Nothing Special   »   [go: up one dir, main page]

Symbolic Interactionism

Download as docx, pdf, or txt
Download as docx, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 5

SYMBOLIC INTERACTIONISM

Symbolic interactionism is a sociological framework that focuses on the different

meanings individuals attach to objects, peoples, and interactions as well as the

corresponding behaviors that reflect those meanings and/ or interpretations. It is a

framework that actualizes the nature of humans to make sense of their actions and

interactions through external cues from their everyday life and environment (Vejar

2015).

George Herbert Mead was an influential figure in the field of symbolic interactionism.

Gestures, according to him, are important in communication. When we interact with

others, our posture, tone of voice, voice inflections, as well as hand and facial

movements convey significance. They can either accentuate or contradict that which we

are verbally stating (Vejar 2015).

Mead's central concept is the self, "the part of an individual's personality composed of

self-awareness and self-image" (Macionis 2007, 124). The process of self-discovery or

self-development is enacted by the usage of gestures threefold through the play stage,

the game stage, and through a stage called generalized other. The term "generalized

other” refers to "widespread cultural norms and values we use as a reference in

evaluating ourselves" (Macionis 2007, 126). Verja (2015, 3-4) describes each stage.

In the play stage, young children identify with key figures in their environments, such as

the mother or father, as well as occupational or gender-specific roles to which they have

been exposed (e.g., police officer, nurse) and replicate the behavioral norms that

correspond with such roles. A young boy might sit on the edge of the bathroom counter,
attentive to the way in which his father goes about shaving, and emulate this action by

scraping the edge of a blunt object across his own face.

During the game stage, children extrapolate from the vantage point of the roles they

have simulated by assuming the roles that their counterparts concurrently undertake.

While engaging in a team sport, for example, it behooves a child to conceptualize the

roles of his teammates and opponents in order to successfully maneuver throughout the

game within his own particular position.

As people developmentally evolve, their anticipation of the generalized other helps them

construct morally sound and appropriate behavior, such as the employee who arrives

promptly to work in order to avoid scrutiny from his colleagues. Moreover, self identity

continuously fluctuates between the I, which is the impulsive, automatic, "knee-jerk"

responses we have to stimuli (Lane, 1984), and the me, which is the socially refined

reactions that were instilled through the process of adopting social standards (Baldwin,

1988).

There are three overarching premises that constitute symbolic interactionism. These

three premises are outlined below with examples as concrete illustrations.

The first assumes that meaning is an important element of human existence, a concept

that is both subjective and individualistic, and that people consequently act in

accordance with the meanings they construe. Imagine the scholar who, upon drawing

on the concept of a book (i.e., object), generates stimulating and intellectual constructs

Meanwhile, someone who struggles academically may harbor feelings of fear and

resentment toward that object. A dyadic conversation (.e., interaction) may consist of
one person disclosing emotionally-laden personal accounts to a person who is furrowing

his brow. Interpretations derived from such a nonverbal gesture can be varied, and the

speaker might either conclude that he has an attentive audience, or that he is being

critiqued. Another example shows how the role of "parent" (i.e., people) might generate

the image of a warm, nurturing, and supportive role model to one person, while eliciting

visualizations of an autocratic and punitive figure to another.

A second premise asserts that people identify and mold their unique symbolic

references through the process of socialization. This postulation suggests that people

are not inherently equipped with interpretive devices that help navigate through the

complex realms of human behavior. Through the act of establishing an intricate series

of relationships they come to certain symbolic determinations, which create a sturdy

platform on which subsequent behavior is structured. When a young child engages in

pleasant behavior that causes his parent to smile, he equates the concept of "good

behavior," with that of a specific facial expression resulting in an upturned mouth." As

the child encounters pleasurable deeds throughout the course of his life, he will be

prompted to implement the symbolic demonstration (1.e., a smile) he initially

corresponded with such acts.

Behaviors are adopted through an obscurely subtle learning process, and the third tenet

of symbolic interactionism affirms that there is a cultural dimension that intertwines the

symbolic "educational" development. For example, in conversation, the amount of

physical space in which we distance our bodies has culturally symbolic significance

(Rothbaum, Morelli, Pott, & Liu-Constant, 2000). Likewise, greetings in the form of

demonstrative affection, such as hugs and kisses can be warmly regarded by one
culture, and deemed as the obstruction of personal space and the crossing of

inappropriate boundaries by another (Graham 2007, Vejar 2015, 1-2)

As a theoretical perspective, symbolic interactionism is not known for homogeneity,

parsimony or consensus among its practitioners. Four of the most prominent

contemporary varieties of symbolic interactionism include the Chicago School, the lowa

School, the dramaturgical approach, and ethnomethodology. All these four schools of

thought or orientations share the view that human beings construct their realities in a

process of social interaction, and agree on the methodological implication of such, that

is, the necessity of “getting inside" the reality of the actor in order to understand what is

going on (Gecas 1980, 1458).

Nonetheless, the four varieties of symbolic interactionisrn differ significantly in terms of

purpose and methodology. The most glaring difference exists between the Chicago

School with a positivist orientation and the lowa School with a humanistic orientation.

Such difference reflects a basic divergence in purpose and methodology in the

discipline of Sociology (Gecas 1980).

Following a positivist orientation and with Thomas Kuhn as its chief progenitor the

Chicago School aims at prediction and unity of method for all the sciences. Adopting a

humanistic orientation, the lowa school under the influence of Herbert Blumer strives for

understanding and a distinctive method for sociology, one that is based on "sympathetic

introspection." The two other contemporary varieties of symbolic interactionism, namely,

Goffman's dramaturgical approach and Garfinkel's ethnomethodology, are both more

aligned with the positivist Chicago School (Gecas 1980, 1458).


Symbolic interaction has both "insider" "and outsider" critics. Insiders' criticism focus on

the method and the central concepts of symbolic interactionism, particularly the

ambiguity of major concepts used particularly the concept of the "self." These criticisms

attack the utility of symbolic interactionism in the production of cumulative and

generalizable knowledge. Outsiders' critique, on the other hand, highlights the

astructural bias in symbolic interactionism. This bias refers to the claim that symbolic

interactionism's perspective is ahistorical, noneconomic, and a limited view of social

power and social organization (Gecas 1980, 1459).

The disagreement between the Chicago School and the lowa School reflects not only

the positivist-humanistic debate in symbolic interactionism, but by extension, the

objectivist-subjectivist dichotomy and debate not only in Sociology but also in the Social

Sciences.

The debate between subjectivist (humanist) and objectivist (positivist or neopositivist)

orientations toward human behavior and social processes is long-standing. The debate

has been between those who focus on the (humanistic) subject matter of the social

sciences and those who call for the same (scientific) method for all the sciences, both

natural and social. The debate has not only divided each of the social sciences, it has

also divided many of the subfields within these sciences, subfields such as social

psychology (Warshay & Warshay 1987).

You might also like