Tail Docking of Canine Puppies: Reassessment of the Tail’s Role in Communication, the Acute Pain Caused by Docking and Interpretation of Behavioural Responses
Abstract
:Simple Summary
Abstract
1. Introduction
2. Animals Live as Dynamically Interacting Whole Entities
2.1. Internal and External Functional Interactivity and Sensory Capacities
2.2. Canine Behaviours That Involve Communication
2.3. Sensory and Behavioural Elements of Canine Communication
- Eye gaze—e.g., averted eyes, brief glances and blinking may be signs of placation; assertive direct eye-to-eye contact may indicate threat.
- Ear position—e.g., ears slightly back and slightly splayed may indicate uncertainty; ears back, flat against the head may indicate anxiety; ears erect or slightly forward may indicate alert, focused attention.
- Mouth shape and tongue—e.g., relaxed open mouth exposing rear teeth, i.e., “smiling”, may signal a relaxed/calm dog; mouth relaxed, slightly open, tongue slightly visible may indicate a dog at ease; mouth closed, no teeth or tongue visible may indicate calm focused attention; fleeting lip or nose licking may indicate unease, discomfort or nervousness; lips curled or lifted to expose teeth and perhaps gums may issue a warning or threat to another; lips retracted, snarling with mouth open and teeth bared may indicate extreme threat and imminent high-level aggression.
- Head position—e.g., the head down, only occasionally pointed at another dog, may be a placatory sign; the head turned to one side may be a calming signal; the head pointed unwaveringly at another dog may signal threat.
- Body features, demeanour and gait—e.g., “play bow” when facing another dog, crouched with front legs extended, rear body and tail elevated, may be an invitation to play; rolling on the back and rubbing the shoulders on the ground may show the dog is calm/relaxed; sitting with one forepaw raised may indicate the dog is unsure/anxious; muzzle nudge to threatening dog may represent placation; when approached, sitting and allowing itself to be sniffed, may show the dog is confident and not threatened; rolling on its side or back exposing its belly with no eye contact may represent extreme placation; lowered body, cringing while looking up may indicate fear or uncertainty; stiff-legged, standing upright may signal a challenge; slow stiff-legged movements, body sloped forward, feet braced may indicate a potentially aggressive dog; hackles raised may indicate threat of aggression or, alternatively, fear or uncertainty.
- Tail behaviours—Table 1 provides examples that are expressive of emotional state and/or intentions, and indicate that tail behaviour, in itself or, importantly, in combination with other behaviours, enhances a dog’s capacity to communicate. Further enhancement is apparently achieved by the laterality of tail wagging, the direction of which may signal positive and negative emotional states. Thus, a dog seeing its owner, a positive stimulus eliciting approach tendencies, exhibits a higher amplitude of tail wagging movements to the right side, whereas a dog seeing a potentially threatening unfamiliar dog, a negative stimulus eliciting withdrawal tendencies, exhibits a higher amplitude of tail wagging movements to the left side [46]. Importantly, dogs also seem to respond emotionally to others displaying such tail-wagging asymmetry, because when seeing another exhibiting left- rather than right-biased tail wagging the observer dog shows elevated cardiac activity and higher scores for anxiety behaviours [47].
2.4. Tail Behaviour Is an Integral Element in Canine Communication
2.5. Conclusion
3. Pain Experience in Canine Puppies: Misleading Interspecies Extrapolations
3.1. Acute Pain and Tail Docking
3.1.1. Neurologically Mature Young
3.1.2. Neurologically Immature Young
3.1.3. Comparison and Flawed Extrapolation
3.2. Elevated Pain Sensitivity, Chronic Pain and Tail Docking of Puppies
3.2.1. Elevated Pain Sensitivity
3.2.2. Chronic Pain
3.3. Conclusions
4. Docking-Related Pain Intensity Inferred from Puppy Behaviour Is Overestimated
4.1. The Human Emotional Drive to Care for and Protect Vulnerable Young
4.2. Interpretation of Behavioural Responses to Docking in Puppies
- “Although it is difficult to objectively quantify the stress experienced by puppies undergoing tail docking, observations recorded during this study suggest that the animals do experience pain.” (Page 335);
- “All pups appeared distressed by the amputation of the tail.” (Page 338);
- “Observations of puppy behaviour suggested that they were initially distressed but returned to normal levels within a short time (minutes rather than hours).” (Page 340).
