Nothing Special   »   [go: up one dir, main page]

skip to main content
10.1145/2696454.2696459acmconferencesArticle/Chapter ViewAbstractPublication PageshriConference Proceedingsconference-collections
research-article

Observer Perception of Dominance and Mirroring Behavior in Human-Robot Relationships

Published: 02 March 2015 Publication History

Abstract

How people view relationships between humans and robots is an important consideration for the design and acceptance of social robots. Two studies investigated the effect of relational behavior in a human-robot dyad. In Study 1, participants watched videos of a human confederate discussing the Desert Survival Task with either another human confederate or a humanoid robot. Participants were less trusting of both the robot and the person in a human-robot relationship where the robot was dominant toward the person than when the person was dominant toward the robot; these differences were not found for a human pair. In Study 2, participants watched videos of a human confederate having an everyday conversation with either another human confederate or a humanoid robot. Participants who saw a confederate mirror the gestures of a robot found the robot less attractive than when the robot mirrored the confederate; the opposite effect was found for a human pair. Exploratory findings suggest that human-robot relationships are viewed differently than human dyads.

References

[1]
Bailenson, J. & Yee, N. (2005). Digital chameleons: Automatic assimilation of nonverbal gestures in immersive virtual environments. Psychol Sci, 16(10), 814--819.
[2]
Bernieri, F. J., Gillis, J. S., Davis, J. M., & Grahe, J. E. (1996). Dyad rapport and the accuracy of its judgment across situations: A lens model analysis. J Pers Soc Psychol, 71(1), 110.
[3]
Boker, S. M., & Rotondo, J. L. (2002). Symmetry building and symmetry breaking in synchronized movement. Mirror neurons and the evolution of brain and language, 163--171.
[4]
Breazeal, C., Wang, A., & Picard, R. (2007). Experiments with a robotic computer: body, affect and cognition interactions. In Proc of the 2nd ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI '07), (pp. 153--160).
[5]
Burgoon, J. K., & Dunbar, N. E. (2000). An interactionist perspective on dominance-submission: Interpersonal dominance as a dynamic, situationally contingent social skill. Communications Monographs, 67(1), 96--121.
[6]
Burgoon, J. K., & Hale, J. L. (1984). The fundamental topoi of relational communication. Communication Monographs, 51(3), 193--214.
[7]
Burgoon, J. K., Johnson, M. L., & Koch, P. T. (1998). The nature and measurement of interpersonal dominance. Communications Monographs, 65(4), 308--335.
[8]
Burgoon, J. K., & Le Poire, B. A. (1999). Nonverbal cues and interpersonal judgments: Participant and observer perceptions of intimacy, dominance, composure, and formality. Communications Monographs, 66(2), 105--124.
[9]
Burgoon, J. K., & Newton, D. A. (1991). Applying a social meaning model to relational message interpretations of conversational involvement: Comparing observer and participant perspectives. Southern Journal of Communication, 56(2), 96--113.
[10]
Byrne, D., 1971. The Attraction Paradigm. Academic Press, New York.
[11]
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. J Pers Soc Psychol, 76(6), 893.
[12]
Condon, W. S., & Ogston, W. D. (1967). A segmentation of behavior. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 5, 221--236.
[13]
Cross, E. S., Liepelt, R., de C, H., Antonia, F., ... & Prinz, W. (2012). Robotic movement preferentially engages the action observation network. Human Brain Mapping, 33(9), 2238--2254.
[14]
Dillard, J. P., Solomon, D. H., & Palmer, M. T. (1999). Structuring the concept of relational communication. Communications Monographs, 66(1), 49--65.
[15]
Feltman, R., & Elliot, A. J. (2011). The influence of red on perceptions of relative dominance and threat in a competitive context. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 33(2), 308--314.
[16]
Floyd, K., & Erbert, L. A. (2003). Relational message interpretations of nonverbal matching behavior: An application of the social meaning model. Journal of Social Psychology, 143(5), 581--59
[17]
Friedman, B., Kahn Jr, P. H., & Hagman, J. (2003, April). Hardware companions': What online AIBO discussion forums reveal about the human-robotic relationship. In Proc of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI '03), (pp. 273--280).
[18]
Gazzola, V., Rizzolatti, G., Wicker, B., & Keysers, C. (2007). The anthropomorphic brain: the mirror neuron system responds to human and robotic actions. Neuroimage, 35(4), 1674--1684.
