US20090298025A1 - Object recognition testing tools and techniques for measuring cognitive ability and cognitive impairment - Google Patents
Object recognition testing tools and techniques for measuring cognitive ability and cognitive impairment Download PDFInfo
- Publication number
- US20090298025A1 US20090298025A1 US11/888,091 US88809107A US2009298025A1 US 20090298025 A1 US20090298025 A1 US 20090298025A1 US 88809107 A US88809107 A US 88809107A US 2009298025 A1 US2009298025 A1 US 2009298025A1
- Authority
- US
- United States
- Prior art keywords
- user
- performance
- test
- images
- cognitive
- Prior art date
- Legal status (The legal status is an assumption and is not a legal conclusion. Google has not performed a legal analysis and makes no representation as to the accuracy of the status listed.)
- Abandoned
Links
Images
Classifications
-
- G—PHYSICS
- G16—INFORMATION AND COMMUNICATION TECHNOLOGY [ICT] SPECIALLY ADAPTED FOR SPECIFIC APPLICATION FIELDS
- G16H—HEALTHCARE INFORMATICS, i.e. INFORMATION AND COMMUNICATION TECHNOLOGY [ICT] SPECIALLY ADAPTED FOR THE HANDLING OR PROCESSING OF MEDICAL OR HEALTHCARE DATA
- G16H20/00—ICT specially adapted for therapies or health-improving plans, e.g. for handling prescriptions, for steering therapy or for monitoring patient compliance
- G16H20/70—ICT specially adapted for therapies or health-improving plans, e.g. for handling prescriptions, for steering therapy or for monitoring patient compliance relating to mental therapies, e.g. psychological therapy or autogenous training
-
- A—HUMAN NECESSITIES
- A61—MEDICAL OR VETERINARY SCIENCE; HYGIENE
- A61B—DIAGNOSIS; SURGERY; IDENTIFICATION
- A61B3/00—Apparatus for testing the eyes; Instruments for examining the eyes
- A61B3/02—Subjective types, i.e. testing apparatus requiring the active assistance of the patient
- A61B3/028—Subjective types, i.e. testing apparatus requiring the active assistance of the patient for testing visual acuity; for determination of refraction, e.g. phoropters
- A61B3/032—Devices for presenting test symbols or characters, e.g. test chart projectors
-
- A—HUMAN NECESSITIES
- A61—MEDICAL OR VETERINARY SCIENCE; HYGIENE
- A61B—DIAGNOSIS; SURGERY; IDENTIFICATION
- A61B5/00—Measuring for diagnostic purposes; Identification of persons
- A61B5/16—Devices for psychotechnics; Testing reaction times ; Devices for evaluating the psychological state
-
- A—HUMAN NECESSITIES
- A61—MEDICAL OR VETERINARY SCIENCE; HYGIENE
- A61B—DIAGNOSIS; SURGERY; IDENTIFICATION
- A61B5/00—Measuring for diagnostic purposes; Identification of persons
- A61B5/40—Detecting, measuring or recording for evaluating the nervous system
- A61B5/4076—Diagnosing or monitoring particular conditions of the nervous system
- A61B5/4088—Diagnosing of monitoring cognitive diseases, e.g. Alzheimer, prion diseases or dementia
-
- A—HUMAN NECESSITIES
- A61—MEDICAL OR VETERINARY SCIENCE; HYGIENE
- A61B—DIAGNOSIS; SURGERY; IDENTIFICATION
- A61B5/00—Measuring for diagnostic purposes; Identification of persons
- A61B5/72—Signal processing specially adapted for physiological signals or for diagnostic purposes
- A61B5/7235—Details of waveform analysis
- A61B5/7264—Classification of physiological signals or data, e.g. using neural networks, statistical classifiers, expert systems or fuzzy systems
Definitions
- the present disclosure relates to tools and techniques for measuring cognitive ability and/or impairment.
- Intrinsic factors that can influence cognitive function include sex, age and genetic makeup.
- Testing visuospatial memory in rodents typically involves a viewer perspective of a world-centered (allocentric) frame of reference with information found throughout a complex environment in which the participant has to navigate. Making direct inferences about performance on navigation tests from performance on table-top tests can be problematic.
- VR Virtual reality
- phobias in phobias
- a virtual environment consisting of a series of interconnected hallways, some leading to dead ends and others leading to a designated goal location, was used to study age and sex differences in spatial navigation.
- Apolipoprotein E Genotype and Age Differences in Cognitive Function
- APOE apolipoprotein E
- ⁇ 2, ⁇ 3, and ⁇ 4 The three major human isoforms of apolipoprotein E (APOE), which are encoded by distinct APOE alleles ( ⁇ 2, ⁇ 3, and ⁇ 4), are involved in the metabolism and redistribution of lipoproteins and cholesterol.
- AD Alzheimer's disease
- Women are at higher risk to develop AD than men, particularly women carrying ⁇ 4.
- the effects of ⁇ 4 on cognitive function in the non-demented elderly old-old are less clear. While some studies have shown poor cognitive performance in non-demented elderly ⁇ 4 carriers compared with non-demented elderly non- ⁇ 4 carriers and a small effect was observed in a meta-analysis, other studies did not.
- testosterone, but not estrogen, levels in serum have correlated positively with cognitive performance in older women, and androgen therapy has been shown to improve cognition in surgically menopausal women.
- the relationship between testosterone levels and cognitive function might be ⁇ 4-dependent.
- low testosterone levels and ⁇ 4 have been shown to interact in increasing the risk of developing AD.
- an interaction between ⁇ 4 and cognitive performance in healthy older men has been reported; while in non- ⁇ 4 carriers higher testosterone levels were associated with better general cognition, in ⁇ 4 carriers higher testosterone levels were associated with lower cognitive performance.
- the Detailed Description is directed to various techniques and tools for measuring cognitive ability and/or detecting cognitive impairment or decline.
- techniques and tools are described that can be used to diagnose or test susceptibility to cognitive impairments in children or in elderly people (such as cognitive impairments associated with Alzheimer's Disease).
- Techniques and tools are described that can be used to evaluate treatment effects and/or measure cognitive decline over time.
- FIG. 1 is a block diagram of a suitable computing environment in conjunction with which several described embodiments may be implemented.
- FIG. 2 is a flowchart of a generalized technique for analysis of cognitive status using VR testing.
- FIG. 3 is a flowchart of a generalized technique for analysis of cognitive status using Novel Image Novel Location (“NINL”) testing.
- NINL Novel Image Novel Location
- FIG. 4 is a diagram showing example panels of a NINL software tool according to one or more described embodiments.
- FIG. 5 is a diagram showing screen shots of a virtual reality, spatial navigation software tool according to one or more described embodiments.
- FIGS. 6A and 6B are charts showing comparable facial recognition scores in male and female participants, and correlation of the Faces I and Faces II scores, respectively.
- FIGS. 7A-7F are charts showing NINL total scores of male and female participants, correlation of NINL I and NINL II, NINL scores for ability to detect a change, NINL scores for ability to detect a novel image, NINL scores for ability to detect a novel location of a familiar image, and correlation of combined NINL total scores and combined facial recognition total scores, respectively, according to one or more described embodiments.
- FIGS. 8A-8E are charts showing, for males and females tested with a virtual reality, spatial navigation software tool in hidden target trials and visible target trials, results for latency to reach the target with (+) or without ( ⁇ ) wearing a head-mounted display (“HMD”), velocity, latency to reach the target, percentage time in the target quadrant, percentage of successful trials, respectively, according to one or more described embodiments.
- FIG. 8F is a chart showing, for males and females in a probe trial, percentage time in four quadrants, according to one or more described embodiments.
- FIGS. 9A-9C are charts showing correlation between NINL total scores and latency to reach the target during a visible target session with a virtual reality, spatial navigation software tool, correlation between NINL total scores and latency to reach the target during a hidden target session with a virtual reality, spatial navigation software tool, and correlation between NINL total scores and percentage of time spent in the target quadrant during a probe trial, respectively, according to one or more described embodiments.
- FIGS. 10A-10D are charts showing, for elderly women and men, an effect of sex on “Family Pictures” test scores, effect of APOE ⁇ 4 on NINL total scores (combined immediate and delayed scores), effect of APOE ⁇ 4 on Novel Location sub-scores (combined immediate and delayed scores), and effect of sex on Novel Location sub-scores, respectively, according to one or more described embodiments.
- FIGS. 11A and 11B are charts showing, for elderly women and men tested with a virtual reality, spatial navigation software tool, effect of sex on velocity during visible target trials, and effect of sex on velocity during hidden target trials, respectively, according to one or more described embodiments.
- FIGS. 12A-12F are charts showing, for elderly women and men tested with a virtual reality, spatial navigation software tool, effect of APOE ⁇ 4 on velocity during a visible target session, effect of ⁇ 4 on velocity during a hidden target session, effect of APOE ⁇ 4 on cumulative distance during a visible target session, effect of APOE ⁇ 4 on cumulative distance during a hidden target session, effect of APOE ⁇ 4 on latency to reach target during a visible target session, and effect of APOE ⁇ 4 on latency to reach target during a hidden target session, respectively, according to one or more described embodiments.
- FIGS. 13A and 13B are charts showing, for elderly women and men tested with a virtual reality, spatial navigation software tool in a probe trial, effect of sex on percentage of time spent in four quadrants, and effect of APOE ⁇ 4 on percentage of time spent in four quadrants, respectively, according to one or more described embodiments.
