Nothing Special   »   [go: up one dir, main page]

Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 23 February 2024
Sec. Psychology of Language
This article is part of the Research Topic Words and Emotions View all 7 articles

Power and vulnerability: managing sensitive language in organizational communication

\r\nPatrick G. T. Healey
Patrick G. T. Healey1*Prashant KharePrashant Khare1Ignacio CastroIgnacio Castro1Gareth Tyson,Gareth Tyson1,2Mladen KaranMladen Karan1Ravi Shekhar,Ravi Shekhar1,3Stephen McQuistin,Stephen McQuistin4,5Colin PerkinsColin Perkins4Matthew Purver,Matthew Purver1,6
  • 1School of Electronic Engineering and Computer Science, Queen Mary University of London, London, United Kingdom
  • 2Department of Electronic and Computer Engineering, Hong Kong University of Science and Technology, Hong Kong, China
  • 3School of Computer Science and Electronic Engineering, University of Essex, Colchester, United Kingdom
  • 4School of Computing Science, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, United Kingdom
  • 5School of Computer Science, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, United Kingdom
  • 6Jožef Stefan Institute, Ljubljana, Slovenia

Organizational responsibilities can give people power but also expose them to scrutiny. This tension leads to divergent predictions about the use of potentially sensitive language: power might license it, while exposure might inhibit it. Analysis of peoples' language use in a large corpus of organizational emails using standardized Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC) measures shows a systematic difference in the use of words with potentially sensitive (ethnic, religious, or political) connotations. People in positions of relative power are ~3 times less likely to use sensitive words than people more junior to them. The tendency to avoid potentially sensitive language appears to be independent of whether other people are using sensitive language in the same email exchanges, and also independent of whether these words are used in a sensitive context. These results challenge a stereotype about language use and the exercise of power. They suggest that, in at least some circumstances, the exposure and accountability associated with organizational responsibilities are a more significant influence on how people communicate than social power.

1 Introduction

There is a belief that people in positions of power can speak more freely than their juniors because they are less vulnerable to criticism. This belief has informed work on politeness in conversation, with the use of impolite language sometimes identified with the exercise of social power (see e.g., Bousfield and Locher, 2008; Danescu-Niculescu-Mizil et al., 2012; Gilbert, 2012; Wang, 2021; Paik and van Swol, 2022). There is also a contrary belief: people in positions of power are monitored and held to account in ways that junior people are not, making them more cautious about what they say and how they say it. We explore these conflicting beliefs in the context of a large corpus of email communication in an international standards organization. The results suggest that, in at least some contexts, vulnerability is a more significant influence on language use than relative social power.

Brown and Levinson's (1987) landmark study of politeness observed that sensitive topics, such as “politics, race, religion, women's liberation” (p. 314), create a social risk for both speakers and hearers. These topics are sensitive because they touch on personal and group identities that are often implicated in interpersonal and group conflict (Triandis, 2000; Levy et al., 2022). In Brown and Levinson's (1987) politeness theory, raising these topics is a potentially divisive, face threatening act because it can suggest that the speaker is insensitive to the hearer's desire to have their social identity or positive face respected. Similarly, these topics can pose a threat to the self-image of a speaker if, for example, a hearer publicly challenges the appropriateness of their remarks.

Judgements about what kinds of talk are (in)appropriate are complex and vary between times, contexts, individuals, and speech communities. Moreover, people can sometimes introduce sensitive topics with the intention of strengthening a relationship, e.g., as a form of self-disclosure or demonstration of informality or community solidarity, just as much as with the intention of threatening a relationship (see e.g., Schnurr et al., 2008).

A significant complicating factor in judgements of what kinds of language are appropriate in a particular situation is social power, defined by Brown and Levinson as mutually agreed relative status. Differences in social power distort the equilibrium of “mutual vulnerability” to face threats (Brown and Levinson, 1987, p. 320). What is the direction of these effects? Brown and Levinson introduce a Power Hypothesis. Roughly, the more powerful a person is, the less vulnerable they are to threats to their own self-esteem (positive face) and the more able they are to resist requests or instructions that might restrict their own freedom of action (negative face). Conversely, they are also less concerned with attending to the face needs of others and, by definition, they are in a position to restrict other people's freedom of action. The Power Hypothesis has informed most of the subsequent quantitative work on politeness and social hierarchies.