4.3. Hypothesis Regarding the Existence of States of Subcortical Awareness in Immature Young
4.4. Conclusions
5. Discussion and Conclusions
- Is there adequate evidence that leaving the dogs intact predisposes them to harmful consequences?
- Is there compelling evidence that the proposed interference is in the best interests of the dogs and would be beneficial to the dogs?
- Would the harmful consequences or the benefits occur in a significant proportion of the dogs and therefore justify conducting the procedure on all dogs of a particular breed?
- Does the proposed interference cause greater harm to the dog than the damage it is intended to prevent?
- Is there another way with no, or fewer, adverse effects that would achieve the same end?
- Does the increase in “value” as a result of the interference justify the harm done to the dog?
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References and Note
- Morton, D. Docking of dogs: Practical and ethical aspects. Vet. Rec. 1992, 131, 301–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Noonan, G.J.; Rand, J.S.; Blackshaw, J.K.; Priest, J. Behavioural observations of puppies undergoing tail docking. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1996, 49, 335–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wansbrough, R.K. Cosmetic tail docking of dogs. Aust. Vet. J. 1996, 74, 59–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Australian Veterinary Association (AVA). AVA calls for nation ban on tail docking. Aust. Vet. J. 1998, 76, 581. [Google Scholar]
- Surgical Alteration to the Natural State of Animals—Policy and Background. Available online: http://www.ava.com.au/policy/31-surgical-alteration-natural-state-animals (accessed on 19 March 2018).
- Bennett, P.C.; Perini, E. Tail docking in dogs: A review of the issues. Aust. Vet. J. 2003, 81, 208–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lefebvre, D.; Lips, D.; Giffroy, J.M. The European Convention for the protection of pet animals and tail docking in dogs. Rev. Sci. Tech. 2007, 26, 619–628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Literature Review on the Welfare Implications of Tail Docking-Dogs. Available online: https://www.avma.org/KB/Resources/LiteratureReviews/Pages/Welfare-Implications-of-Tail-Docking-Dogs-Backgrounder.aspx (accessed on 19 March 2018).
- Cosmetic Alteration—Position Statement. Available online: https://www.canadianveterinarians.net/documents/cosmetic-alteration (accessed on 19 March 2018).
- RSPCA-Australia: Royal Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals—Australia. Is the Tail Docking of Dogs Legal? Available online: http://kb.rspca.org.au/is-the-tail-docking-of-dogs-legal_277.html (accessed on 19 March 2018).
- College of Veterinarians of British Columbia Bylaws, Part 4: Ethics and Standards. Available online: http://cvbc.ca/CVBC1/Registration_Contents/Files/Part%204%20-%20Ethics%20and%20Standards.pdf (accessed on 20 March 2018).
- Canine Tail Docking: Independent Report. Available online: https://www.mpi.govt.nz/dmsdocument/19013-canine-tail-docking-independent-report (accessed on 5 April 2018).
- Sinmez, C.C.; Yigit, A.; Aslim, G. Tail docking and ear cropping in dogs: A short review of laws and welfare aspects in the Europe and Turkey. Ital. J. Anim. Sci. 2017, 16, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Position Statement on Elective/Cosmetic Surgeries. Available online: http://www.wafederation.org/position-statements/ (accessed on 22 April 2018).