- FIG. 14 is a chart showing, for elderly women and men, effect of ⁇ 4 on salivary testosterone levels.
- FIGS. 15A and 15B are charts showing, for elderly men, correlation of salivary cortisol levels with NINL I novel image recognition, and correlation of salivary cortisol levels with NINL II novel image recognition, respectively, according to one or more described embodiments.
- FIGS. 16A , 16 B and 16 C are charts showing effects of APOE ⁇ 4 on performance in 7-10 year-old boys and girls tested with a virtual reality, spatial navigation software tool, according to one or more described embodiments.
- Described embodiments are directed to techniques and tools for measuring cognitive ability and/or detecting cognitive impairment or decline. For example, techniques and tools are described that can be used to diagnose or test susceptibility to cognitive impairments in children or in elderly people (such as cognitive impairments associated with Alzheimer's Disease). Techniques and tools are described that can be used to evaluate treatment effects and/or measure cognitive decline over time. The various techniques and tools described herein may be used independently. Some of the described techniques and tools can be used in combination.
- Adjusting for effects of X refers to adjusting an interpreted result such that the condition X does not skew the interpreted result.
- Age-related cognitive decline refers to a reduction in cognition associated with advancing age, e.g., an age-related dementia.
- “Artificial intelligence” refers to information processing performed by one or more computers that mimics human reasoning.
- “Cognitive status” refers to status of cognition—mental processes related to knowing, thinking, learning and/or judging.
- First-person refers to a simulation of a perspective a user would have if the user were physically present in a virtual environment.
- Training of navigation skills refers to gradual improvement of navigation skills through repetition, such as navigation skills used in a virtual reality environment.
- Training of landscape refers to gradual improvement of knowledge of a landscape, such as a landscape in a virtual reality environment.
- “Measuring neural activity” refers to detecting activated brain regions.
- neural activity can be measured using functional magnetic resonance imaging (“fMRI”) techniques that measure changes in neuroanatomical activity, such as increased blood flow to areas of the brain having corresponding neurological functions.
- fMRI functional magnetic resonance imaging
- No change score refers to a performance signifier that measures performance of a user in identifying situations with no change in image content or image location for a second set of one or more images relative to a first set of one or more images.
- Novel image refers to a new image in a second set of one or more images relative to a first set of one or more images.
- Novel location refers to a new location of an image in a second set of one or more images relative to a first set of one or more images.
- Pattern recognition refers to identification of a pattern in data and association of the identified pattern with a condition or other data.
- “Pediatric cognitive disability” refers to diminished cognition (as compared to unaffected normal peers) in a human child under the age of 18.
- Pediatric cognitive disability can be associated with, for example, a genetic abnormality or a neuropsychological disturbance.
- Pre-clinical Alzheimer's disease refers to Alzheimer's disease in its early stages before memory disturbance significantly interferes with psychosocial function to an extent that a clinical diagnosis can be made based on the memory disturbance.
- Providing a treatment regimen refers to setting or adjusting a treatment regimen, such as a dose of an anti-Alzheimer's disease medication or the degree to which an environment is structured to address the effects of dementia.
- Score refers to a performance signifier, such as a number or percentage of successful trials.
- “User” refers to a human being that uses or interacts with computer software and/or a computerized system.
- Virtual reality environment refers to an environment that simulates a physical environment.
- a computer can display a virtual reality environment to a user via a graphical display, and the user can interact with the virtual reality environment by transmitting input to the computer.
- FIG. 1 illustrates a generalized example of a suitable computing environment ( 100 ) in which several of the described embodiments may be implemented.
- the computing environment ( 100 ) is not intended to suggest any limitation as to scope of use or functionality, as the techniques and tools may be implemented in diverse general-purpose or special-purpose computing environments.
- the computing environment ( 100 ) includes at least one processing unit ( 110 ) and memory ( 120 ).
- the processing unit ( 110 ) executes computer-executable instructions and may be a real or a virtual processor. In a multi-processing system, multiple processing units execute computer-executable instructions to increase processing power.
- the memory ( 120 ) may be volatile memory (e.g., registers, cache, RAM), non-volatile memory (e.g., ROM, EEPROM, flash memory, etc.), or some combination of the two.
- the memory ( 120 ) stores software ( 180 ) implementing one or more of the described techniques and tools for testing cognitive ability and/or cognitive impairment.
- a computing environment may have additional features.
- the computing environment ( 100 ) includes storage ( 140 ), one or more input devices ( 150 ), one or more output devices ( 160 ), and one or more communication connections ( 170 ).
- An interconnection mechanism such as a bus, controller, or network interconnects the components of the computing environment ( 100 ).
- operating system software provides an operating environment for other software executing in the computing environment ( 100 ), and coordinates activities of the components of the computing environment ( 100 ).
- the storage ( 140 ) may be removable or non-removable, and includes magnetic disks, magnetic tapes or cassettes, CD-ROMs, DVDs, flash memory, or any other medium which can be used to store information and which can be accessed within the computing environment ( 100 ).
- the storage ( 140 ) stores instructions for the software ( 180 ).
- the input device(s) ( 150 ) may be a touch input device such as a keyboard, mouse, pen, touch screen, or trackball, a voice input device, a scanning device, or another device that provides input to the computing environment ( 100 ).
- the input device(s) ( 150 ) may be a sound card, video card, TV tuner card, or similar device that accepts audio or video input in analog or digital form, or a CD-ROM, CD-RW or DVD that reads audio or video samples into the computing environment ( 100 ).
- the output device(s) ( 160 ) may be a display, printer, speaker, CD- or DVD-writer, or another device that provides output from the computing environment ( 100 ).
- the communication connection(s) ( 170 ) enable communication over a communication medium to another computing entity.
- the communication medium conveys information such as computer-executable instructions, audio or video input or output, or other data in a modulated data signal.
- a modulated data signal is a signal that has one or more of its characteristics set or changed in such a manner as to encode information in the signal.
- communication media include wired or wireless techniques implemented with an electrical, optical, RF, infrared, acoustic, or other carrier.
- Computer-readable media are any available media that can be accessed within a computing environment.
- Computer-readable media include memory ( 120 ), storage ( 140 ), communication media, and combinations of any of the above.
- program modules include routines, programs, libraries, objects, classes, components, data structures, etc. that perform particular tasks or implement particular abstract data types.
- the functionality of the program modules may be combined or split between program modules as desired in various embodiments.
- Computer-executable instructions for program modules may be executed within a local or distributed computing environment.
- FIG. 2 shows a generalized technique ( 200 ) for analysis of cognitive status of a user using testing with a virtual reality (“VR”) environment.
- a software tool such as one operating in the computer system environment shown in FIG. 1 or other tool performs the technique.
- Example use scenarios and example clinical applications for the generalized technique ( 200 ) are described below.
- the tool receives ( 210 ) input from the user as the user interacts with software presenting a VR environment.
- the VR environment includes a first-person, three-dimensional graphical rendering of the environment as well as sound cues for the environment.
- the environment is graphically rendered on a computer monitor or, for a more immersive experience, presented to the user using virtual reality goggles or another head mounted display.
- Inputs (such as direction of movement, speed of movement) are received from the user using a force-feedback joystick, other joystick, mouse, keyboard or other input device.
- the organization of the VR environment depends on implementation but typically includes areas such as quadrants which may be organized in terms of a coordinate space.
- the tool tracks position of the user over time in the coordinate space.
- the results of the tracking are stored in memory or a file (e.g., as timestamped coordinate locations) for later analysis of patterns of user behavior.
- the tool measures one or more of the following: (1) distance (e.g., cumulative or start to target) traversed in the VR environment, (2) time elapsed before reaching a target (or targets) in the VR environment, (3) percentage of successful trials (where success is, e.g., finding a target), (4) time spent in a target area of the VR environment, (5) velocity of movement in the VR environment, (6) pattern of movement (e.g., between multiple areas or in terms of coordinates) in the VR environment, and/or (7) pattern of time spent in respective areas of the VR environment.
- the tool measures performance using other and/or additional metrics. Some metrics (such as velocity of movement and time elapsed before reaching a target) may depend on each other to some extent, while other metrics do not.
- the tool measures performance in a series of VR tests, with some tests having one or more “visible” targets and other tests having one or more “hidden” targets.
- one or more visual or audible cues assist the user in finding a target. For example, a prominent flag or other graphical cue is placed next to the target to help the user find the target, or directional arrows guide the user to the target.
- the hidden target trials the performance of the user in finding the target(s) is measured without giving the user the cues from the visible target testing. Such tests help measure memory retention of the user in navigating the VR environment.
- the tool uses ( 230 ) the measured performance of the user in the VR environment in analysis of cognitive status. For example, the tool assesses: (a) the presence or extent of age-related cognitive decline (e.g., a decline in memory performance or learning performance), (b) presence or extent of pediatric cognitive disability (e.g., a memory performance problem or learning performance problem), (c) presence or extent of progression of Alzheimer's disease, (d) presence of a characteristic of pre-clinical Alzheimer's disease, and/or (e) response of the user to therapeutic intervention to treat cognitive decline.
- the tool can use artificial intelligence mechanisms such as classifiers (e.g., neural networks) for pattern recognition, statistical analysis, etc. Example therapeutic interventions are presented below.
- the tool uses the measured performance for a different type of analysis.
