Young Language Learner (YLL) Research:: Ion Drew Angela Hasselgreen
Young Language Learner (YLL) Research:: Ion Drew Angela Hasselgreen
Young Language Learner (YLL) Research:: Ion Drew Angela Hasselgreen
Ion Drew
Associate professor at the Faculty of Arts and Education, University of Stavanger
Angela Hasselgreen
Professor at the Faculty of Education, Bergen University College
Abstract
Growing attention is being given to young language learners (YLL), following a global
trend to introduce foreign languages at earlier stages of the curriculum. This article
provides an overview of some recent international YLL research and follows this up by
comparing it to current YLL research in Norway. A YLL research network has recently
been established in Norway in order to share and promote research in the field. Eight
Norwegian YLL research projects, involving both English and other foreign
languages, are presented. The scope of this research in Norway is compared with
international research being conducted in the field. Although the Norwegian YLL
research is active in certain areas, such as starting age of second foreign languages,
teachers’ practices and attitudes, the use of extensive reading, vocabulary and the
analysis of written texts, there appear to be gaps in other areas. These include
research into the effects of story-telling and drama in the classroom, processes in
writing, pupils’ cognitive skills, the use of textbooks and the effect of the curriculum.
The establishment of a YLL network is considered as an important step in both
consolidating current YLL research in Norway and initiating new research projects.
Introduction
The phenomenon Young Language Learners (YLLs) has begun to make its mark
in several arenas. These include special interest groups in organisations such as
IATEFL and EALTA, special editions of periodicals, such as Language Testing
2000 (17/2), and, as demonstrated below, conferences and research networks.
Why then do YLLs need such attention? What makes this group so ‘special’
(and how indeed may they be defined?)? To take the last question first, for the
purposes of this article, YLLs are defined as learners from five years, up to
around 12/13 years, which seems to reflect the lower and upper limits of primary
school education, at least in Europe. Why YLLs are special is, basically, because
they are children. Hasselgreen (2000, pp. 262-3) lists some of the features which
put YLLs in a special situation. These include their enthusiasm for and openness
to the learning of new languages, but at the same time, their need for special
classroom methods. Not only do the learners themselves form a particular group,
but also their teachers tend, very often, not to be specialists in language or
language learning. Moreover, the L2 language itself produced by YLLs is
different from that of adults, just as their L1s are different.
In the traditional study of foreign language learning, teaching and their
‘output’, has had as its default group, adults. Methods based on early theories of
language) at the ages of 4, 8 and 11. The youngest learners scored the highest in
attitude and motivation. Another argument for an early start is that it will
ultimately lead to higher proficiency levels (Singleton, 2003). In contrast to an
early start, some studies have shown that older learners acquire a foreign
language at a faster rate than younger learners (e.g. Marinova-Todd et al. 2000)
and that older learners can reach levels of proficiency in morphology and syntax
that approximate native speakers (Juffs & Harrington, 1995).
Other factors that have been explored in addition to age are time and
intensity. Curtain (2000) used achievement tests and teacher interviews to
explore time, intensity and how time was used in early foreign language
programmes involving English first language (L1) children learning Spanish at
ages 5, 6 and 9 in the US. Each of the age groups was taught in programmes
with different time allocations. The results generally supported the hypothesis
that time and intensity would influence language achievement, but that the time
allocated to a language itself is less important than the way that time is used. As
Curtain (2000, p.108) points out:
Students given more time will do better than students with less time; students in
more intense programmes will do better than students in less intense
programmes. Students who have more time to use the target language will
perform better than students who have less time.
YLL teachers
Turning to language teachers of young learners, research into this group has
focussed on both pre-service and in-service teachers. For example, Matteoudakis
et al. (2007) investigated how pre-service teachers from the Czech Republic,
Hungary and Greece applied theoretical principles they had learned about story-
based language teaching in practical situations. The trainees were asked to adapt
and present a story of their own choice to their peers. Results showed that the
quality of work varied from country to country and that the trainees’ educational
context partly influenced how they performed the task. Czech and Hungarian
trainees were training to be specialists at the primary level, whereas the Greek
trainees were training to teach EFL at both the primary and secondary levels.