An alternative intuition, which we gloss as the Exposure Hypothesis, is that people with higher social status are potentially more vulnerable to face threats. People with significant decision-making responsibilities may also be subject to additional social pressures. They often need to work to build a consensus around decisions and to anticipate and manage potential challenges and objections. Most importantly, they are often also held publicly or legally accountable for those decisions in ways that people without executive power are not. Documents such as emails provide a record that may be scrutinized by a variety of people, in a variety of contexts and over unpredictable timespans. As far as we are aware, the Exposure Hypothesis and the intuition behind it has not previously been systematically tested.

These two hypotheses make contrasting predictions. The Power Hypothesis suggests that people with more social power should be more likely to use potentially sensitive words, whereas the Exposure Hypothesis suggests that they should be less likely.

Previous work testing the relationship between patterns of language use and power in large datasets of institutional communication has found mixed results. Gilbert's (2012) study of the Enron corpus looked at whether differences in language use could distinguish between emails sent up and down the corporate hierarchy. Although some specific phrases predict the direction of the communication in this corpus -e.g., “the ability to,” “attach,” and “I took” associate with emails up the corporate hierarchy and “have you been,” “to manage the,” and “you gave” do not- there is no clear generalization across the two sets of predictor phrases. A follow-up LIWC analysis (see below) looking at several different generic aspects of language use found a difference in the level of evidence of cognitive processes suggesting that junior people may prefer to provide senior people with answers rather than reasoning (Gilbert, 2012). Eckhaus and Sheaffer (2018) analyzed changes in pronoun use over time by two of the most senior managers at Enron and connected a shift toward use of first person singular (“I”) to an increase in managerial hubris over time. Danescu-Niculescu-Mizil et al. (2012) analyzed Wikipedia edit discussions and oral arguments before the US high court. They found evidence that people with lower social power tend to accommodate their language use -defined as matching of function words- more to the language use of those higher in power than vice versa. However, Tan et al. (2016) complicate this picture, finding that lower accommodation -defined here in terms of match/mismatch of content words- appears to make arguments more persuasive in Reddit discussions. Cotterill et al. (2015) provide data from an experimental task in which power is directly controlled using a manipulation of roles. They show that training a classifier on a variety of stylometric features including characters per word, punctuation marks, interjections, polite expressions, and function words, can distinguish between messages up, across, and down the experimentally created hierarchy.

Overall, the picture from previous studies is mixed and, while there is evidence of a systematic relationship between language use and social power, it is not clear exactly what that relationship is. There are several factors contributing to this mixed picture. There is a lack of consensus about what aspects of language use are measured: e.g., function words vs. content words vs. stylometric features; the underlying processes assumed to be associated with social power e.g., accommodation vs. cognitive demands; and about the possible connections between these processes and features of language use. It is also likely that norms about what is and is not appropriate vary across different institutional contexts and cultures and we return to this point in the discussion.

To try to disentangle some of these issues and to test Brown and Levinsons' original predictions, we focus directly on language relating to politics, race, and religion. We analyze a large corpus of organizational email communication between people with different levels of social power operationalized in terms of their organizational role. We use this data to test the contrasting predictions of the Power Hypothesis and the Exposure Hypothesis for the use of potentially sensitive language.

2 Methods

The dataset consists of emails produced by the Internet Engineering Task Force (IETF). The IETF is a debate-based, consensus-driven forum that brings together multiple stakeholders (industry, academia, civil society) to agree on many of the key technical standards (e.g., TCP/IP, HTTP) that ensure the Internet works. The IETF is structured into Working Groups (WGs), each with a particular technical focus (e.g., HTTP protocol) and a special mailing list (most IETF works takes place via email). WG chairs facilitate the work with responsibilities including moderating mailing lists, organizing meetings, setting the agenda, and judging consensus on major decisions. We use two publicly available data sources: the IETF mail archives and the Datatracker.1 The mail archives cover WG activities, meetings, and administration.2 The Datatracker provides information about organizational roles of participants.3