- the cognitive status assessment relates the measured performance to a cognitive status classification.
- the tool can compensate for the effects of sex, age and/or learning about the VR environment (e.g., navigation skills, landscape) on the measured performance.
- the following sections describe observed correlations between sex, age and learning in example uses of the generalized technique ( 200 ), and such correlations can be compensated for during the assessment of cognitive status.
- the user repeatedly takes the VR navigation test and the performance of the user over time is measured so as to assess changes in cognitive status of the user. Typically, this involves comparing cognitive status assessments from trial to trial for the user. In other implementations, the results of testing are compared for multiple users, e.g., as part of population studies for the efficacy of a therapy.
- FIG. 3 shows a generalized technique ( 300 ) for using measured performance of a user on a Novel Image Novel Location (“NINL”) test in analysis of cognitive status.
- a software tool such as one operating in the computer system environment shown in FIG. 1 or other tool performs the technique.
- the NINL test is administered by a human supervisor using paper materials.
- Example use scenarios and example clinical applications for the generalized technique ( 300 ) are described below.
- the tool receives ( 310 ) input from the user as the user takes the NINL test.
- a software tool graphically presents the NINL test to the user on a computer monitor as a series of images in panels or a slideshow.
- the software tool accepts input from the user via a keyboard, touchpad or mouse, or the software tool receives and process voice input from the user, or the software tool receives and processes another kind of input.
- the input indicates whether the user perceives “no change” in an image or group of images presented to the user, a “new location” in the image(s), or a “new image” in the image(s).
- the choices presented to the user and/or selections received from the user have a different format.
- the tool measures ( 320 ) performance of the user in the NINL test based at least in part upon the received input.
- the tool presents a first set of images organized by location to the user.
- the first set of images includes multiple panels with each panel including multiple images.
- the tool then presents a second set of images organized by location to the user.
- the second set of images includes multiple panels with each panel including multiple images.
- one or more images of the second set of images differs in image content and/or image location relative to the first set of images.
- the sets of images for the NINL test have a different configuration.
- the tool measures one or more of the following: (1) a novel location score indicating performance of the user in identifying changes in locations of images; (2) a novel image score indicating performance of the user in identifying new images in a second set of one or more images relative to a first set of one or more images; and/or (3) a no change score indicating performance of the user in identifying situations with no change in image content or image location for a second set of one or more images relative to a first set of one or more images.
- the tool measures performance using other and/or additional metrics.
- the tool measures performance in an “immediate” NINL test and also measures performance in a “delayed” NINL test.
- the immediate NINL test occurs shortly after the user reviews a training set of images for the NINL test
- the delayed NINL test occurs some defined period (e.g., five minutes) after the immediate NINL test. The duration of the defined delay period depends on implementation and is set to measure memory retention performance of the user.
- the tool uses ( 330 ) the measured performance of the user on the test in analysis of cognitive status. For example, the tool assesses: (a) the presence or extent of age-related cognitive decline (e.g., a decline in memory performance or learning performance), (b) presence or extent of pediatric cognitive disability (e.g., a memory performance problem or learning performance problem), (c) presence or extent of progression of Alzheimer's disease, (d) presence of a characteristic of pre-clinical Alzheimer's disease, and/or (e) response of the user to therapeutic intervention to treat cognitive decline. Example therapeutic interventions are presented below. Alternatively, the tool assesses cognitive status for a different type of cognitive assessment.
- age-related cognitive decline e.g., a decline in memory performance or learning performance
- pediatric cognitive disability e.g., a memory performance problem or learning performance problem
- c presence or extent of progression of Alzheimer's disease
- a characteristic of pre-clinical Alzheimer's disease e.g., a characteristic of pre-clinical Alzheimer's disease
- the cognitive status assessment relates the measured performance to a cognitive status classification.
- the tool can compensate for the effects of sex, age and/or learning about the testing on the measured performance.
- the following sections describe observed correlations between sex, age and learning in example uses of the generalized technique ( 300 ), and such correlations can be compensated for during the assessment of cognitive status.
- the user repeatedly takes the NINL test and the performance of the user over time is measured so as to assess changes in cognitive status of the user. Typically, this involves comparing cognitive status assessments from trial to trial for the user. In other implementations, the results of testing are compared for multiple users, e.g., as part of population studies for the efficacy of a therapy.
- the generalized techniques ( 200 , 300 ) can be used in various scenarios, including but not limited to home use scenarios, professional use scenarios with fMRI equipment, professional use scenarios with MRI equipment, and professional use scenarios with just the testing.
- the equipment when used with MRI equipment (or fMRI equipment), the equipment measures neural activity of the user as the user takes the NINL test and/or VR test. A cognitive status assessment for the user can then also be based on the measured neural activity.
- the user when used in a home use scenario, takes the NINL test and/or the VR test on a home computer system such as a desktop or laptop computer.
- the test can be delivered to the user on a computer-readable medium such as a disk or delivered to the user over a network connection from a server computer system.
- the user inputs can be received and processed locally to measure performance and assess cognitive status, or information can be forwarded to a remote server computer site to measure performance and/or assess cognitive status.
- the NINL testing and/or VR testing is performed in conjunction with other and/or additional batteries of cognitive tests or physical evaluations of the user.
- Described implementations can be used in a variety of contexts, such as psychological testing or clinical trials involving children and/or the elderly.
- the NINL object recognition test and the Memory Island spatial navigation test turned out to be sensitive to detect differences in learning and memory performance in these two populations.
- the sensitivity of these tests is a potential benefit or advantage over other technology.
- these tests were shown to be sensitive to effects of APOE ⁇ 4 (a risk factor for developing Alzheimer's disease and cognitive impairments following various environmental challenges) in non-demented elderly and children.
- APOE ⁇ 4 a risk factor for developing Alzheimer's disease and cognitive impairments following various environmental challenges
- cognitive status assessments are used to make decisions about therapeutic interventions for users (e.g., children or elderly).
- a therapeutic intervention is a known or proposed treatment for cognitive decline, such as the cognitive decline caused by normal aging or a pathological process, such as Alzheimer's disease or another condition associated with dementia, such as a neurological disease (for example, Huntington's Disease, Parkinson's disease, Creutzfeldt-Jakob Disease or a brain tumor), a vascular disorder (such as multi-infarct dementia or stroke), an infectious etiology (such as HIV/AIDS, spongiform encephalopathy, or syphilis), a toxic exposure (for example, to lead or alcohol), or an undesired effect of a drug.
- the treatment is a proposed treatment, it can be administered as part of a clinical trial, and the response of the subject to the treatment can be assessed by the performance of the subject in the VR environment and/or NINL testing.
- the therapeutic intervention includes a drug therapy
- the cognitive status assessment is used in determining a therapeutic dose of the drug therapy.
- the therapeutic intervention is an APOE ⁇ 4 inhibitor, an APOE ⁇ 3 or APOE ⁇ 2 mimetic, a cholinesterase inhibitor, an N-methyl-aspartate receptor antagonist, or a vitamin.
- the therapeutic intervention includes hormone therapy using testosterone and/or another androgen, or using estrogen.
- a computer-generated VR island environment was developed to mirror the water maze paradigm of spatial learning and memory sensitive to effects of sex on age-related cognitive decline in mouse studies.
- the participants were trained to navigate to a visible target and subsequently to a hidden target.
- a joystick was used to control direction and speed of body movement.
- potential effects of sex on facial recognition and object recognition were tested using faces and NINL testing, respectively.
- HMD head-mounted display
- the testing began with two non-computerized memory tests. The first of these was a facial recognition test (Faces I and Faces II), a part of the Wechsler Memory Scale III developed and published by the Psychological Corporation. In this test, the participant was shown a series of 24 faces and asked to remember each one. Immediately after, the participant was shown another series of 48 faces (the 24 original faces plus 24 distracter faces) and asked to indicate whether each face was one of the faces they were directed to remember earlier or not (Faces I score). After an interval of five minutes, the participant was shown a different set of 48 faces (the same 24 original faces plus 24 new distracter faces) and again asked to indicate whether each face was one of the faces they were directed to remember earlier or not (Faces II score). For the Faces I and Faces II scores, the participant received one point for a correct response and zero points for an incorrect response with a maximal total of 48 points.
- NINL Novel Image, Novel Location
- FIG. 4 is a diagram showing example panels of the NINL software tool. On the left are panels from the first set. On the right are the corresponding panels from the second set, containing a novel location (A), novel image (B), or no change (C).
- A novel location
- B novel image
- C no change
- the participant was asked to remember the images and their positions. After the participant had been presented the first set of 12 panels, they were immediately presented with a second set of 12 panels. After five minutes, they were again presented the second set of 12 panels. In the second set, the panels were either identical to, or slightly different from their counterparts in the first set. The variations in the new panels were either in the positioning of one of the three images (Novel Location) or in that they contained a novel image in the location previously containing one of the three familiar images (Novel Image). Out of the 12 panels in the second set, 4 panels were identical to panels shown in the first set, four panels contained a familiar image in a novel location, and four panels contained a novel image in place of a familiar image.
- the participant was asked to identify the new panel as being either identical to the corresponding panel in the first set (“Yes” answer), or containing a novel image or a novel location of a familiar image (“No” answer), with a maximum score of 12. These answers provided the total score.