- Nielsen, B.L. Making sense of it all: The importance of taking into account the sensory abilities of animals in their housing and management. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jones, M.P.; Pierce, K.E.; Ward, D. Avian vision: A review of form and function with special consideration to birds of prey. J. Exot. Pet Med. 2007, 16, 69–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peters, R.P.; Mech, L.D. Scent-Marking in Wolves. Am. Sci. 1975, 63, 628–637. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Gazit, I.; Terkel, J. Domination of olfaction over vision in explosives detection by dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2003, 82, 65–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bijland, L.; Bomers, M.; Smulders, Y. Smelling the diagnosis: A review on the use of scent in diagnosing disease. Neth. J. Med. 2013, 71, 300–307. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Lord, K. A Comparison of the Sensory Development of Wolves (Canis lupus lupus) and Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris). Ethology 2013, 119, 110–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Broom, D.M.; Fraser, A.F. Introduction and concepts. In Domestic Animal Behaviour and Welfare, 5th ed.; CAB International: Wallingford, UK, 2015; pp. 3–19. [Google Scholar]
- Polgár, Z.; Miklósi, Á.; Gácsi, M. Strategies used by pet dogs for solving olfaction-based problems at various distances. PLoS ONE 2016, 10, e0131610. [Google Scholar]
- Holland, R.A.; Waters, D.A.; Rayner, J.M.V. Echolocation signal structure in the Megachiropteran bat Rousettus aegyptiacus Geoffroy 1810. J. Exp. Biol. 2004, 207, 4361–4369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hopkins, C.D. A biological function for electroreception in sharks and rays. J. Exp. Biol. 2010, 213, 1005–1007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- How Skates, Sharks Use Electricity to Sense Prey. Available online: https://www.the-scientist.com/?articles.view/articleNo/48748/title/How-Skates--Sharks-Use-Electricity-to-Sense-Prey/ (accessed on 7 April 2018).
- Seigel, R.A.; Collins, J.T. Snakes: Ecology and Behavior; MsGraw-Hill, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Schwenk, K. Of tongues and noses, chemoreception in lizards and snakes. Trends Ecol. Evol. 1995, 10, 7–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fox, M.W. The anatomy of aggression and its ritualization in Canidae: A developmental and comparative study. Behaviour 1969, 35, 243–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kleiman, D.G. Social behaviour of the Maned Wolf (Chrysocyon brachyurus) and Bush Dog (Speothos venaticus): A study in contrast. J. Mammal. 1972, 53, 791–806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miklósi, Á. Intraspecific social organization in dogs and related forms. In Dog Behaviour, Evolution and Cognition, 2nd ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2015; pp. 172–182. [Google Scholar]
- Miklósi, Á. The perceptual world of the dog. In Dog Behaviour, Evolution and Cognition, 2nd ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2015; pp. 183–204. [Google Scholar]
- Miklósi, Á. Affiliative and agonistic social relationships. In Dog Behaviour, Evolution and Cognition, 2nd ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2015; pp. 223–251. [Google Scholar]
- Bradshaw, J.; Rooney, N. Dog social behaviour and communication. In The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behavior and Interactions with People; Serpell, J., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2017; pp. 133–159. [Google Scholar]
- Rugaas, T. On Talking Terms with Dogs: Calming Signals, 2nd ed.; Dogwise Publishing: Wenatchee, WA, USA, 2006; ISBN 1929242360. [Google Scholar]
- Miklósi, Á.; Topál, J.; Csányi, V. Comparative social cognition: What can dogs teach us? Anim. Behav. 2004, 67, 995–1004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coren, S. How to Speak Dog; Simon and Schuster: New York, NY, USA, 2000; ISBN 9781471109416. [Google Scholar]
- Range, F.; Viranyi, Z. Social cognition and emotions underlying dog behavior. In The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behavior and Interactions with People; Serpell, J., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2017; pp. 182–209. [Google Scholar]