The sample consists of all WG email communications in 2019, limited to the last “pre-COVID” year for which we have complete data. We chose this year to avoid the possible impact of the sudden shift to online communication during the pandemic. Following the approach used by McQuistin et al. (2021) and Khare et al. (2022), each email is coded for the WG list in which it occurs and the identity of the sender and then anonymized as a numeric ID with their organizational role. We use only the aggregated data in the statistical analysis below. We distinguish three organizational roles as an index of the relative level of social power someone has in a particular workgroup and its associated email list:

1. Group chair codes whether an email is produced by the current chair of the workgroup in which the email occurs.

2. Other chair codes whether an email is produced by someone who is the chair of a different workgroup from the one in which the email occurs.

3. None codes whether an email originates from people who are not and have not been (at the time of sampling) a workgroup chair.

We order relative social power in these roles in a particular workgroup as Group Chair > Other Chair > None on the basis of their relative influence over group decision making and reporting responsibilities with respect to the workgroup and the IETF.

Three Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC-22: Boyd et al., 2022) categories are used to provide an index of potentially sensitive language. We use the pre-defined LIWC measures closest to those cited by Brown and Levinson (see above):

1. Politics: words commonly used in political discussions (e.g., congress, parliament, president, democratic) or legal (court, law) discourse.

2. Ethnicity: words that identify national, regional, linguistic, ethnic, or racial identities.4

3. Religion: use of religious words such as “church, altar, god, Christmas, hell, mosque, temple.”5

The LIWC categories are standardized measures that have been validated by human judges and explored in a variety of datasets (Boyd et al., 2022). It is important to note that these three LIWC-22 categories encode only mentions of specific words in a category; context is not considered and there is no attempt to disambiguate words that might have alternative senses in this technical domain. This means that, for example, questions about European IP address assignment policies and comments about British humor are both categorized as LIWC Ethnicity. We do not attempt to separate these cases but address this point in the discussion.

3 Results

Each email is coded according to the working group it is in and who sent it (Group Chair /Other Chair/None). The emails are pre-processed to remove subject lines, headers, and any embedded quotes from previous emails, yielding 101,857 texts by 2,300 individuals across 176 group mailing lists. Each text is assigned a score on each of the three language categories using the LIWC software and the resulting scores are averaged for each individual sender for statistical analysis.

The LIWC category scores are positively skewed, so Generalized Linear Mixed Model (GLMM) with a Gamma Distribution6 and Log link is used for the statistical analysis, but means are reported on the original scale. Organizational Role (Group Chair /Other Chair/None) is included as a fixed factor. Effect sizes for the pairwise comparisons are computed using Hedges g because of unequal sample sizes in the comparisons. The three analyses indicate a consistent pattern across the different LIWC categories.

GLMM analysis of the LIWC Politics measure shows an overall main effect of Role [F(2,790) = 20.5, p < 0.001]. Pairwise comparisons show that None are reliably more likely than either Other Chair or Group Chair to use political words [Group Chair vs. None: t(790) = −6.13, p < 0.001, Hedges' g = 0.32; Other Chair vs. None: t(790) = −2.83, p = 0.010, = Hedges' g = 0.21]. Group Chair and Other Chair are not reliably different [t(790) = 1.72, p = 0.086].

GLMM analysis of LIWC Ethnicity also shows a main effect of Role [F(2,227) = 25.7, p < 0.001]. Pairwise comparisons show that None use more Ethnicity-related words than either Other Chair or Group Chair: [Group Chair vs. None: t(227) = −3.89, p < 0.001, Hedges' g = 0.33; Other Chair vs. None: t(227) = −4.478, p = 0.010, Hedges' g = 0.39]. Group Chair and Other Chair are not reliably different [t(227) = 0.93, p = 0.354].

GLMM for LIWC Religion also shows an overall effect of Role [F(2,408) = 5.12, p = 0.006]. The pairwise comparisons between Group Chair and None [t(408) = −2.76, p = 0.018, Hedges' g = 1.5] and Other Chair and None [t(408) = −2.39, p = 0.03, Hedges' g = 0.95] are also reliable.