In a study focussing on in-service teachers, Lundberg (2007) conducted a
three-year action research project whose aim was to enhance the competence of
language teachers of young learners in Sweden who lacked formal qualifications
in teaching English. Before the training course started, Lundberg investigated
the content of typical language lessons, the use of the target language, and
pupils’ and teachers’ attitudes to the subject. She discovered that pupils were
generally demotivated to learn English and the teachers were generally insecure
and lacked confidence. Lessons were characterised by lack of variation, tempo,
boring materials, lack of planning, lack of continuity from previous lessons,
insufficient challenges, lack of physical activities, and lack of long-term
The results support the link between cognitive skills and young learners’
achievements in learning a foreign language. Certain cognitive skills, such as
memory faculties, analytic skills, sound discrimination capacities, visual
perception and inductive learning ability appear to contribute to the language
learning of young learners. Accordingly, the author argued that teaching
methods should be adjusted to match, promote or enhance the progress of young
language learners.
Finally, several studies have compared young learners of two foreign
languages. Firstly, Nikolov (2007) collected data in Hungary from two main
sources between 2000 and 2007: large scale surveys into Hungarian pupils’
language achievements in English and German and qualitative data collected in
classrooms and interviews with young teachers of young learners. The
quantitative data was based on 8000 pupils of English and German in each of
three cohorts aged 12, 14 and 16. Strong relationships were found between
achievements and intensity (number of weekly hours) and socioeconomic status
indicated by parents’ level of education. The qualitative data suggested that
there was a considerable gap between what teachers claim to do and what they
actually practise. It also indicated that practice and methodology appropriate to
the age group were rarely in tune with each other.
In another study, Csizér and Kormos (2007) investigated motivation and
inter-cultural contact in a large population of Hungarian children aged 13 and 14
who were studying English and German. The results indicated differences
between the two languages. Learners of English showed more positive attitudes
to target language speakers than learners of German. In addition learners of
English had a greater degree of linguistic self-confidence, put more effort into
language learning and received more support from their environment than was
the case among learners of German. The results also showed that learners of
English experienced wider contact with the target language through the media
than learners of German. For learners with low levels of motivation, contact
with the target language and its usefulness seem to be the factors most
influencing motivational intensity.
Finally, Huszti et al. (2007) reported on a longitudinal study comparing the
learning of English and Ukrainian in Hungarian schools in the Ukraine. The
subjects were 76 eight-year-old 3rd graders who had been learning English and
Ukrainian for at least two years. The learners’ proficiency level in the four
language skills was tested in the two languages through a number of tasks. In
addition, syllabi and textbooks were analysed and teachers of English and
Ukrainian were interviewed. The findings indicated that there were major
differences between the opportunities to learn English and Ukrainian and that
performances in English as a foreign language were superior to Ukrainian as a
second language.
• To establish what YLL research was being carried out, by whom and
where
• To establish where researchers may have common or overlapping
research fields, and how cooperation may be beneficial
• To see if distinct strands of research would emerge, giving rise to possible
sub-groupings
• To share resources, tools and channels for dissemination
• To identify areas which may be neglected or in need of more focus
• To plan future events which would maintain contact within the group, and
which would directly serve our needs as YLL researchers.
As the first aim was paramount at this stage, all participants who felt in a
position to present actual research projects were invited to do so; eight such
presentations were made. The remaining time was devoted to discussion around
the other aims.
Of the projects presented, five involve English, one involves ‘other foreign
languages’, and two are not restricted to any specific language(s). Five of the
projects concern methods/procedures, and three the analysis of pupil language,
with one project involving both. One project studies the beliefs of language
teachers. The projects vary in maturation from those at their very outsets, to one
where a finished ‘product’ is available, and another where a process has been
trialled and evaluated. An overview of the projects follows, broadly using the
same groupings as in the previous section. However, the first project presented
lies outside any of these particular categories, as it concerns virtually all YLL
issues.
YLL teachers
‘Language teachers’ beliefs after KL06’ is another study which puts the spot-
light on teachers, rather than on actual YLLs, being embarked on at the Uni-
versity of Stavanger (Alexandre Dessingue). This project proposes to conduct a
comparative study of language teachers’ beliefs involving the major languages
taught in Norwegian schools (including Norwegian as a second language). In
this study, “beliefs” are defined as “a form of personal knowledge consisting of
implicit assumptions about students, learning, classrooms, and the subject
matter to be taught.” (Kagan, 1992). The first part of the study will focus on a
questionnaire survey in the county of Rogaland, aiming to explore in-service
language teachers’ beliefs concerning the teaching of foreign languages
following the implementation of the Knowledge Promotion, 2006. This part of
the project will also look into language teachers’ beliefs about CLL (Curriculum
reading are based on the ‘Running Records’ work in New Zealand and Australia
(Clay, 2000) and, as such, share common ground with the Early Years Literacy
Programme described above. Features of the texts pupils appear able to read at
different levels will be identified. The assessment of writing involves close
analysis of pupils’ language, and is carried out in cooperation with the Writing
of Young Learners and the CEFR project, cited below. The project is at an early
stage, but from autumn 2008 will be extended to include classes in Spain,
Slovenia and Lithuania, as part of the ECML (European Centre for Modern
Languages) third medium term programme, 2008-2011. It will also include
pupils in the Bergen area who have other L1 backgrounds; in these cases the
pupils’ development in Norwegian as a second language will be studied.