- Adachi, I.; Kuwahata, H.; Fujita, K. Dogs recall their owner’s face upon hearing the owner’s voice. Anim. Cogn. 2007, 10, 17–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Miklósi, Á. Communication, play and collaboration. In Dog Behaviour, Evolution and Cognition, 2nd ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2015; pp. 252–279. [Google Scholar]
- Miklósi, Á. Social learning and social problem solving. In Dog Behaviour, Evolution and Cognition, 2nd ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2015; pp. 280–298. [Google Scholar]
- Miklósi, Á. The organization of individual behaviour. In Dog Behaviour, Evolution and Cognition, 2nd ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2015; pp. 324–345. [Google Scholar]
- Hart, B.J.; Hart, L.A. Breed and gender differences in dog behavior. In The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behavior and Interactions with People; Serpell, J., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2017; pp. 118–132. [Google Scholar]
- Lockwood, R. Ethology, ecology and epidemiology of canine aggression. In The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behavior and Interactions with People; Serpell, J., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2017; pp. 160–181. [Google Scholar]
- Serpell, J.; Duffy, D.L.; Jagoe, J.A. Becoming a dog: Early experience and the development of behavior. In The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behavior and Interactions with People; Serpell, J., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2017; pp. 93–117. [Google Scholar]
- Byosiere, S.-E.; Espinosa, J.; Smuts, B. Integrating the function of play bows in adult pet dogs (Canis lupus familiaris). Behav. Processes 2016, 125, 106–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Quartana, A.; Siniscalchi, M.; Vallortigara, G. Asymmetric tail-wagging responses by dogs to different emotive stimuli. Curr. Biol. 2007, 17, R199–R201. [Google Scholar]
- Siniscalchi, M.; Lusito, R.; Vallortigara, G.; Quaranta, A. Seeing left- or right-asymmetric tail wagging produces different emotional responses in dogs. Curr. Biol. 2013, 23, 2279–2282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Leaver, S.D.A.; Reimchen, T.E. Behavioural responses of Canis familiaris to different tail lengths of a remotely-controlled life-size dog replica. Behaviour 2008, 145, 377–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tail Docking Statements. Available online: https://www.massfeddogs.org/Downloads/TailDockStatements.pdf (accessed on 22 April 2018).
- CKC Supports Freedom of Choice Despite BC Ban on Tail Docking. Available online: https://www.ckc.ca/en/News/2016/November/CKC-Supports-Freedom-of-Choice-Despite-BC-Ban-on-T (accessed on 22 April 2018).
- Mellor, D.J.; Murray, L. Effects of tail docking and castration on behaviour and plasma cortisol concentrations in young lambs. Res. Vet. Sci. 1989, 46, 387–391. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Mellor, D.J.; Murray, L. Changes in the cortisol responses of lambs to tail docking, castration and ACTH injection during the first seven days after birth. Res. Vet. Sci. 1989, 46, 392–395. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Mellor, D.J.; Molony, V.; Robertson, I.S. Effects of castration on behaviour and plasma cortisol concentrations in young lambs, kids and calves. Res. Vet. Sci. 1991, 51, 149–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Molony, V.; Kent, J.E.; Robertson, I.S. Behavioural responses of lambs of three ages in the first three hours after three methods of castration and tail docking. Res. Vet. Sci. 1993, 55, 236–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Molony, V.; Kent, J.E.; Robertson, I.S. Assessment of acute and chronic pain after different methods of castration of calves. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1995, 46, 33–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stillwell, H.J.; St. Louis, P.A.; Nippo, M.M.; Rhodes, R.C., III. Physiological responses associated with tail-docking in 3- and 7-day old lambs. J. Anim. Sci. 1994, 72 (Suppl. 1), 323. [Google Scholar]