- Test performance was also analyzed with four sub-scores. If a panel was identified as containing a novel image, the participant was asked which image on the panel was novel. If a panel was identified as containing a novel location of a familiar image, the participant was asked the novel location of the familiar image on the panel.
- the “No Change” sub-score (four points max) reflected correct identification of panels identical to those seen in the first set. No points were deducted for incorrect identification of a panel as being identical to one seen in the first set.
- the “Change” sub-score (eight points max) reflected the ability to identify and characterize the type of anomaly in the panel (novel location or novel image), but not whether the particular image that changed was identified.
- a computer-generated virtual reality world (Memory Island) was used to assess spatial learning and memory.
- the participants were immersed in a computer-generated three-dimensional environment through a HMD system (HMD model V8, Virtual Research System Inc., Santa Clara, Calif.) comprised of special LCD video goggles and Sennheiser headphones.
- HMD system HMD model V8, Virtual Research System Inc., Santa Clara, Calif.
- LCD video goggles and Sennheiser headphones Inside the visor of the helmet were two video screens, one for each eye, generating a three-dimensional visual experience.
- Two earphones presented stereo sounds that coincided with the visual images in the visor, further enhancing the immersion experience.
- a Microsoft Sidewinder joystick determined the direction and speed of movement in the virtual world.
- an additional cohort of participants was tested with and without the HMD in two subsequent sessions using a counterbalanced design.
- Each session included four visible target trials, four hidden target trials (target only visible in very close proximity to the target), and a probe trial (no target present). Movement of the participant was tracked and recorded in time-stamped coordinate files, which were used to calculate speed of movement, time to reach the target (latency), and percentage time spent in each quadrant during the visible target session and hidden target session. Percentage time spent in each quadrant is a valuable measure, as it is usually independent of velocity.
- FIG. 5 is a diagram showing screen shots of the virtual reality, spatial navigation Memory Island software tool.
- a flag marks the location of the target during the visible target session (A) while no flag is present during the hidden target session (B).
- Each quadrant of the island has a different target.
- the target in quadrant 1 is a fountain (C), in quadrant 2 a piece of moving art (D), in quadrant 3 a seal (E), and in quadrant 4 a seagull (F).
- each quadrant contained a different target item.
- the participant was first asked to navigate to a target location visibly marked with a flag adjacent to the target (visible target).
- Targets in all four quadrants were used for visible target training in four consecutive trials.
- the starting orientation of the participant was varied in each trial, and these variations were kept consistent for all participants. As the starting orientation for a particular trial influenced the difficulty level of that trial, mean performance over the four trials of the visible or hidden target session were used for data analysis rather than performance during individual trials.
- the participant was trained to navigate to a hidden target (here, no flag adjacent to the target, so the participant had to remember where the hidden target was and how to get there). The location of the hidden target was kept constant for each participant.
- Participants were given four trials with the hidden target. If the participant was unable to locate the target within two minutes, an arrow appeared to guide them to it. Trials in which the target was located within two minutes were defined as successful trials. The percentage of successful trials in the visible and hidden target session was used as an additional performance measure. Following the hidden target trials, the participant received a thirty second probe trial (target removed).
- FIGS. 8A-8E are charts showing, for males and females tested with a virtual reality, spatial navigation software tool in hidden target and visible target trials, (A) results for latency to reach the target with (+) or without ( ⁇ ) wearing a HMD, (B) velocity, (C) latency to reach the target, (D) percentage time in the target quadrant, (E) percentage of successful trials, respectively.
- Percentage time in the target quadrant which is typically not affected by velocity, was also measured.
- female and male participants had no difficulty in locating the target and spent close to 100% of their time searching in the target quadrant ( FIG. 8D ).
- a successful trial was defined as a trial in which the target was located within 120 seconds.
- FIGS. 9A-9C are charts showing correlation between NINL total scores and latency to reach the target during a visible target session with a virtual reality, spatial navigation software tool, correlation between NINL total scores and latency to reach the target during a hidden target session with a virtual reality, spatial navigation software tool, and correlation between NINL total scores and percentage of time spent in the target quadrant during a probe trial, respectively.
- test-retest NINL total scores during the same visit were highly correlated, comparable to the test-retest correlations obtained in the established facial recognition test.
- spatial navigation test effects were identified for sex on spatial learning and memory during the session with the hidden, but not visible, target. No statistically significant effects were identified for sex on object recognition.
- the identified sex differences in spatial learning and memory on Memory Island are consistent with sex differences in visual spatial perception and in spatial learning and memory in real and other virtual environment navigation tasks.
- Functional magnetic resonance imaging (“fMRI”) during navigational tasks has shown that women recruit the right parietal and right prefrontal area, whereas men recruit the left hippocampal area, which may relate to the predominant use of landmark cues by women and geometric and landmark cues by men. However, it might be more complex.
- the Memory Island test environment contains landmarks predicting the target location and still showed sex differences in performance. These data tend to show that the sex differences in spatial memory do not require the exclusion of stable landmarks.
- the Memory Island test can be distinguished from prior studies that have adapted the water maze test to study spatial learning and memory in humans using VR. Programs designed to mirror the water maze test in rodent studies might lack elements found in real world situations. Compared to the prior studies, Memory Island involves a higher degree of immersion into the virtual environment. For example, Memory Island also contains environmental sounds (e.g., birds). While not a water maze environment, the design and analysis of the water maze test was incorporated into the design of Memory Island. Importantly, none of the participants experienced nausea or dizziness on Memory Island, while 10% of the participants experienced these symptoms after exposure to a virtual environment of interconnected hallways and other virtual environments in some prior studies.
- Non- ⁇ 4- and ⁇ 4-carrying men and women did not differ in age, or Mini-Mental State Examination, Wide Range Achievement Test-Reading, Beck Anxiety Inventory, or reaction time scores.
- an effect was identified for ⁇ 4 on the object recognition and spatial navigation tests, however, with non- ⁇ 4 carriers outperforming ⁇ 4 carriers, but not in the other cognitive tests. No relationship was found for sex and ⁇ 4 status or sex and performance during the hidden target session of Memory Island.
- salivary cortisol levels correlated with object recognition.
- MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination
- the final sample was composed of 115 participants, all whites.
- the sample was divided into two APOE genotype groups, ⁇ 4 carriers and non- ⁇ 4 carriers. Those in the non- ⁇ 4 carriers group represented ⁇ 3/ ⁇ 3 homozygotes and ⁇ 2/ ⁇ 3 heterozygotes. Those in the ⁇ 4 carriers group represented ⁇ 4/ ⁇ 4 homozygotes, ⁇ 2/ ⁇ 4, and ⁇ 3/ ⁇ 4 heterozygotes (Table 1).
- the group of women consisted of ⁇ 6 individuals (mean age ⁇ S.E.M., 81.2 ⁇ 0.7 years of age), among them 65 non- ⁇ 4 carriers and 21 ⁇ 4 carriers.
- the group of men consisted of 29 individuals (mean age ⁇ S.E.M., 82.9 ⁇ 0.9 years of age), among them 24 non- ⁇ 4 carriers and five ⁇ 4 carriers. There was no significant sex difference in the proportion of ⁇ 4 carriers among men and women.
- 111 participants had a MMSE score greater than 23 which corresponds to a cutoff score for cognitively healthy people.
- the four participants who obtained a MMSE score below 24 performed well on the other cognitive tests.
- MMSE scores can be affected by other conditions such as hearing impairment, the data were analyzed with and without these four individuals included. Both analyses revealed a similar pattern of results. Therefore, these four participants were not excluded from the study.
- Premorbid intellectual functioning general intelligence levels were evaluated using the Wide Range Achievement Test-Reading (“WRAT-R”) instead of years of formal education. As anxiety levels and reaction times can influence performance on cognitive tests, they were analyzed as well. Levels of anxiety were assessed using the Beck Anxiety Inventory (“BAI”). Reaction times were measured by presenting (on a computer screen) a series of colored ellipses at varying time intervals and asking the participants to press a button as soon as the ellipse appeared (Gary Darby, “Reaction Times,” http://www.delphiforfun.org/Programs/Reaction_times.htm ( ⁇ 2000-2007)). The amount of time between the appearance of the stimulus and the time the button was pressed was recorded.
- BAI Beck Anxiety Inventory
- samples of saliva were collected at the beginning of an evaluation session for a user. (See Table 3.)
- Test stage a Description 1 Collection of a saliva sample b 2 Facial Recognition Immediate [Faces I] 5 min delay Face Recognition Delayed [Faces II] 3 NINL I 5 min delay NINL II 4 Reaction times 5 Memory Island: 3 Visible trials (three different targets: Seagull Art Piece Seal) 3 Hidden trials (Seagull Target) Probe trial 6 MMSE 7 FP I 5 min delay FP II 8 OraGene DNA Test c a Three months following this session, additional tests such as BAI, the WAIS SSF and SSB, and the WRAT-R were conducted. These tests were not included in the first visit to minimize the length of the visit and potential fatigue. b Saliva samples were used to determine salivary cortisol and testosterone levels. c OraGene tests were used to determine APOE genotype.
- the neuropsychological tests were administered in a designated apartment of the retirement community in two visits lasting around two hours and one hour, respectively.
- Table 3 illustrates the sequence of neuropsychological testing in both visits and the salivary collection. To control for circadian variations in hormone levels, all examinees were tested in the morning starting at 8:30 a.m.
