134. [Google Scholar]
- Winkler, I.; Háden, G.P.; Ladinig, O.; Sziller, I.; Honing, H. Newborn infants detect the beat in music. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2009, 106, 2468–2471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Zentner, M.; Eerola, T. Rhythmic engagement with music in infancy. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2010, 107, 5768–5773. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Williamon, A.; Davidson, J.W. Exploring co-performer communication. Music. Sci. 2002, 6, 53–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Healey, P.G.; Leach, J.; Bryan-Kinns, N. Inter-play: Understanding group music improvisation as a form of everyday interaction. In Proceedings of the Less Is More—Simple Computing in an Age of Complexity, Cambridge, UK, 27–28 April 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Caballero-Pintado, M.V.; Matilla-García, M.; Ruiz Marín, M. Symbolic recurrence plots to analyze dynamical systems. Chaos 2018, 28, 063112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boldini, A.; Karakaya, M.; Ruiz Marín, M.; Porfiri, M. Application of symbolic recurrence to experimental data, from firearm prevalence to fish swimming. Chaos 2019, 29, 113128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ravignani, A.; Delgado, T.; Kirby, S. Musical evolution in the lab exhibits rhythmic universals. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2016, 1, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Porfiri, M.; Ruiz Marín, M. Transfer entropy on symbolic recurrences. Chaos 2019, 29, 063123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Badke-Schaub, P.; Neumann, A.; Lauche, K.; Mohammed, S. Mental models in design teams: A valid approach to performance in design collaboration? CoDesign 2007, 3, 5–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cataldo, C. The art of improvising: The Be-Bop language and the major seventh chords. Art Des. Rev. 2017, 5, 222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schreiber, T. Measuring information transfer. Phys. Rev. Lett. 2000, 85, 461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Thompson, P.; Colebatch, J.; Brown, P.; Rothwell, J.; Day, B.; Obeso, J.; Marsden, C. Voluntary stimulus-sensitive jerks and jumps mimicking myoclonus or pathological startle syndromes. Mov. Disord. 1992, 7, 257–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2019; Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 17 February 2020).
- Sueur, J.; Aubin, T.; Simonis, C. Seewave: A free modular tool for sound analysis and synthesis. Bioacoustics 2008, 18, 213–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fox, J.; Weisberg, S. An R Companion to Applied Regression, 3rd ed.; Sage Publishing: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Oliphant, T.E. A guide to NumPy; Trelgol Publishing: Spanish Fork, UT, USA, 2006; Volume 1. [Google Scholar]
- Cross, I. Music, cognition, culture, and evolution. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2001, 930, 28–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Honing, H.; ten Cate, C.; Peretz, I.; Trehub, S.E. Without it no music: Cognition, biology and evolution of musicality. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 2015, 370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Desain, P.; Honing, H. The formation of rhythmic categories and metric priming. Perception 2003, 32, 341–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Savage, P.E.; Brown, S.; Sakai, E.; Currie, T.E. Statistical universals reveal the structures and functions of human music. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2015, 112, 8987–8992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Trehub, S.E. Cross-cultural convergence of musical features. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2015, 112, 8809–8810. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ng, H.H. Collective free music improvisation as a sociocommunicative endeavor: A literature review. Update Appl. Res. Music Educ. 2019, 37, 15–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sawyer, R.K. Improvised conversations: Music, collaboration, and development. Psychol. Music 1999, 27, 192–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krumhansl, C.L.; Toivanen, P.; Eerola, T.; Toiviainen, P.; Järvinen, T.; Louhivuori, J. Cross-cultural music cognition: Cognitive methodology applied to North Sami yoiks. Cognition 2000, 76, 13–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hannon, E.E.; Soley, G.; Ullal, S. Familiarity overrides complexity in rhythm perception: A cross-cultural comparison of American and Turkish listeners. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2012, 38, 543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Stevens, C.J. Music perception and cognition: A review of recent cross-cultural research. Top. Cogn. Sci. 2012, 4, 653–667. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yates, C.M.; Justus, T.; Atalay, N.B.; Mert, N.; Trehub, S.E. Effects of musical training and culture on meter perception. Psychol. Music 2017, 45, 231–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kolinski, M. A cross-cultural approach to metro-rhythmic patterns. Ethnomusicology 1973, 17, 494–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- LeBlanc, A. An interactive theory of music preference. J. Music Ther. 1982, 19, 28–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morrison, S.J.; Demorest, S.M. Cultural constraints on music perception and cognition. Prog. Brain Res. 2009, 178, 67–77. [Google Scholar]
- Polak, R.; Jacoby, N.; Fischinger, T.; Goldberg, D.; Holzapfel, A.; London, J. Rhythmic prototypes across cultures: A comparative study of tapping synchronization. Music Percept. 2018, 36, 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Soley, G.; Hannon, E.E. Infants prefer the musical meter of their own culture: A cross-cultural comparison. Dev. Psychol. 2010, 46, 286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Turek, R.; Schindler, A. The Elements of Music: Concepts and Applications; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Azzara, C.D. An aural approach to improvisation. Music Educ. 1999, 86, 21–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Becker, H.S. The etiquette of improvisation. Mind Cult. Act. 2000, 7, 171–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Biasutti, M.; Frezza, L. Dimensions of music improvisation. Creat. Res. J. 2009, 21, 232–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McDermott, J.; Hauser, M. The origins of music: Innateness, uniqueness, and evolution. Music Percept. 2005, 23, 29–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Patel, A.D. Musical rhythm, linguistic rhythm, and human evolution. Music Percept. 2006, 24, 99–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bates, D.; Mächler, M.; Bolker, B.; Walker, S. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 2015, 67, 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hothorn, T.; Bretz, F.; Westfall, P. Simultaneous Inference in General Parametric Models. Biom. J. 2008, 50, 346–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Marwan, N.; Romano, M.C.; Thiel, M.; Kurths, J. Recurrence plots for the analysis of complex systems. Phys. Rep. 2007, 438, 237–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mero, A.; Komi, P.V. Reaction time and electromyographic activity during a sprint start. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 1990, 61, 73–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Nakayama, S.; Soman, V.R.; Porfiri, M. Musical Collaboration in Rhythmic Improvisation. Entropy 2020, 22, 233. https://doi.org/10.3390/e22020233
Nakayama S, Soman VR, Porfiri M. Musical Collaboration in Rhythmic Improvisation. Entropy. 2020; 22(2):233. https://doi.org/10.3390/e22020233
Chicago/Turabian StyleNakayama, Shinnosuke, Vrishin R. Soman, and Maurizio Porfiri. 2020. "Musical Collaboration in Rhythmic Improvisation" Entropy 22, no. 2: 233. https://doi.org/10.3390/e22020233
APA StyleNakayama, S., Soman, V. R., & Porfiri, M. (2020). Musical Collaboration in Rhythmic Improvisation. Entropy, 22(2), 233. https://doi.org/10.3390/e22020233