- Petrie, N.J.; Mellor, D.J.; Stafford, K.J.; Bruce, R.A.; Ward, R.N. Cortisol responses of calves to two methods of tail docking used with or without local Anaesthesia. N. Z. Vet. J. 1996, 44, 4–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mellor, D.J.; Cook, C.J.; Stafford, K.J. Quantifying some responses to pain as a stressor. In The Biology of Animal Stress: Assessment and Implications for Welfare; Moberg, G.P., Mench, J.A., Eds.; CAB International: Wallingford, UK, 2000; pp. 171–198. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, C.B.; Sylvester, S.P.; Stafford, K.J.; Mitchinson, S.L.; Ward, R.N.; Mellor, D.J. Effects of age on the electroencephalographic response to castration in lambs anaesthetised using halothane in oxygen from birth to 6 weeks old. Vet. Anaesth. Analg. 2009, 36, 273–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Taylor, A.A.; Weary, D.M. Vocal responses of piglets to castration: Identifying procedural sources of pain. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2000, 70, 17–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sutherland, M.A.; Tucker, C.B. The long and short of it: A review of tail docking in farm animals. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2011, 135, 179–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sutherland, M.A. Welfare implications of invasive piglet husbandry procedures, methods of alleviation and alternatives: A review. N. Z. Vet. J. 2015, 63, 52–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Anand, K.J.S.; Hickey, P.R. Pain and its effects in the human neonate and fetus. N. Eng. J. Med. 1987, 317, 1321–1329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Craig, K.D.; Whitfield, M.F.; Grunau, R.V.E.; Linton, J.; Hadjistavropoulos, H.D. Pain in the preterm neonate: Behavioural and physiological indices. Pain 1993, 52, 287–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schuster, A.; Lenard, H.G. Pain in newborns and prematures: Current practice and knowledge. Brain Dev. 1990, 12, 459–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goubet, N.; Clifton, R.K.; Shah, B. Learning about pain in preterm newborns. J. Dev. Behav. Pediat. 2001, 22, 418–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marchant, A. ‘Neonates do not feel pain’: A critical review. Biosci. Horizons 2014, 7, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mellor, D.J.; Gregory, N.G. Responsiveness, behavioural arousal and awareness in fetal and newborn lambs: Experimental, practical and therapeutic implications. N. Z. Vet. J. 2003, 51, 2–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mellor, D.J.; Diesch, T.J.; Gunn, A.J.; Bennet, L. The importance of ‘awareness’ for understanding fetal pain. Brain Res. Rev. 2005, 49, 455–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mellor, D.J.; Diesch, T.J. Onset of sentience: The potential for suffering in fetal and newborn farm animals. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2006, 100, 48–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mellor, D.J.; Diesch, T.J. Birth and hatching: Key events in the onset of ‘awareness’ in lambs and chicks. N. Z. Vet. J. 2007, 55, 51–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Diesch, T.J.; Mellor, D.J.; Johnson, C.B.; Lentle, R.G. Responsiveness to painful stimuli in anaesthetised newborn and young animals of varying neurological maturity (wallaby joeys, rat pups and lambs). AATEX J. 2008, 14, 549–552. [Google Scholar]
- Mellor, D.J.; Diesch, T.J.; Gunn, A.J.; Bennet, L. Fetal ‘awareness’ and ‘pain’: What precautions should be taken to safeguard fetal welfare during experiments? AATEX J. 2008, 14, 79–83. [Google Scholar]
- Mellor, D.J.; Diesch, T.J.; Johnson, C.B. When do mammalian young become sentient? ALTEX 2010, 27, 275–280. [Google Scholar]
- Mellor, D.J. Galloping colts, fetal feelings and reassuring regulations: Putting animal welfare science into practice. J. Vet. Med. Educ. 2010, 37, 94–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Campbell, M.L.H.; Mellor, D.J.; Sandøe, P. How should the welfare of fetal and neurologically immature postnatal animals be protected? Anim. Welf. 2014, 23, 369–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mellor, D.J.; Lentle, R.G. Survival implications of the development of behavioural responsiveness and awareness in different groups of mammalian young. N. Z. Vet. J. 2015, 63, 131–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mellor, D. Transitions in neuroinhibition and neuroactivation in neurologically mature young at birth, including the potential role of thoracic compression during labour; page 7. In Aleman, M.; Weich, K.M.; Madigan, J.E. Survey of veterinarians using a novel physical compression squeeze