- FP I Family Pictures I
- FP II Family Pictures II
- Wechsler Memory Scale III developed and published by the Psychological Corporation.
- the participants were asked to memorize as many details as they could from four different cartoon-like family scenes. After the four scenes were displayed for 10 seconds each, the examinees were prompted to describe which characters were in each scene, where they were positioned in the scene, and what they were doing. After an interval of five minutes, the study participants were asked the same question as in FP I. Each correct answer was scored as one point. Performance was analyzed using FP I and FP II scores.
- NINL Novel Image Novel Location
- This example NINL test consists of two sets of three-image panels (12 panels in each set).
- the three pictures on each panel are similar in complexity but different in content and how they are randomly located in three of the four quadrants of the panel (A, B, C, and D).
- the first set of 12 panels is the reference set (here, the set participants are asked to memorize).
- the second set is the test set; panels are either identical to, or slightly different from, their counterparts in the first set.
- the variations in the second set of panels are either in the positioning of one of the three images or in that they contain a novel image instead of one of the three familiar images.
- Out of the 12 panels in the second set four panels are identical to panels shown in the first set, four panels contain a familiar image in a novel location, and four panels contain a novel image in the original position of a familiar image.
- NINL I maximum of 12 points
- NINL II NINL delayed score
- Test performance was analyzed using NINL I, NINL II and three sub-scores for each.
- the Novel Location and Novel Image sub-scores reflected the ability to correctly identify the exact Novel Location and Novel Image, respectively (maximal four points each).
- the No Change sub-score reflected correct identification of panels identical to those seen in the first set (maximal four points).
- SSF and SSB are also part of the Wechsler Memory Scale III developed and published by the Psychological Corporation. They provide a nonverbal measure of immediate memory. Both tests involve a board containing ten randomly anchored cubes. The participants were asked to watch the investigator tap the cubes (1 cube/second) in a prearranged order and to reproduce these tapping sequences. In the SSF test the tapping is performed in order of the example (forward), while in the SSB test it is performed in reverse order compared with the example (backward). The difficulty increases with the number of cubes tapped; from two cubes up to nine cubes. A correct sequence is scored as one point. Testing ended when the participants failed to reproduce the sequence correctly after two trials or when the pair of nine cubes sequence was tapped correctly.
- Memory Island a computer-generated virtual reality (“VR”) world described in Section VI, was used in the second series of trials to assess spatial learning and memory requiring navigation. Briefly, the participants were immersed through a high quality 19-in. Dell computer monitor and a Harmon Kardon HK395 stereo speaker system with subwoofer. A Microsoft Sidewinder joystick was used to determine the direction and speed of movement in the virtual world. The virtual world simulated an island environment of 347 ⁇ 287 m 2 composed of four quadrants, each containing a different target item.
- VR virtual reality
- salivary cortisol and testosterone levels were assessed for assessment of salivary cortisol and testosterone levels and to assess potential correlations of these levels with measures of cognitive performance.
- samples of saliva were collected at the beginning of the session. (See Table 3.)
- Salivary cortisol and testosterone levels were determined using commercial kits by the General Clinical Research Center at Oregon Health Science University.
- ⁇ 2 might provide protection against age-related cognitive decline
- possible effects of ⁇ 2 on cognitive performance of the study participants were assessed. As no such effects were found, the study participants were divided in ⁇ 4 carriers and non- ⁇ 4 carriers. The lack of an effect of ⁇ 2 on cognitive performance of the study participants might have been caused by the relatively low number of ⁇ 2 carriers in this sample.
- a mixed effects model replicated measures design
- This model was also adopted for analyzing performance across trials in Memory Island. Based on the Bayesian information criterion, a compound symmetrical structure as the variance-covariance matrix was selected.
- GEE generalized estimating equation
- SPSS SPSS version 14.0: SPSS Inc., Chicago, Ill., USA
- Statistical Analysis System version 9.1 SAS Institute, Cary, N.C., USA
- the estimated mean for the Faces II was 35.70, while the estimated mean for the Faces I was 33.95 with a standard error of 0.53.
- FIGS. 10C and 10D are charts showing, for elderly women and men, effect of ⁇ 4 on Novel Location sub-scores (combined immediate and delayed scores), and effect of sex on Novel Location sub-scores, respectively.
- ⁇ 4 ⁇ 4 carriers performing better than ⁇ 4 carriers
- FIGS. 11A and 11B are charts showing, for elderly women and men, effect of sex on velocity during visible target trials, and effect of sex on velocity during hidden target trials, respectively.
- FIGS. 11A and 11B are charts showing, for elderly women and men, effect of sex on velocity during visible target trials, and effect of sex on velocity during hidden target trials, respectively.
- 12A-12F are charts showing, for elderly women and men, (A) effect of ⁇ 4 on velocity during a visible target session, (B) effect of ⁇ 4 on velocity during a hidden target session, (C) effect of ⁇ 4 on cumulative distance during a visible target session, (D) effect of ⁇ 4 on cumulative distance during a hidden target session, (E) effect of ⁇ 4 on latency to reach target during a visible target session, and (F) effect of ⁇ 4 on latency to reach target during a hidden target session (P ⁇ 0.05), respectively.
- FIGS. 13A and 13B are charts showing, for elderly women and men tested with a virtual reality, spatial navigation software tool in a probe trial (target removed), effect of sex on percentage of time spent in four quadrants, and effect of ⁇ 4 on percentage of time spent in four quadrants, respectively.
- Non- ⁇ 4 carriers spent more time in the target compared with the left quadrant (P ⁇ 0.001) and the opposite quadrant (P ⁇ 0.05) while ⁇ 4 carriers spent more time in the right quadrant compared with the target quadrant (P ⁇ 0.01).
- Cortisol Level Measure Women Men Non ⁇ 4 ⁇ 4 MMSE a ⁇ 0.05 ⁇ 0.32 ⁇ 0.05 ⁇ 0.22 BAI 0.05 0.12 0.03 0.39 WRAT-R ⁇ 0.08 0.03 ⁇ 0.03 ⁇ 0.14 Reaction 0.16 ⁇ 0.33 0.05 ⁇ 0.10 times b Spatial SSF 0.00 0.27 0.03 0.03 span c SSB ⁇ 0.03 ⁇ 0.11 ⁇ 0.11 0.05 Faces d Faces immediate 0.04 ⁇ 0.22 0.03 ⁇ 0.17 Faces delayed ⁇ 0.11 ⁇ 0.23 ⁇ 0.04 ⁇ 0.49 $ Family Family Pictures ⁇ 0.08 ⁇ 0.07 0.05 ⁇ 0.23 Pictures immediate Family Pictures ⁇ 0.03 ⁇ 0.15 0.04 ⁇ 0.15 delayed NINL a No change immediate ⁇ 0.08 ⁇ 0.06 ⁇ 0.09 0.02 No change delayed ⁇ 0.09 ⁇ 0.02 ⁇ 0.08 ⁇ 0.07 Novel image ⁇ 0.05 ⁇ 0.45
- FIGS. 15A and 15B are charts showing, for elderly men, correlation of salivary cortisol levels with NINL I novel image recognition, and correlation of salivary cortisol levels with NINL II novel image recognition, respectively.
- salivary testosterone levels there was a sex ⁇ 4 interaction, with higher salivary testosterone levels in ⁇ 4-carrying than non- ⁇ 4-carrying men and lower salivary testosterone levels in ⁇ 4-carrying than non- ⁇ 4-carrying women. (See FIG. 14 .)
- salivary testosterone levels there was a sex ⁇ 4 interaction, with higher salivary testosterone levels in ⁇ 4-carrying than non- ⁇ 4-carrying men and lower salivary testosterone levels in ⁇ 4-carrying than non- ⁇ 4-carrying women.
- the object recognition and spatial navigation tests were sensitive to effects of ⁇ 4 in the elderly. Differences in neuroanatomy, brain glucose metabolism during mental activity, and brain activation in memory tasks between non-demented non- ⁇ 4 and ⁇ 4 carriers might contribute to these effects. As the mean age of the participants of this study was 82, these data indicate that while the ⁇ 4-associated risk to develop AD is age-dependent and maximal before this age, effects of ⁇ 4 on cognitive performance can be revealed in the old-old using episodic memory tests.
- APOE ⁇ 4 is a risk factor for age-related cognitive decline and cognitive impairments following environmental challenges.
- APOE ⁇ 4 has effects on cognitive performance in children, they were given standardized cognitive tests as well as an object recognition test and a spatial navigation test sensitive to effects of APOE ⁇ 4 in the elderly.
- Children with APOE ⁇ 4 showed reduced novel location recognition, reduced ability to navigate to a visible target, and reduced spatial memory retention.
- the early effect of APOE ⁇ 4 on cognition indicates predisposition to cognitive impairments later in life.
- ⁇ 4 increases the risk of age-related cognitive decline and cognitive injury following environmental insults.
- 55 healthy 7-10 year-olds (girls: 17 non- ⁇ 4 and eight- ⁇ 4; boys: 24 non- ⁇ 4 and six- ⁇ 4) were given established cognitive tests, as well as an object recognition test and a spatial navigation test requiring navigation sensitive to effects of ⁇ 4 in non-demented elderly, and provided saliva samples for APOE genotyping and cortisol levels.
- the inclusion criteria were healthy boys and girls 7-10 years of age.