[19]
Goodrich, M. A., & Schultz, A. C. (2007). Human-robot interaction: a survey. Foundations and trends in human-computer interaction, 1(3), 203--275.
[20]
Groom, V., Chen, J., Johnson, T., Kara, F. A., & Nass, C. (2010, March). Critic, compatriot, or chump?: Responses to robot blame attribution. In Proc of the 5th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI '10), (pp. 211--217).
[21]
Hinds, P., Roberts, T., & Jones, H. (2004). Whose job is it anyway? A study of human-robot interaction on a collaborative task. Human Computer Interaction, 19, 151--181.
[22]
Hoffman, G., & Ju, W. (2014). Designing Robots with Movement in Mind. Journal of Human-Robot Interaction, 3(1), 89--122.
[23]
Kendon, A. (1970). Movement coordination in social interaction: Some examples described. Acta Psychologica, 32, 100--125.
[24]
Kim, N., Han, J., & Ju, W. (2014, March). Is a robot better than video for initiating remote social connections among children?. In Proc of the 2014 ACM/IEEE International Conference on HumanRobot Interaction (HRI '14), (pp. 208--209).
[25]
LaFrance, M. (1979). Nonverbal synchrony and rapport: Analysis by the cross-lag panel technique. Social Psychology Quarterly, 42(1): 66--70.
[26]
LaFrance, M. (1985). Postural mirroring and intergroup relations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11(2), 207--217.
[27]
LaFrance M. & Broadbent M. (1976). Group rapport: Posture sharing as a nonverbal indicator. Group Organ Stud, 1, 328--333
[28]
Lakin, J. L., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychol Sci, 14(4), 334--339.
[29]
Li, J., & Chignell, M. (2011). Communication of emotion in social robots through simple head and arm movements. International Journal of Social Robotics, 3(2), 125--142.
[30]
Little, A. C., & Hill, R. A. (2007). Attribution of red suggests special role in dominance signaling. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 5(1), 161--168.
[31]
Mutlu, B., Forlizzi, J., & Hodgins, J. (2006, December). A storytelling robot: Modeling and evaluation of human-like gaze behavior. In Proc of the 6th IEEE-RAS International Conference on Humanoid Robots, (pp. 518--523).
[32]
Nass, C., Moon, Y., Fogg, B. J., Reeves, B., & Dryer, D. C. (1995). Can computer personalities be human personalities?. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 43(2), 223--239.
[33]
Nass, C., Moon, Y., & Green, N. (1997). Are machines gender neutral? Gender-stereotypic responses to computers with voices. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 27(10), 864--876.
[34]
Parrill, F., & Kimbara, I. (2006). Seeing and hearing double: The influence of mimicry in speech and gesture on observers. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 30(4), 157--166.
[35]
Press, C., Bird, G., Flach, R., & Heyes, C. (2005). Robotic movement elicits automatic imitation. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(3), 632--640.
[36]
Reeves, B., & Nass, C. (1996). The Media Equation. Cambridge University Press.
[37]
Riek, L. D., Paul, P. C., & Robinson, P. (2010). When my robot smiles at me: Enabling human-robot rapport via real-time head gesture mimicry. Journal on Multimodal User Interfaces, 3(1--2), 99--108.
[38]
Rogers, Everett M. (1962). Diffusion of Innovations. Glencoe: Free Press
[39]
Roubroeks, M. A., Ham, J. R., & Midden, C. J. (2010). The dominant robot: Threatening robots cause psychological reactance, especially when they have incongruent goals. In Persuasive Technology (pp. 174--184). Springer Berlin Heidelberg.
[40]
Scheflen A.E. (1964). The significance of posture in communication systems. Psychiatry, 27, 316--331.
[41]
Short, E., Hart, J., Vu, M., & Scassellati, B. (2010). No fair!! An interaction with a cheating robot. In Proc. of 5th ACM/IEEE Int'l Conf on Human-Robot Interaction, (pp. 219--226).
[42]
Tiedens, L. Z., Unzueta, M. M., & Young, M. J. (2007). An unconscious desire for hierarchy? The motivated perception of dominance complementarity in task partners. J Pers Soc Psychol, 93(3), 402.
[43]
Trout, D., & Rosenfeld, H. (1980). The effect of postural lean and body congruence on the judgment of psychotherapeutic rapport. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 4, 176--190.
[44]
Walters, M. L., Syrdal, D. S., Dautenhahn, K., Te Boekhorst, R., & Koay, K. L. (2008). Avoiding the uncanny valley: Robot appearance, personality and consistency of behavior in an attention-seeking home scenario for a robot companion. Autonomous Robots, 24(2), 159--178.