procedure in the management of neonatal maladjustment syndrome in foals. Animals 2017, 7, 69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chang, P.; Fabrizi, L.; Olhede, S.; Fitzgerald, M. The development of nociceptive network activity in the somatosensory cortex of freely moving rat pups. Cereb. Cortex 2016, 26, 4513–4523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Diesch, T.J.; Mellor, D.J.; Johnson, C.B.; Lentle, R.G. Electroencephalographic responses to tail clamping in anaesthetised rat pups. Lab. Anim. 2009, 43, 224–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fox, M.W. Postnatal development of the EEG in the dog—1. J. Small Anim. Prac. 1967, 8, 71–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fox, M.W. Postnatal development of the EEG in the dog—II. J. Small Anim. Prac. 1967, 8, 77–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fox, M.W. Postnatal development of the EEG in the dog—III. J. Small Anim. Prac. 1967, 8, 109–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Khazipov, R.; Luhmann, H. Early patterns of electrical activity in the developing cerebral cortex of humans and rodents. Trends Neurosci. 2006, 29, 414–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ren, K.; Anseloni, V.; Zou, S.-P.; Wade, E.; Novikova, S.; Ennis, M.; Traub, R.; Gold, M.; Dubner, R.; Lidow, M. The sensitive period and developmental time course for long-term alterations in pain responsivity after neonatal local inflammatory insult. Pain 2004, 110, 588–596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- LaPrairie, J.L.; Murphy, A.Z. Long term impact of neonatal injury in male and female rats: Sex differences, mechanisms and clinical implications. Front. Neuroendocr. 2010, 31, 193–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Taddio, A.; Goldbach, M.; Ipp, M.; Stevens, B.; Koren, G. Effect of neonatal circumcision on pain responses during vaccination in boys. Lancet 1995, 345, 291–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taddio, A.; Katz, J.; Ilersich, A.L.; Koren, G. Effect of neonatal circumcision on pain response during subsequent routine vaccination. Lancet 1997, 349, 599–603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Giminiani, P.; Edwards, S.A.; Malcolm, E.M.; Leach, M.C.; Herskin, M.S.; Sandercock, D.A. Characterisation of short- and long-term mechanical sensitisation following surgical tail amputation in pigs. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 4827. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McCracken, L.; Waran, N.; Mitchinson, S.; Johnson, C.B. Effect of age at castration on behavioural response to subsequent tail docking in lambs. Vet. Anaesth. Analges. 2010, 37, 375–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Impey, S. The Effect of Early Post-Natal Castration on Subsequent Electroencephalogram Response to Tail Docking in Lambs. Master’s Thesis, Massey University, Palmerston North, New Zealand, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Gregory, N.G. Pain. In Physiology and Behaviour of Animal Suffering; Blackwell Publishing: Oxford, UK, 2004; pp. 94–140. [Google Scholar]
- Campbell, J.N.; Meyer, R.A. Mechanisms of neuropathic pain. Neuron 2006, 52, 77–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gross, T.L.; Carr, S.H. Amputation neuroma of docked tails in dogs. Vet. Path. 1990, 27, 61–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Herskin, M.S.; Thodberg, K.; Jensen, H.E. Effects of tail docking and docking length on neuroanatomical changes in healed tail tips of pigs. Animal 2015, 9, 677–681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sandercock, D.A.; Smith, S.H.; Di Giminiani, P.; Edwards, S.A. Histopathological characterization of tail injury and traumatic neuroma development after tail docking in piglets. J. Comp. Path. 2016, 155, 40–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- French, N.P.; Morgan, K.L. Neuromas in docked lamb’s tails. Res. Vet. Sci. 1992, 52, 389–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mellor, D.J. Farmer and public perspectives on neonatal lamb mortality. Welf. Pulse 2010, 6, 12–13. [Google Scholar]
- Mellor, D.J.; Diesch, T.J.; Johnson, C.B. Should mammalian fetuses be excluded from regulations protecting animals during experiments? ALTEX 2010, 27, 199–202. [Google Scholar]
- Darwin, C. The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1872. [Google Scholar]