- the exclusion criteria were children whose birth mother or legal guardian could not be interviewed, children with severe visual impairments, children born more than 35 weeks premature, children with epilepsy, head injury, Tourette's syndrome, cerebral palsy, congenital abnormalities, severe brain trauma, diagnosed with leukemia or any other medical condition that could interfere with cognitive ability, or children exposed to elicit drugs during pregnancy.
- Children were from middle/upper class families and 84% Caucasian, 7% Hispanic, 3.5% African American, and 3.5% Pacific Islander.
- the average age of the mother's was 29 years old with 12.0% reporting smoking and 17.2% reporting at least some alcohol consumption during pregnancy.
- FIGS. 16A , 16 B and 16 C show effects of ⁇ 4 on Memory Island performance in 7-10 year-old boys and girls.
- FIG. 16A shows effects of sex on velocity during the visible and hidden target trials (*p ⁇ 0.04).
- FIG. 16B shows effects of ⁇ 4 on cumulative distance to the target in the visible (*p ⁇ 0.005), but not hidden, target trials.
- FIG. 16C shows percentage of time spend in quadrants for ⁇ 4 and non- ⁇ 4 children (*p ⁇ 0.04 for the target quadrant versus all other quadrants). Compared to the left and right quadrants, the children tended to spend more time in the opposite quadrant. This might be due to the fact that the start orientation in the probe trial faces the opposite quadrant.
- non- ⁇ 4 carriers showed spatial memory retention in the probe trial (no target present) and searched most of the time in the quadrant previously containing the target (p ⁇ 0.04) but ⁇ 4 carriers did not. (See FIG. 16C .) Similarly, while 75.6% of non- ⁇ 4 carriers showed target preference, only 43% of ⁇ 4 carriers did (p ⁇ 0.04, Fisher's exact test).
- effects of ⁇ 4 on spatial learning and memory are already detected in 7-10 year-old children and indicate predisposition to cognitive injury following environmental challenges and/or age-related cognitive decline.
- the cognitive status of an elderly subject suspected to have dementia is assessed.
- the subject is given the Memory Island and NINL tests described herein.
- the subject's performance on the Memory Island test e.g., distance traversed, time elapsed before reaching target, percentage of successful trials, time spent in target area, velocity of movement
- NINL tests e.g., Novel Location score, Novel Image score, No Change score, total NINL score
- the cognitive status of an elderly subject suspected to have age-associated dementia is assessed over time.
- the subject is initially given Memory Island and NINL tests described herein.
- the subject's performance on the Memory Island and NINL tests is compared with previously measured performance benchmarks for non-demented individuals of the same sex and similar age.
- One year later the subject is given a second round of Memory Island and NINL tests.
- the subject's performance on the Memory Island and NINL tests is compared with the subject's own performance the previous year.
- the cognitive status of a child subject suspected to have a genetic predisposition toward dementia later in life is assessed.
- the child subject is given the Memory Island and NINL tests described herein.
- the child subject's performance on the Memory Island and NINL tests is compared with previously measured performance benchmarks for non-demented individuals of the same sex and similar age.
- a subject who has been diagnosed with dementia is given the Memory Island and NINL tests described herein.
- Baseline measures are determined (for Memory Island: distance traversed, time elapsed before reaching target, percentage of successful trials, time spent in target area, velocity of movement; for NINL: Novel Location score, Novel Image score, No Change score, total NINL score), and the subject is given an anti-Alzheimer's drug (such as Donepezil (ARICEPT) or anti-APOE ⁇ 4 drug) as a therapeutic intervention.
- an anti-Alzheimer's drug such as Donepezil (ARICEPT) or anti-APOE ⁇ 4 drug
- the subject For a subject given Donepezil, the subject is treated with 5 mg per day of the drug for a period of at least four to six weeks, and then the subject is once again given the Memory Island and NINL tests. If a desired improvement in scores is not obtained, then the dose of the drug is increased to 10 mg per day. Testing with the Memory Island and NINL tests is then repeated to assess the response to the drug, where a change in test scores indicates a response to the drug.
- a subject who has been diagnosed with dementia is given the Memory Island and NINL tests described herein.
- Baseline measures are determined as described in Example 4, and the subject is given an anti-Alzheimer's drug (such as Donepezil (ARICEPT) or anti-APOE ⁇ 4 drug) as a therapeutic intervention.
- an anti-Alzheimer's drug such as Donepezil (ARICEPT) or anti-APOE ⁇ 4 drug
- the subject For a subject given Donepezil, the subject is treated with 5 mg per day of the drug for a period of at least four to six weeks, and then the subject is once again given the Memory Island and NINL tests. If a desired improvement in scores is not obtained, the subject is given a different drug. Testing with the Memory Island and NINL tests is then repeated to assess the response to the different drug, where a change in test scores indicates a response to the different drug.
Landscapes
- Health & Medical Sciences (AREA)
- Life Sciences & Earth Sciences (AREA)
- Engineering & Computer Science (AREA)
- General Health & Medical Sciences (AREA)
- Neurology (AREA)
- Medical Informatics (AREA)
- Public Health (AREA)
- Psychology (AREA)
- Psychiatry (AREA)
- Hospice & Palliative Care (AREA)
- Developmental Disabilities (AREA)
- Child & Adolescent Psychology (AREA)
- Veterinary Medicine (AREA)
- Biophysics (AREA)
- Physics & Mathematics (AREA)
- Surgery (AREA)
- Molecular Biology (AREA)
- Heart & Thoracic Surgery (AREA)
- Biomedical Technology (AREA)
- Animal Behavior & Ethology (AREA)
- Pathology (AREA)
- Social Psychology (AREA)
- Epidemiology (AREA)
- Primary Health Care (AREA)
- Neurosurgery (AREA)
- Physiology (AREA)
- Ophthalmology & Optometry (AREA)
- Educational Technology (AREA)
- Measurement Of The Respiration, Hearing Ability, Form, And Blood Characteristics Of Living Organisms (AREA)
- User Interface Of Digital Computer (AREA)
Abstract
Techniques and tools for measuring cognitive ability and/or detecting cognitive impairment or decline. For example, techniques and tools are described that can be used to diagnose or test susceptibility to cognitive impairments in children or in elderly people (such as cognitive impairments associated with Alzheimer's Disease). Techniques and tools are described that can be used to evaluate treatment effects and/or measure cognitive decline over time.
Description
- The present application claims the benefit of U.S. Provisional Patent Application No. 60/928,577, entitled “Computer Software-Oriented Tools and Techniques for Measuring Cognitive Ability and Cognitive Impairment,” filed May 9, 2007, the disclosure of which is incorporated by reference.
- The inventions described in this patent application were made in part by government support under NIH Grant # P30 AG08017. The United States Government may have rights in these inventions.
- The present disclosure relates to tools and techniques for measuring cognitive ability and/or impairment.
- Many environmental and intrinsic factors influence cognitive function. Intrinsic factors that can influence cognitive function include sex, age and genetic makeup.
- Effects of sex on cognitive function have been shown in humans and animal models using established tests. Sex differences have been demonstrated in both episodic memory tasks (favoring women) and spatial visualization tasks (favoring men). Interestingly, in some studies alcohol consumption abolished sex differences in spatial visualization, but not episodic memory performance. In addition, stress has been shown to differentially affect fear conditioning in men and women.
- Consistent with the human studies, effects of sex on cognitive function have also been reported in animal models using established tests. In general, studies of spatial learning and memory in rodents have shown that males learn more quickly than females and exhibit superior performance in a variety of mazes. Some studies, however, have not shown such differences between the sexes. Sex differences in classical fear conditioning and shuttlebox avoidance conditioning in rats have also been reported. In addition, in some studies neonatal isolation facilitated appetitive response learning in adult female, but not male, rats.
- Cognitive tests administered to humans and animals frequently involve large differences. Therefore, it often remains difficult to directly compare results on these tests across species. For example, while spatial learning and memory can be easily assessed in humans and animal models, to compare assessments of spatial learning and memory in humans and mice, navigation to a target can be important. In some tests of spatial memory, when all the information is within one field of view, the participant has an aerial perspective and a body-centered (egocentric) frame of reference (e.g. table-top tests of spatial memory). Such tests are routinely used to assess visuospatial memory, but are very different from tests of spatial memory typically used for rodents. Testing visuospatial memory in rodents typically involves a viewer perspective of a world-centered (allocentric) frame of reference with information found throughout a complex environment in which the participant has to navigate. Making direct inferences about performance on navigation tests from performance on table-top tests can be problematic.
- Virtual reality (“VR”), which has been used to assess, expose, and desensitize (in phobias) event and place-related memories, to assess and teach driving and flying skills, and to distract in pain management, can also be used to assess spatial learning and memory in humans using a navigational task. Navigation in a virtual environment has been shown to be sensitive to effects of sex of participants in some, but not all, studies. In one study, a virtual environment consisting of a series of interconnected hallways, some leading to dead ends and others leading to a designated goal location, was used to study age and sex differences in spatial navigation. In this study, there was no significant effect of sex on time to complete the maze or total distance traveled, but there was an effect of sex on total number of deviations from the correct route into a dead-end corridor, and there was an effect of sex on how often participants traveled on a portion of the correct route through which they had already traveled. However, as there was no cued version of this test, it is difficult to distinguish task learning performance from spatial learning and memory performance. In another study from the same authors, a virtual water maze environment was used to study the effects of age and sex on spatial learning and memory in humans. (The water maze paradigm is commonly used to assess spatial learning and memory in rodents.) An effect of age, but not of sex, was detected on performance. In this study, a trial with a visible target was given following the trials with a hidden target.