Cited By

View all
  • (2025)Measuring and implementing lexical alignment: A systematic literature reviewComputer Speech & Language10.1016/j.csl.2024.10173190(101731)Online publication date: Mar-2025
  • (2024)Body Movement Mirroring and Synchrony in Human–Robot InteractionACM Transactions on Human-Robot Interaction10.1145/368207413:4(1-26)Online publication date: 23-Oct-2024
  • (2024)Dominant or Submissive? Exploring Social Perceptions Across the Human-Robot SpectrumProceedings of the 2024 4th International Conference on Human-Machine Interaction10.1145/3678429.3678431(8-14)Online publication date: 24-May-2024
  • Show More Cited By

Recommendations

Comments

Please enable JavaScript to view thecomments powered by Disqus.

Information & Contributors

Information

Published In

cover image ACM Conferences
HRI '15: Proceedings of the Tenth Annual ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction
March 2015
368 pages
ISBN:9781450328838
DOI:10.1145/2696454
Permission to make digital or hard copies of all or part of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others than the author(s) must be honored. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, or republish, to post on servers or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission and/or a fee. Request permissions from [email protected].

Sponsors

Publisher

Association for Computing Machinery

New York, NY, United States

Publication History

Published: 02 March 2015

Permissions

Request permissions for this article.

Check for updates

Author Tags

  1. dominance
  2. human-robot relationships
  3. mimicry
  4. mirroring
  5. observers
  6. social robots

Qualifiers

  • Research-article

Funding Sources

Conference

HRI '15
Sponsor:

Acceptance Rates

HRI '15 Paper Acceptance Rate 43 of 169 submissions, 25%;
Overall Acceptance Rate 268 of 1,124 submissions, 24%

Contributors

Other Metrics

Bibliometrics & Citations

Bibliometrics

Article Metrics

  • Downloads (Last 12 months)158
  • Downloads (Last 6 weeks)51
Reflects downloads up to 16 Nov 2024

Other Metrics

Citations

Cited By

View all
  • (2025)Measuring and implementing lexical alignment: A systematic literature reviewComputer Speech & Language10.1016/j.csl.2024.10173190(101731)Online publication date: Mar-2025
  • (2024)Body Movement Mirroring and Synchrony in Human–Robot InteractionACM Transactions on Human-Robot Interaction10.1145/368207413:4(1-26)Online publication date: 23-Oct-2024
  • (2024)Dominant or Submissive? Exploring Social Perceptions Across the Human-Robot SpectrumProceedings of the 2024 4th International Conference on Human-Machine Interaction10.1145/3678429.3678431(8-14)Online publication date: 24-May-2024
  • (2024)Human Impression of Humanoid Robots Mirroring Social CuesCompanion of the 2024 ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction10.1145/3610978.3640580(458-462)Online publication date: 11-Mar-2024
  • (2024)Power in Human-Robot InteractionProceedings of the 2024 ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction10.1145/3610977.3634949(269-282)Online publication date: 11-Mar-2024
  • (2024)Cobotic service teams and power dynamics: Understanding and mitigating unintended consequences of human-robot collaboration in healthcare servicesJournal of the Academy of Marketing Science10.1007/s11747-024-01004-1Online publication date: 2-Mar-2024
  • (2023)A Practical Taxonomy of TAS-related Usecase ScenariosProceedings of the First International Symposium on Trustworthy Autonomous Systems10.1145/3597512.3597523(1-6)Online publication date: 11-Jul-2023
  • (2023)“Should I Follow the Human, or Follow the Robot?” — Robots in Power Can Have More Influence Than Humans on Decision-MakingProceedings of the 2023 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems10.1145/3544548.3581066(1-13)Online publication date: 19-Apr-2023
  • (2023)Improving Efficiency of Human-Robot Coexistence While Guaranteeing Safety: Theory and User StudyIEEE Transactions on Automation Science and Engineering10.1109/TASE.2022.320197020:4(2706-2719)Online publication date: Oct-2023
  • (2023)Tactical Empathy for Long-Term HRI in Commercial In-Home Robots: An Academic Approach to Building a Bridge to the HRI Industry2023 32nd IEEE International Conference on Robot and Human Interactive Communication (RO-MAN)10.1109/RO-MAN57019.2023.10309651(9-15)Online publication date: 28-Aug-2023
  • Show More Cited By

View Options

Login options

View options

PDF

View or Download as a PDF file.

PDF

eReader

View online with eReader.

eReader

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share this Publication link

Share on social media