might pupils’ written English be characterised at this stage?’ This project aims
to find out more about pupils’ competence upon leaving primary school, posing
the questions: ‘Which language aspects do pupils master and which aspects are
still difficult?’ Pupils’ texts have been collected from eight different schools in a
school district in Trøndelag. The texts – 172 altogether – were written in the
form of a test, similar to the national tests in English that teachers were familiar
with at that time (which included a test in writing).
The first stage of the project focussed on selected formal aspects which are
central in the teaching of English in primary school, such as concord, the
spelling of a few central function words and the genitive form. Sentence
complexity has also been investigated. Sentence patterns have been found that
roughly compare with patterns identified in other comparable materials.
Moreover, a fairly limited range of dependent clauses has been observed, but at
the present stage and with only parts of the material analysed, only limited
conclusions can be drawn.
The project is still in an early phase, where the texts have only partially been
examined. In the next stage, aspects such as code switching and vocabulary will
be focussed on. It is also planned to collect texts from the same pupils when they
reach 10th grade in order to add a longitudinal perspective to the project.
‘The Writing of Young Learners and the CEFR’, undertaken at AKSIS,
University of Bergen (Eli Moe), is the final study to be presented. In common
with the two previous studies, this project examines a range of aspects of pupils’
writing, including clause and phrase complexity, vocabulary and selected error
types. In common with the study of the assessment of the literacy of young
learners, this study is concerned with identifying features that characterise
pupils’ writing as they pass through stages on the CEFR. A principal feature of
this study is that it has access to a great many samples of writing from both 7th
and 10th grade pupils, both rated by multiple raters on the same (CEFR) scale,
through the national testing of English in 2005. This means that writing assessed
to be at the same ‘level’, but from pupils of different ages, can be compared.
Additionally, data similar to that of the 7th grade material was collected from a
control group of native speaker pupils around 11 years old. The results showed
distinct differences in performance between groups who differed principally in
age, suggesting that certain language features of writing are related more to
maturity than ‘level’ rated on a scale of proficiency such as CEFR. These
include the use of cohesive connectors (such as because, when, etc), where the
younger pupils (both Norwegian and native speaker) shared a more or less
common range, which was significantly narrower than that of the older pupils.
This finding has important consequences for the criteria on which the writing
of YLLs, however proficient, are judged. It is also an important step towards the
necessary work needed to scrutinise the limits of applicability of the CEFR to
young learners. In the presentation, a call was made for cooperation on the
not represented in the Norwegian research documented here. These include the
question of who should be teaching English at this early stage: specialists or
class teachers (begging the question, of course, of whether the basic training of
teachers should include an FL as a compulsory element, which it does not at
present). Other issues not addressed here include parents’ practices and attitudes
towards reading, processes in writing, pupils’ cognitive skills, the use of
textbooks and the effect of the curriculum. Furthermore, other areas not reported
as being studied in this article include the place of literature (stories, rhymes,
drama etc) in the language classroom (although this is an area which has
recently been taken up within the Norwegian network), as well as a focus on the
spoken language of YLLs.
YLL research, relative to ‘general’ research into language learning, is at an
early stage worldwide. In some respects, such as the close analysis of written
language, the compilation of learner corpora, the issue of the CEFR in YLL
assessment, and studies of YLL literacy, it seems that the Norwegian research
within the network is relatively strong. In other respects, serious gaps are
evident. It would therefore appear that Norway could both benefit from and
contribute to the international field of YLL research. This was exemplified at the
EALTA conference, Athens 2008, where a paper presented by Hasselgreen
(2008) and one presented by Papp and Jones (2008) demonstrated how each one
could draw on the findings of the other. If we are to make up for lost time within
this important field, cooperation both within and across national boundaries is
essential.