The results for all three analyses are illustrated in Figure 1. The consistent pattern is that people with organizational responsibilities (Group Chair or Other Chair) are less likely to use words potentially connected with sensitive topics than those without organizational responsibilities (None), even though they are all part of the same discussions on the same email lists. Averaging across the two Chair categories, the means indicate that people in positions of social power are 3.2 times less likely to use sensitive language overall than those who are not (by category: Politics: 2.0 times; Religion 2.3 times; Ethnicity 5.5 times).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Patterns of language use by organizational role. Estimated marginal means for the LIWC categories: (A) Politics, (B) Ethnicity, and (C) Religion. Error bars show 95% confidence intervals.

It is worth noting that, in absolute terms, potentially sensitive language use is rare in this dataset, as might be expected for primarily work-related communication. The modal LIWC score in both raw data and the averages scores is zero. For comparison, the reported mean percentage occurrence of each category in the present dataset, natural conversation, and Twitter (now X) is summarized in Table 1 (Boyd et al., 2022). Despite the low overall incidence of potentially sensitive language, the results show systematic differences in language use according to organizational role with medium to large effect sizes.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Comparison of relative occurrence of words in the LIWC Politics, religion and ethnicity categories in the IETF corpus, and the natural conversation and twitter samples reported by Boyd et al. (2022).

Post-hoc Chi2 tests were used to assess whether people who use one form of sensitive language are also more likely to use other forms.7 The data for each participant and each LIWC category were recoded into binary variables; 0 where the mean instances of a category were 0 and 1 otherwise. The results show reliable associations between each pair of LIWC categories [Politics and Ethnicity: Chi (1)2 = 184, p < 0.001, n = 2,209; Politics and Religion: Chi (1)2 = 342 p < 0.001, n = 2,209; Religion and Ethnicity: Chi (1)2 = 403, p < 0.001, n = 2,209]. Overall, people who use one category of sensitive language are also more likely to use other.

4 Discussion

People who have more social power within the IETF, i.e., WG chairs, are systematically less likely to use potentially sensitive words. Given that these interactions all occur on shared mailing lists, this also implies that when WG chairs encounter these words they tend not to reciprocate or align on them. This pattern of results is consistent with the Exposure hypothesis but incompatible with the Power hypothesis.

As noted in the introduction, prior corpus work has typically assumed the basic relationship between power and impoliteness and then sought to identify the linguistic markers that best index that relationship. The analysis reported here also finds a systematic relationship between language and power but tests the direction of this relationship, i.e., whether relative social power is linked to more or less use of potentially sensitive language. The results suggests that, in at least some contexts, the relationship runs in the opposite direction to the one normally assumed: people in positions of power appear to avoid using potentially sensitive words. This finding also runs counter to the more general stereotype of assertive and competitive leaders who use language to project power and confidence, sometimes in overtly hostile ways (see e.g., Tepper, 2007; Koenig et al., 2011; Jordan et al., 2019).

The results have an interesting possible parallel with the language of politicians who also avoid potentially sensitive expressions in exposed, public contexts (see e.g., Bavelas et al., 1990; Obeng, 1997; Bull, 2015). Equivocation by politicians appears to be specifically associated with contexts in which they must contend with the conflicting interests of different groups in building a broader coalition. This description could equally apply to the situations IETF workgroup chairs encounter. However, the LIWC categories studied here relate to social identities and not to conflicting technical or professional interests. It would be interesting to investigate whether sensitivity to potential organizational conflict or sensitivity to wider social norms are more influential in these effects.

Importantly, as noted above, the LIWC categories are not sensitive to linguistic context. So, for example, the use of the word “Christian” as a first name or as a reference to religious belief are not distinguished. One possibility is that this insensitivity to context merely adds noise to a large dataset; however, the effects are still large enough that we observe systematic differences in usage. Another possibility is that the context independence of the LIWC categories accurately reflects a degree of context independence in the way people process potentially sensitive words (see e.g., Madan et al., 2017). Speculatively, people might avoid potentially sensitive language even in apparently unproblematic contexts because alternative meanings are still activated during language production and comprehension (see e.g., Rodd, 2018; Hansen et al., 2019). Something similar may be involved in the removal of, for example, color terms from ostensibly non-racial contexts such as “black cloak” and “white with fear” (see O'Neill, 2023). Exploration of this kind of generalized inhibitory effect would require controlled experiments on processing of LIWC target words in different linguistic and social contexts.