- Lorenz, K. Die angeborenen Formen möglicher Erfahrung. Z. Tierpsychol. 1943, 5, 235–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fullard, W.; Reiling, A.M. An investigation of Lorenz’s “Babyness”. Child Dev. 1976, 47, 1191–1193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gould, S.J. The Panda’s Thumb; W.W. Norton and Company: New York, NY, USA, 1980. [Google Scholar]
- Morreall, J. Cuteness. Br. J. Aesthet. 1991, 31, 39–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morris, P.H.; Reddy, V.; Bunting, R.C. The survival of the cutest: Who’s responsible for the evolution of the teddy bear? Anim. Behav. 1995, 50, 1697–1700. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lorenz, K. Studies in Animal and Human Behavior; Methuen: London, UK, 1971; Volume II. [Google Scholar]
- Brosch, T.; Sander, D.; Scherer, K.R. That Baby Caught My Eye … Attention Capture by Infant Faces. Emotion 2007, 7, 685–689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Glocker, M.L.; Langleben, D.D.; Ruparel, K.; Loughead, J.W.; Gur, R.C.; Sachser, N. Baby schema in infant faces induces cuteness perception and motivation for caretaking in adults. Ethology 2009, 115, 257–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bowlby, J. An ethological approach to research in child development. Br. J. Med. Psychol. 1957, 30, 230–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bowlby, J. Attachment and loss. In Volume 1: Attachment; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Kringelbach, M.L.; Lehtonen, A.; Squire, S.; Harvey, A.G.; Craske, M.G.; Holliday, I.E.; Green, A.L.; Aziz, T.Z.; Hansen, P.C.; Cornelissen, P.L.; et al. A specific and rapid neural signature for parental instinct. PLoS ONE 2008, 3, e1664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lawrence, E.A. Neoteny in American perceptions of animals. In Perceptions of Animals in American Culture; Hoage, R.J., Ed.; Smithsonian Institution Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Archer, J. Why do people love their pets? Evol. Hum. Behav. 1997, 18, 237–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stafford, K. The Welfare of Dogs; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Allin, J.T.; Banks, E.M. Effects of temperature on ultrasound production by infant albino rats. Dev. Psychobiol. 1971, 4, 149–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Newman, J.D. Neural circuits underlying crying and cry responding in mammals. Behav. Brain Res. 2007, 182, 155–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Noonan, G.J.; Rand, J.S.; Blackshaw, J.K.; Priest, J. Tail docking in dogs: A sample of attitudes of veterinarians and dog breeders in Queensland. Aust. Vet. J. 1996, 73, 86–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mellor, D.J. Enhancing animal welfare by creating opportunities for ‘positive affective engagement’. N. Z. Vet. J. 2015, 63, 3–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mellor, D.J. Moving beyond the ‘Five Freedoms’ by updating the ‘Five Provisions’ and introducing aligned ‘Animal Welfare Aims’. Animals 2016, 6, 59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mellor, D.J.; Beausoleil, N.J. Extending the ‘Five Domains’ model for animal welfare assessment to incorporate positive welfare states. Anim. Welf. 2015, 24, 241–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mellor, D.J.; Patterson-Kane, E.; Stafford, K.J. The Sciences of Animal Welfare; Wiley-Blackwell Publishing: Oxford, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Green, T.C.; Mellor, D.J. Extending ideas about animal welfare assessment to include ‘quality of life’ and related concepts. N. Z. Vet. J. 2011, 59, 316–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Merker, B. Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: A challenge for neuroscience and medicine. Behav. Brain Sci. 2007, 30, 63–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Panksepp, J. Emotional feelings originate below the neocortex: Toward a neurobiology of the soul. Commentary on Merker, B. Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: A challenge for neuro-science and medicine. Behav. Brain Sci. 2007, 30, 101–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- USA Kennel Clubs Tail Docking Statements 2009. Available online: https://www.massfeddogs.org/Downloads/TailDockStatements.pdf (accessed on 30 May 2018).