References
Alexiou, T. 2007. Cognitive skills promoting young learners’ second language learning and
their relation to learning styles. Paper presented at TeMoLaYoLe conference, University
of Pécs, Hungary.
Blondin, C., Chandelier, M., Edelenbos, P., Johnstone, R., Kubanek-German, A., &
Taeschner, T. 1998. Foreign languages in primary and pre-school education: a review of
recent research within the European Union. London: Centre for Information on Language
Teaching.
Cenoz, J. 2003. The influence of age on the acquisition of English: general proficiency,
attitudes and code mixing. In M. del Pilar Garcia Mayo & M. L.G. Lecumberri (Eds.),
Age and the acquisition of English as a foreign language (pp. 77-93). Clevedon:
Multilingual Matters.
Chiang, I-C. N. 2007. Can parents help their children read extensively? Paper presented at
TeMoLaYoLe conference, University of Pécs, Hungary.
Clay, M. M. 2000. Running records for classroom teachers. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Cobb, T. 2008. Ten 250-word Stemach and Williams kid spoken vocab lists. Accessed
12.03.08 from http://www.lextutor.ca/vp/kids/kid
Council of Europe. 2001. Common European framework of reference for languages:
learning, teaching, assessment. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Csizer, K., & Kormos, J. 2007. An investigation into the relationship of L2 motivation and
cross-cultural contact among elementary-school students. Paper presented at
TeMoLaYoLe conference, University of Pécs, Hungary.
Curtain, H. 2000. Time as a factor in early start programmes. In J. Moon & M. Nikolov
(Eds.), Research into teaching English to young learners (pp.87-120). Pécs: University
Press Pécs.
Donato, R., Tucker G.R., Wudthayagorn, J., & Igarashi, K. 2000. Converging evidence:
attitudes, achievements, and instruction in the later years of FLES. Foreign Language
Annals, 33, 377-93.
Drew, I. 2007. Using the Early Years Literacy Programme in a primary EFL Norwegian
classroom. Paper presented at the TeMoLaYoLe Conference, University of Pécs,
Hungary.
Drew, I. 2008. Lesing og skriving i engelsk på barnetrinnet: den australske måten. In T.M
Guldal, O.F.Lillemyr, G. Løkken, N. Naastad & F. Rønning (Eds.) FoU i Praksis 2007.
Rapport fra konferanse om praksisrettet FoU i lærerutdanning, (pp.93-102). Trondheim:
Tapir Adademisk Forlag.
Griva, E., Tsakiridou, H., & Nihoritou, I. 2007. A study of the composing process and writing
strategies employed by young learners. Paper presented at TeMoLaYoLe conference,
University of Pécs, Hungary.
Harris, J., & O’Leary, D. 2007. A third language at primary level in Ireland: an independent
evaluation of The Modern Languages in Primary Schools Initiative. Paper presented at
TeMoLaYoLe conference, University of Pécs, Hungary.
Hasselgreen, A. 2000. The assessment of the English ability of young learners in Norwegian
schools: an innovative approach. Language testing, 17/2, (pp. 261-277)
Hasselgreen, A. 2008 Assessing the literacy of children: A CEFR-linked longitudinal study.
Paper presented at 5th EALTA Conference, Athens.
http://ealta2008.hau.gr/conf_program.htm.
Huszti, I., Fabian, M., & Komari, E.B. 2007. Differences between the processes and outcomes
in 3rd graders’ learning English and Ukrainian in Hungarian schools in Betregszasz.
Paper presented at TeMoLaYoLe conference, University of Pécs, Hungary.
Johnson, J., & Newport, E. 1989. Critical period effects in second language learning: the
influence of the maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language.
Cognitive Psychology, 21, 60-99.
Johnstone, R. 2007. Early languages learning: where are we now and where might we go?
Keynote lecture presented at TeMoLaYoLe conference, Pécs, Hungary.
Juffs, A., & Harrington, M. 1995. Parsing effects in second language sentence processing:
subject and object asymmetries in wh-extraction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition,
17, 483-516.
Kagan, D.M. 1992. Implications of research on teacher belief. Educational Psychologist, 27
(1), 65-90.
Krashen, S.D. 1982. Principles and practice in second language acquisition. Exeter:
Pergamon.
Krashen, S. D. 1984. Writing. Research, theory and applications. New York: Oxford.
Lightbown, P. 2000. Classroom second language acquisition research and second language
teaching. Applied Linguistics, 21(4), 431-82.