The relationship between language use and social power observed here is likely to depend to some extent on the institutional context and especially on the relative exposure, i.e., accountability and transparency, of a decision-making process. The IETF is committed to a high degree of accountability and transparency, as demonstrated by the way it documents and publishes its organizational communication in a searchable archive. This relatively high level of public exposure may itself lead people to be more cautious in what they say, although this pressure should apply equally to group chairs and group members. The IETF also emphasizes a relatively open, egalitarian, consensus-building culture that may promote a more careful, considered approach to organizational communication than other organizations (although this culture has also been the subject of criticism, see Cath, 2021). An interesting direction for future research may be to explore how organizational practice and openness affects language use, comparing the IETF data with other public communities (e.g., open-source software development communities, public discussion forums) and more closed organizations to explore whether the behavior observed correlates with organizational openness rather than consensus-driven operation.

An important group of alternative, trait-based explanations for the effects reported above is that the kind of people who move into more senior organizational roles are, by disposition, experience, or training, just less likely to use sensitive language. In this case, the observed differences in language use could be due to the kinds of individuals who typically occupy each role rather than the effects of the situation they find themselves in. The data presented here are compatible with both interpretations, although other analyses for different aspects of language use in the same corpus suggest organizational situation may be more important than individual disposition (see also Bavelas et al., 1990; Khare et al., 2023). Future corpus research could explore this issue using longitudinal analysis of the IETF mailing list archives as participants change roles or carry out synchronic comparisons of people in different situations. Whether the working group chairs adjust their language due to the organizational situation they are in or because of the type of person they are, both explanations run counter to the assumption that power should be identified with a tendency to use more sensitive language i.e., with the ability to threaten the “face” of others and to resist face threats by others (Eckhaus and Sheaffer, 2018; Jordan et al., 2019). The analysis of sensitive language use needs to be replicated for other large datasets, especially those that represent different organizational contexts with different forms of accountability. The results suggest that quantitative analyses of the relationship between language use and power need to attend to both what is licensed and what may be inhibited and to how this interacts with situations of use. Future work also needs to allow for the relatively fast pace of changes in norms about (in)appropriateness of specific kinds of language use and changes in both leadership stereotypes and observed leadership practices (Lord et al., 2017).

Quantitative analysis also needs to be complemented by in-depth qualitative work that investigates the contexts of use captured by the LIWC categories and how people perceive and, especially, respond to uses of sensitive language by different people in different organizational contexts (see Bousfield and Locher, 2008). Another interesting avenue for future research would be to explore how people in different organizational roles perceive their relative levels of social power and social exposure.

Although positions of social power are often thought of as positions of strength, the results suggest that, in at least some contexts, they can also be positions of vulnerability because of their heightened exposure and this can be reflected in more cautious, circumspect patterns of language use.

Data availability statement

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found at: https://mailarchive.ietf.org/ and https://datatracker.ietf.org/– administrative database of IETF.

Ethics statement

Ethical approval was not required for the studies involving humans, because this was a retrospective analysis of publicly available data from the IETF. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. People provided written informed consent for their data to be published (publicly) in the IETF archive and the IETF approved the use of this data for the study.

Author contributions

PH: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review & editing. PK: Data curation, Methodology, Software, Writing—review & editing. IC: Methodology, Supervision, Writing—review & editing. GT: Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing—review & editing. MK: Data curation, Methodology, Software, Writing—review & editing. RS: Data curation, Methodology, Software, Writing—review & editing. SM: Data curation, Methodology, Software, Writing—review & editing. CP: Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing—review & editing. MP: Funding acquisition, Methodology, Supervision, Writing—review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This project was funded by the Engineering and Physical Sciences Research Council (EPSRC) through the project “Streamlining Social Decision Making for Improved Internet Standards” (EP/S033564/1 and EP/S036075/1). MP was also partly supported by the Slovenian Research Agency via research core funding for the programme Knowledge Technologies (P2-0103) and the project Sovrag (Hate speech in contemporary conceptualizations of nationalism, racism, gender and migration, J5-3102).