- The three major human isoforms of apolipoprotein E (APOE), which are encoded by distinct APOE alleles (ε2, ε3, and ε4), are involved in the metabolism and redistribution of lipoproteins and cholesterol. Compared with ε2 and ε3, ε4 is associated with increased risk of cognitive impairments and of developing Alzheimer's disease (AD). Women are at higher risk to develop AD than men, particularly women carrying ε4. In contrast to the risk to develop AD, the effects of ε4 on cognitive function in the non-demented elderly old-old (>75 years of age) are less clear. While some studies have shown poor cognitive performance in non-demented elderly ε4 carriers compared with non-demented elderly non-ε4 carriers and a small effect was observed in a meta-analysis, other studies did not.
- In the elderly, high cortisol and low testosterone levels might contribute to reduced cognitive function. In older men and women, higher cortisol levels have been associated with poorer cognitive performance in some studies. However, in another study cortisol levels only inversely correlated with paragraph recall in older participants with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) but not in elderly control participants. APOE genotype might also influence cortisol levels. In AD patients, higher cerebrospinal cortisol levels in ε4 than non-ε4 carriers have been reported, although comparable cerebrospinal cortisol in non-ε4 and ε4 carriers have also been reported. In elderly men, low testosterone levels might also contribute to reduced cognitive function. In older men, testosterone levels have been positively correlated with cognitive function, and cognitive function could be improved by testosterone treatments. Similarly, testosterone, but not estrogen, levels in serum have correlated positively with cognitive performance in older women, and androgen therapy has been shown to improve cognition in surgically menopausal women. The relationship between testosterone levels and cognitive function might be ε4-dependent. In men, low testosterone levels and ε4 have been shown to interact in increasing the risk of developing AD. In addition, an interaction between ε4 and cognitive performance in healthy older men has been reported; while in non-ε4 carriers higher testosterone levels were associated with better general cognition, in ε4 carriers higher testosterone levels were associated with lower cognitive performance.
- This Summary is provided to introduce a selection of concepts in a simplified form that are further described below in the Detailed Description. This Summary is not intended to identify key features or essential features of the claimed subject matter, nor is it intended to be used to limit the scope of the claimed subject matter.
- In summary, the Detailed Description is directed to various techniques and tools for measuring cognitive ability and/or detecting cognitive impairment or decline. For example, techniques and tools are described that can be used to diagnose or test susceptibility to cognitive impairments in children or in elderly people (such as cognitive impairments associated with Alzheimer's Disease). Techniques and tools are described that can be used to evaluate treatment effects and/or measure cognitive decline over time.
- The foregoing and other objects, features, and advantages of the invention will become more apparent from the following detailed description, which proceeds with reference to the accompanying figures.
-
FIG. 1 is a block diagram of a suitable computing environment in conjunction with which several described embodiments may be implemented. -
FIG. 2 is a flowchart of a generalized technique for analysis of cognitive status using VR testing. -
FIG. 3 is a flowchart of a generalized technique for analysis of cognitive status using Novel Image Novel Location (“NINL”) testing. -
FIG. 4 is a diagram showing example panels of a NINL software tool according to one or more described embodiments. -
FIG. 5 is a diagram showing screen shots of a virtual reality, spatial navigation software tool according to one or more described embodiments. -
FIGS. 6A and 6B are charts showing comparable facial recognition scores in male and female participants, and correlation of the Faces I and Faces II scores, respectively. -
FIGS. 7A-7F are charts showing NINL total scores of male and female participants, correlation of NINL I and NINL II, NINL scores for ability to detect a change, NINL scores for ability to detect a novel image, NINL scores for ability to detect a novel location of a familiar image, and correlation of combined NINL total scores and combined facial recognition total scores, respectively, according to one or more described embodiments. -
FIGS. 8A-8E are charts showing, for males and females tested with a virtual reality, spatial navigation software tool in hidden target trials and visible target trials, results for latency to reach the target with (+) or without (−) wearing a head-mounted display (“HMD”), velocity, latency to reach the target, percentage time in the target quadrant, percentage of successful trials, respectively, according to one or more described embodiments.FIG. 8F is a chart showing, for males and females in a probe trial, percentage time in four quadrants, according to one or more described embodiments. -
FIGS. 9A-9C are charts showing correlation between NINL total scores and latency to reach the target during a visible target session with a virtual reality, spatial navigation software tool, correlation between NINL total scores and latency to reach the target during a hidden target session with a virtual reality, spatial navigation software tool, and correlation between NINL total scores and percentage of time spent in the target quadrant during a probe trial, respectively, according to one or more described embodiments. -
FIGS. 10A-10D are charts showing, for elderly women and men, an effect of sex on “Family Pictures” test scores, effect of APOE ε4 on NINL total scores (combined immediate and delayed scores), effect of APOE ε4 on Novel Location sub-scores (combined immediate and delayed scores), and effect of sex on Novel Location sub-scores, respectively, according to one or more described embodiments. -
FIGS. 11A and 11B are charts showing, for elderly women and men tested with a virtual reality, spatial navigation software tool, effect of sex on velocity during visible target trials, and effect of sex on velocity during hidden target trials, respectively, according to one or more described embodiments. -
FIGS. 12A-12F are charts showing, for elderly women and men tested with a virtual reality, spatial navigation software tool, effect of APOE ε4 on velocity during a visible target session, effect of ε4 on velocity during a hidden target session, effect of APOE ε4 on cumulative distance during a visible target session, effect of APOE ε4 on cumulative distance during a hidden target session, effect of APOE ε4 on latency to reach target during a visible target session, and effect of APOE ε4 on latency to reach target during a hidden target session, respectively, according to one or more described embodiments. -
FIGS. 13A and 13B are charts showing, for elderly women and men tested with a virtual reality, spatial navigation software tool in a probe trial, effect of sex on percentage of time spent in four quadrants, and effect of APOE ε4 on percentage of time spent in four quadrants, respectively, according to one or more described embodiments. -
FIG. 14 is a chart showing, for elderly women and men, effect of ε4 on salivary testosterone levels. -
FIGS. 15A and 15B are charts showing, for elderly men, correlation of salivary cortisol levels with NINL I novel image recognition, and correlation of salivary cortisol levels with NINL II novel image recognition, respectively, according to one or more described embodiments. -
FIGS. 16A , 16B and 16C are charts showing effects of APOE ε4 on performance in 7-10 year-old boys and girls tested with a virtual reality, spatial navigation software tool, according to one or more described embodiments. - Described embodiments are directed to techniques and tools for measuring cognitive ability and/or detecting cognitive impairment or decline. For example, techniques and tools are described that can be used to diagnose or test susceptibility to cognitive impairments in children or in elderly people (such as cognitive impairments associated with Alzheimer's Disease). Techniques and tools are described that can be used to evaluate treatment effects and/or measure cognitive decline over time. The various techniques and tools described herein may be used independently. Some of the described techniques and tools can be used in combination.
- The following paragraphs include a discussion of terms used herein.
- “Adjusting for effects of X” refers to adjusting an interpreted result such that the condition X does not skew the interpreted result.
- “Age-related cognitive decline” refers to a reduction in cognition associated with advancing age, e.g., an age-related dementia.
- “Artificial intelligence” refers to information processing performed by one or more computers that mimics human reasoning.
- “Based at least in part on X” means based on X and zero or more other acts, results, or conditions.
- “Cognitive status” refers to status of cognition—mental processes related to knowing, thinking, learning and/or judging.
- “First-person” refers to a simulation of a perspective a user would have if the user were physically present in a virtual environment.
- “Learning of navigation skills” refers to gradual improvement of navigation skills through repetition, such as navigation skills used in a virtual reality environment.
- “Learning of landscape” refers to gradual improvement of knowledge of a landscape, such as a landscape in a virtual reality environment.
- “Measuring neural activity” refers to detecting activated brain regions. For example, neural activity can be measured using functional magnetic resonance imaging (“fMRI”) techniques that measure changes in neuroanatomical activity, such as increased blood flow to areas of the brain having corresponding neurological functions.
- “No change score” refers to a performance signifier that measures performance of a user in identifying situations with no change in image content or image location for a second set of one or more images relative to a first set of one or more images.
- “Novel image” refers to a new image in a second set of one or more images relative to a first set of one or more images.
- “Novel location” refers to a new location of an image in a second set of one or more images relative to a first set of one or more images.
- “Pattern recognition” refers to identification of a pattern in data and association of the identified pattern with a condition or other data.
- “Pediatric cognitive disability” refers to diminished cognition (as compared to unaffected normal peers) in a human child under the age of 18. Pediatric cognitive disability can be associated with, for example, a genetic abnormality or a neuropsychological disturbance.
- “Pre-clinical Alzheimer's disease” refers to Alzheimer's disease in its early stages before memory disturbance significantly interferes with psychosocial function to an extent that a clinical diagnosis can be made based on the memory disturbance.
- “Providing a treatment regimen” refers to setting or adjusting a treatment regimen, such as a dose of an anti-Alzheimer's disease medication or the degree to which an environment is structured to address the effects of dementia.
- “Score” refers to a performance signifier, such as a number or percentage of successful trials.
- “User” refers to a human being that uses or interacts with computer software and/or a computerized system.