- Fadeyemi, A.J. Tail docking in dogs: Evaluation of current practices and ethical aspects in southwest Nigeria. J. Vet. Med. Anim. Health 2014, 6, 18–24. [Google Scholar]
- Eyarefe, O.D.; Oguntoye, C.O. Cosmetic tail docking: An overview of abuse and report of an interesting case. BMC Vet. Res. 2016, 12, 41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mellor, D.J. Positive welfare states and promoting environment-focused and animal-to-animal interactive behaviours. N. Z. Vet. J. 2015, 63, 9–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- The Evolution of the Legal Status of Animals: From Things to Sentient Beings. 2018. Available online: https://www.globalanimalnetwork.org/evolution-legal-status-animals-things-sentient-beings (accessed on 28 April 2018).
- UDAW: Universal Declaration on Animal Welfare. 2014. Available online: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Universal_Declaration_on_Animal_Welfare (accessed on 28 April 2018).
- World Organisation for Animal Health: Paris, France. 2017. Available online: http://www.oie.int/fileadmin/Home/eng/Animal_Welfare/docs/pdf/Others/EN_OIE_AW_Strategy.pdf (accessed on 10 May 2018).
- Diesel, G.; Pfeiffer, D.; Crispin, S.; Brodbelt, D. Risk factors for tail injuries in dogs in Great Britain. Vet. Rec. 2010, 166, 812–817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Tail Behaviour | Emotional State and Intentions |
---|---|
Fast tail wag | Excited |
Broad tail wag, wide swings pull the hips from side to side | Happily greeting special individual |
Broad tail wag | Friendly |
Slight tail wag, each swing only small | Greeting |
Tail lower than horizontal but some distance from the legs, sometimes swings back and forth | Unconcerned, relaxed |
Tail half lowered, with slow wag | Insecure, not sure what to do next |
Tail down, near hind legs, legs straight, tail swings back and forth slowly | May feel unwell, somewhat depressed or in moderate pain |
Tail down, near hind legs, hind legs bent to lower the body | Timid, apprehensive, insecure |
Tail tucked between hind legs | Fearful, anxious |
Tail horizontal, not stiff, pointing away from the dog | Focused attention |
Tail horizontal, stiff, pointing straight out, away from the dog | Initial challenge, might lead to aggression |
Tail up and slightly curved over back | Confident, feels in control |
© 2018 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Mellor, D.J. Tail Docking of Canine Puppies: Reassessment of the Tail’s Role in Communication, the Acute Pain Caused by Docking and Interpretation of Behavioural Responses. Animals 2018, 8, 82. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani8060082
Mellor DJ. Tail Docking of Canine Puppies: Reassessment of the Tail’s Role in Communication, the Acute Pain Caused by Docking and Interpretation of Behavioural Responses. Animals. 2018; 8(6):82. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani8060082
Chicago/Turabian StyleMellor, David J. 2018. "Tail Docking of Canine Puppies: Reassessment of the Tail’s Role in Communication, the Acute Pain Caused by Docking and Interpretation of Behavioural Responses" Animals 8, no. 6: 82. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani8060082
APA StyleMellor, D. J. (2018). Tail Docking of Canine Puppies: Reassessment of the Tail’s Role in Communication, the Acute Pain Caused by Docking and Interpretation of Behavioural Responses. Animals, 8(6), 82. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani8060082