Conflict of interest

CP is Chair of the Internet Research Task Force (https://irtf.org/) and an ex-officio member of the Internet Architecture Board (https://iab.org/). He was an IETF working group chair from 1998 to 2008 and from 2016 to July 2023.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

1. ^https://mailarchive.ietf.org/

2. ^https://datatracker.ietf.org/ – administrative database of IETF.

3. ^See both http://www.ietf.org/about/note-well/ and the IETF privacy policy available at http://www.ietf.org/privacy-statement/. IETF leadership confirmed that our work conforms with acceptable use.

4. ^Words reflecting racial or ethnic slurs and swearwords are excluded from these categories and included as part of the LIWC swear category.

5. ^This list is not exhaustive, but a full definition is not provided in the LIWC documentation, so we take some of this on faith.

6. ^As a result, the degrees of freedom vary across the three analyses because Gamma distribution ignores zeros.

7. ^We are grateful to one of our anonymous reviewers for suggesting this analysis and the inclusion of comparative estimates of overall incidence of sensitive language use.

References

Bavelas, J. B., Black, A., Chovil, N., and Mullett, J. (1990). Equivocal Communication. London: Sage Publications, Inc.

Google Scholar

Bousfield, D., and Locher, M. A. (2008). Impoliteness in Language: Studies on its Interplay With Power in Theory and Practice, Vol. 21. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.

Google Scholar

Boyd, R. L., Ashokkumar, A., Seraj, S., and Pennebaker, J. W. (2022). The Development and Psychometric Properties of LIWC-22. Austin, TX: University of Texas at Austin, 1–47.

Google Scholar

Brown, P., and Levinson, S. C. (1987). Politeness: Some Universals in Language Usage, Vol. 4. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Bull, P. (2015). Political Language and Persuasive Communication. Language, Discourse and Social Psychology. London: Palgrave Macmillan, 255–275.

Google Scholar

Cath, C. (2021). The technology we choose to create: human rights advocacy in the internet engineering task force. Telecommun. Policy 45, 102144. doi: 10.1016/j.telpol.2021.102144

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cotterill, R., Muir, K., Joinson, A., and Dewdney, N. (2015). Identifying linguistic correlates of social power. IJCLA 11, 1–15. Available online at: http://www.ijcla.bahripublications.com/2015-1/IJCLA-2015-1-pp-011-024-Identifying-Linguistic-Correlates.pdf

Google Scholar

Danescu-Niculescu-Mizil, C., Lee, L., Pang, B., and Kleinberg, J. (2012). “Echoes of power: Language effects and power differences in social interaction,” in Proceedings of the 21st International Conference on World Wide Web (New York, NY: Association for Computing Machinery), 699–708.

Google Scholar

Eckhaus, E., and Sheaffer, Z. (2018). Managerial hubris detection: the case of Enron. Risk Manage. 20, 304–325. doi: 10.1057/s41283-018-0037-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbert, E. (2012). “Phrases that signal workplace hierarchy,” in Proceedings of the ACM 2012 conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work (New York, NY: Association for Computing Machinery), 1037–1046.

Google Scholar

Hansen, S. J., McMahon, K. L., and de Zubicaray, G. I. (2019). The neurobiology of taboo language processing: fMRI evidence during spoken word production. Soc. Cognit. Aff. Neurosci. 14, 271–279. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsz009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jordan, K. N., Sterling, J., Pennebaker, J. W., and Boyd, R. L. (2019). Examining long-term trends in politics and culture through language of political leaders and cultural institutions. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 116, 3476–3481. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1811987116

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Khare, P., Karan, M., McQuistin, S., Perkins, C., Tyson, G., Purver, M., et al. (2022). The web we weave: Untangling the social graph of the IETF. Proc. Int. AAAI Conf. Web Soc. Media 16, 500–511. doi: 10.1609/icwsm.v16i1.19310

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Khare, P., Shekhar, R., Karan, M., McQuistin, S., Perkins, C., Castro, I., et al. (2023 July). Tracing linguistic markers of influence in a large online organisation. Proc. 61st Ann. Meeting Assoc. Comput. Ling. 2, 82–90. doi: 10.18653/v1/2023.acl-short.8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenig, A. M., Eagly, A. H., Mitchell, A. A., and Ristikari, T. (2011). Are leader stereotypes masculine? A meta-analysis of three research paradigms. Psychol. Bullet. 137, 616–642. doi: 10.1037/a0023557

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Levy, S., Kraut, R. E., Yu, J. A., Altenburger, K. M., and Wang, Y. C. (2022). “Understanding conflicts in online conversations,” in Proceedings of the ACM Web Conference 2022, 2592–2602.