Lugossy, R. 2007. Real picture books in the lives of young EFL learners and their teachers.
In M. Nikolov et al. (Eds.), The TeMoLaYoLe book: teaching modern languages to young
learners: teachers, curricula and materials (pp. 77-90). Strasbourg/Graz: Council of
Europe/European Centre for Modern Languages.
Lundberg, G. 2007. Teachers in action: Action research in in-service programmes for teachers
of languages to young learners. In M. Nikolov et al. (Eds.), The TeMoLaYoLe book:
Teaching modern languages to young learners: Teachers, curricula and materials (pp.
21-34). Strasbourg/Graz: Council of Europe/European Centre for Modern Languages.
Marinova-Todd, S.H., Marshall, D.B., & Snow C.E. 2000. Three misconceptions about age
and L2 learning. TESOL Quarterly, 34, 9-34.
Matteoudakis, M., Dvorakova, K., & Lang, K. 2007. Story-based language teaching: An
experimental study on the implementation of a module in three European countries. In M.
Nikolov et al. (Eds.), The TeMoLaYoLe book: teaching modern languages to young
learners: teachers, curricula and materials (pp. 59-76). Strasbourg/Graz: Council of
Europe/European Centre for Modern Languages.
Mitchell, R., & Myles, F. 1998. Second language learning theories. London: Arnold.
Moon, J., & Nikolov, M. (Eds.). 2000. Research into teaching English to young learners.
Pécs: University Press Pécs.
Nation, P. 2001. Learning vocabulary in another language. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Nikolov, M. 2000. Issues in research into early foreign language programmes. In J. Moon &
M. Nikolov (Eds.), Research into teaching English to young learners (pp.21 – 48). Pécs:
University Press Pécs.
Nikolov, M., Mihalević, D.J., Matteoudakis, M., Lundberg, G., & Flanagan, T. (Eds.). 2007.
The TeMoLaYoLe book: teaching modern languages to young learners: teachers,
curricula and materials. Strasbourg/Graz: Council of Europe/European Centre for
Modern Languages.
Nikolov, M. 2007. Early modern foreign language programmes and outcomes: factors
contributing to achievements of Hungarian learners. Keynote lecture presented at
TeMoLaYoLe conference, University of Pécs, Hungary.
Orosz, A. 2007. The growth of young learners’ English vocabulary size. Paper presented at
TeMoLaYoLe conference, University of Pécs, Hungary.
Papp, S., & Jones, N. 2008. Use of young learner can-dos as part of formative assessment.
Paper presented at 5th EALTA Conference, Athens.
http://ealta2008.hau.gr/conf_program.htm.
Rixon, S. 2000a. Young learners of English: background and issues. Modern English
Teacher, 9(4), 5-10.
Rixon, S. 2000b. Collecting eagle’s eye data on the teaching of English to young learners:
The British Council overview. In J. Moon & M. Nikolov (Eds.), Research into teaching
English to young learners (pp. 153-170). Pécs: University Press Pécs.
Shohamy, E., & Inbar, O. 2007. Effective teaching of EFL to young learners: Homeroom or
FL teachers? Paper presented at TeMoLaYoLe conference, University of Pécs, Hungary.
Singleton, D. 2003. Critical period or general age factor(s)? In M. del Pilar Garcia Mayo &
M. L.G. Lecumberri (Eds.), Age and the acquisition of English as a foreign language (pp.
3-22). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Speitz, H., & Simonsen, T. 2008. Tidligere start med flere fremmedspråk. Språk og
språkundervisning, 2008(1), 15-18.
Stemach, G., & Williams, W.B. 1988. Word express: the 1st hundred words of spoken
English. Novato, CA: Academic therapy publications.
Szpotowicz, M. 2007. Factors influencing vocabulary acquisition of lower primary learners of
English as a foreign language. Paper presented at TeMoLaYoLe conference, University
of Pécs, Hungary.
Vickov, G. 2007. Learners’ own cultural identity in early EFL. In M. Nikolov et al. (Eds.),
The TeMoLaYoLe book: teaching modern languages to young learners: teachers,
curricula and materials (pp. 105-120). Strasbourg/Graz: Council of Europe/European
Centre for Modern Languages.
Wawrzyniak-Sliwska, M. 2007. Learner autonomy in young learner class – the teacher’s
perspective. Paper presented at TeMoLaYoLe conference, University of Pécs, Hungary.