Google Scholar

Lord, R. G., Day, D. V., Zaccaro, S. J., Avolio, B. J., and Eagly, A. H. (2017). Leadership in applied psychology: three waves of theory and research. J. Appl. Psychol. 102, 434.451. doi: 10.1037/apl0000089

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Madan, C. R., Shafer, A. T., Chan, M., and Singhal, A. (2017). Shock and awe: DISTINCT effects of taboo words on lexical decision and free recall. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 70, 793–810. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1167925

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McQuistin, S., Karan, M., Khare, P., Perkins, C., Tyson, G., Purver, M., et al. (2021). “Characterising the IETF through the lens of RFC deployment,” in Proceedings of the 21st ACM Internet Measurement Conference, 137–149.

Google Scholar

Obeng, S. G. (1997). Language and politics: indirectness in political discourse. Disc. Soc. 8, 49–83. doi: 10.1177/0957926597008001004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Neill, B. (2023). The Rewriting of Roald Dahl is an Act of Cultural Vandalism. The Spectator. Available online at: https://www.spectator.co.uk/article/the-rewriting-of-roald-dahl-is-an-act-of-cultural-vandalism/ (accessed July 13, 2023).

Google Scholar

Paik, J. E., and van Swol, L. M. (2022). The effects of expertise, status power, and language use on perception of task partner. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 41, 396–427. doi: 10.1177/0261927X211067249

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodd, J. (2018). “Lexical ambiguity,” in Oxford Handbook of Psycholinguistics, 2nd Edn, eds S. A. Rueschemeyer and M. G. Gaskell (Oxford: OUP), 120–144.

Google Scholar

Schnurr, S., Marra, M., and Holmes, J. (2008). “Impoliteness as a means of contesting power relations in the workplace,” in Impoliteness in Language: Studies on its Interplay With Power in Theory and Practice, Vol. 21, eds D. Bousfield, and M. A. Locher (Berlin: Walter de Gruyter), 243–267.

Google Scholar

Tan, C., Niculae, V., Danescu-Niculescu-Mizil, C., and Lee, L. (2016). “Winning arguments: Interaction dynamics and persuasion strategies in good-faith online discussions,” in Proceedings of the 25th International Conference on World Wide Web (Geneva: International World Wide Web Conferences Steering Committee), 613–624.

Google Scholar

Tepper, B. J. (2007). Abusive supervision in work organizations: review, synthesis, and research agenda. J. Manage. 33, 261–289. doi: 10.1177/0149206307300812

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Triandis, H. C. (2000). Culture and conflict. Int. J. Psychol. 35, 145–152. doi: 10.1080/002075900399448

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y. (2021). The price of being polite: politeness, social status, and their joint impacts on community Q&A efficiency. J. Comput. Soc. Sci. 4, 101–122. doi: 10.1007/s42001-020-00068-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: politeness, communication, dialogue, power, organization

Citation: Healey PGT, Khare P, Castro I, Tyson G, Karan M, Shekhar R, McQuistin S, Perkins C and Purver M (2024) Power and vulnerability: managing sensitive language in organizational communication. Front. Psychol. 14:1266425. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1266425

Received: 24 July 2023; Accepted: 10 November 2023;
Published: 23 February 2024.

Edited by:

Gary Libben, Brock University, Canada

Reviewed by:

Cynthia Whissell, Laurentian University, Canada
Bin Yin, Fujian Normal University, China

Copyright © 2024 Healey, Khare, Castro, Tyson, Karan, Shekhar, McQuistin, Perkins and Purver. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Patrick G. T. Healey, p.healey@qmul.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.