- “Virtual reality environment” refers to an environment that simulates a physical environment. For example, a computer can display a virtual reality environment to a user via a graphical display, and the user can interact with the virtual reality environment by transmitting input to the computer.
-
FIG. 1 illustrates a generalized example of a suitable computing environment (100) in which several of the described embodiments may be implemented. The computing environment (100) is not intended to suggest any limitation as to scope of use or functionality, as the techniques and tools may be implemented in diverse general-purpose or special-purpose computing environments. - With reference to
FIG. 1 , the computing environment (100) includes at least one processing unit (110) and memory (120). InFIG. 1 , this most basic configuration (130) is included within a dashed line. The processing unit (110) executes computer-executable instructions and may be a real or a virtual processor. In a multi-processing system, multiple processing units execute computer-executable instructions to increase processing power. The memory (120) may be volatile memory (e.g., registers, cache, RAM), non-volatile memory (e.g., ROM, EEPROM, flash memory, etc.), or some combination of the two. The memory (120) stores software (180) implementing one or more of the described techniques and tools for testing cognitive ability and/or cognitive impairment. - A computing environment may have additional features. For example, the computing environment (100) includes storage (140), one or more input devices (150), one or more output devices (160), and one or more communication connections (170). An interconnection mechanism (not shown) such as a bus, controller, or network interconnects the components of the computing environment (100). Typically, operating system software (not shown) provides an operating environment for other software executing in the computing environment (100), and coordinates activities of the components of the computing environment (100).
- The storage (140) may be removable or non-removable, and includes magnetic disks, magnetic tapes or cassettes, CD-ROMs, DVDs, flash memory, or any other medium which can be used to store information and which can be accessed within the computing environment (100). The storage (140) stores instructions for the software (180).
- The input device(s) (150) may be a touch input device such as a keyboard, mouse, pen, touch screen, or trackball, a voice input device, a scanning device, or another device that provides input to the computing environment (100). For audio or video encoding, the input device(s) (150) may be a sound card, video card, TV tuner card, or similar device that accepts audio or video input in analog or digital form, or a CD-ROM, CD-RW or DVD that reads audio or video samples into the computing environment (100). The output device(s) (160) may be a display, printer, speaker, CD- or DVD-writer, or another device that provides output from the computing environment (100).
- The communication connection(s) (170) enable communication over a communication medium to another computing entity. The communication medium conveys information such as computer-executable instructions, audio or video input or output, or other data in a modulated data signal. A modulated data signal is a signal that has one or more of its characteristics set or changed in such a manner as to encode information in the signal. By way of example, and not limitation, communication media include wired or wireless techniques implemented with an electrical, optical, RF, infrared, acoustic, or other carrier.
- The techniques and tools can be described in the general context of computer-readable media. Computer-readable media are any available media that can be accessed within a computing environment. By way of example, and not limitation, with the computing environment (100), computer-readable media include memory (120), storage (140), communication media, and combinations of any of the above.
- The techniques and tools can be described in the general context of computer-executable instructions, such as those included in program modules, being executed in a computing environment on one or more target real processors or virtual processors. Generally, program modules include routines, programs, libraries, objects, classes, components, data structures, etc. that perform particular tasks or implement particular abstract data types. The functionality of the program modules may be combined or split between program modules as desired in various embodiments. Computer-executable instructions for program modules may be executed within a local or distributed computing environment.
-
FIG. 2 shows a generalized technique (200) for analysis of cognitive status of a user using testing with a virtual reality (“VR”) environment. A software tool such as one operating in the computer system environment shown inFIG. 1 or other tool performs the technique. Example use scenarios and example clinical applications for the generalized technique (200) are described below. - The tool receives (210) input from the user as the user interacts with software presenting a VR environment. In example implementations (including those described in the following sections), the VR environment includes a first-person, three-dimensional graphical rendering of the environment as well as sound cues for the environment. The environment is graphically rendered on a computer monitor or, for a more immersive experience, presented to the user using virtual reality goggles or another head mounted display. Inputs (such as direction of movement, speed of movement) are received from the user using a force-feedback joystick, other joystick, mouse, keyboard or other input device.
- Returning to
FIG. 2 , the tool measures (220) performance of the user in the VR environment based at least in part upon the received input. The organization of the VR environment depends on implementation but typically includes areas such as quadrants which may be organized in terms of a coordinate space. As an intermediate part of measuring performance, for example, the tool tracks position of the user over time in the coordinate space. The results of the tracking are stored in memory or a file (e.g., as timestamped coordinate locations) for later analysis of patterns of user behavior. - In terms of metrics, the tool measures one or more of the following: (1) distance (e.g., cumulative or start to target) traversed in the VR environment, (2) time elapsed before reaching a target (or targets) in the VR environment, (3) percentage of successful trials (where success is, e.g., finding a target), (4) time spent in a target area of the VR environment, (5) velocity of movement in the VR environment, (6) pattern of movement (e.g., between multiple areas or in terms of coordinates) in the VR environment, and/or (7) pattern of time spent in respective areas of the VR environment. Alternatively, the tool measures performance using other and/or additional metrics. Some metrics (such as velocity of movement and time elapsed before reaching a target) may depend on each other to some extent, while other metrics do not.
- In some implementations, the tool measures performance in a series of VR tests, with some tests having one or more “visible” targets and other tests having one or more “hidden” targets. In the “visible” target trials, one or more visual or audible cues assist the user in finding a target. For example, a prominent flag or other graphical cue is placed next to the target to help the user find the target, or directional arrows guide the user to the target. In the hidden target trials, the performance of the user in finding the target(s) is measured without giving the user the cues from the visible target testing. Such tests help measure memory retention of the user in navigating the VR environment.
- Returning to
FIG. 2 , the tool uses (230) the measured performance of the user in the VR environment in analysis of cognitive status. For example, the tool assesses: (a) the presence or extent of age-related cognitive decline (e.g., a decline in memory performance or learning performance), (b) presence or extent of pediatric cognitive disability (e.g., a memory performance problem or learning performance problem), (c) presence or extent of progression of Alzheimer's disease, (d) presence of a characteristic of pre-clinical Alzheimer's disease, and/or (e) response of the user to therapeutic intervention to treat cognitive decline. To make the assessment, the tool can use artificial intelligence mechanisms such as classifiers (e.g., neural networks) for pattern recognition, statistical analysis, etc. Example therapeutic interventions are presented below. Alternatively, the tool uses the measured performance for a different type of analysis. - The cognitive status assessment relates the measured performance to a cognitive status classification. In making the assessment, the tool can compensate for the effects of sex, age and/or learning about the VR environment (e.g., navigation skills, landscape) on the measured performance. The following sections describe observed correlations between sex, age and learning in example uses of the generalized technique (200), and such correlations can be compensated for during the assessment of cognitive status.
- In some implementations, the user repeatedly takes the VR navigation test and the performance of the user over time is measured so as to assess changes in cognitive status of the user. Typically, this involves comparing cognitive status assessments from trial to trial for the user. In other implementations, the results of testing are compared for multiple users, e.g., as part of population studies for the efficacy of a therapy.
-
FIG. 3 shows a generalized technique (300) for using measured performance of a user on a Novel Image Novel Location (“NINL”) test in analysis of cognitive status. A software tool such as one operating in the computer system environment shown inFIG. 1 or other tool performs the technique. Alternatively, the NINL test is administered by a human supervisor using paper materials. Example use scenarios and example clinical applications for the generalized technique (300) are described below. - The tool receives (310) input from the user as the user takes the NINL test. In example implementations, a software tool graphically presents the NINL test to the user on a computer monitor as a series of images in panels or a slideshow. The software tool accepts input from the user via a keyboard, touchpad or mouse, or the software tool receives and process voice input from the user, or the software tool receives and processes another kind of input. In a typical NINL test, the input indicates whether the user perceives “no change” in an image or group of images presented to the user, a “new location” in the image(s), or a “new image” in the image(s). Alternatively, the choices presented to the user and/or selections received from the user have a different format.
- Returning to
FIG. 3 , the tool measures (320) performance of the user in the NINL test based at least in part upon the received input. In some implementations, as described in the following sections, the tool presents a first set of images organized by location to the user. For example, the first set of images includes multiple panels with each panel including multiple images. The tool then presents a second set of images organized by location to the user. For example, the second set of images includes multiple panels with each panel including multiple images. To measure whether the user detects changes in image content and/or location, one or more images of the second set of images differs in image content and/or image location relative to the first set of images. Alternatively, the sets of images for the NINL test have a different configuration. - In terms of metrics, the tool measures one or more of the following: (1) a novel location score indicating performance of the user in identifying changes in locations of images; (2) a novel image score indicating performance of the user in identifying new images in a second set of one or more images relative to a first set of one or more images; and/or (3) a no change score indicating performance of the user in identifying situations with no change in image content or image location for a second set of one or more images relative to a first set of one or more images. Alternatively, the tool measures performance using other and/or additional metrics.
- In some implementations, the tool measures performance in an “immediate” NINL test and also measures performance in a “delayed” NINL test. For example, the immediate NINL test occurs shortly after the user reviews a training set of images for the NINL test, and the delayed NINL test occurs some defined period (e.g., five minutes) after the immediate NINL test. The duration of the defined delay period depends on implementation and is set to measure memory retention performance of the user.
- Returning to
FIG. 3 , the tool uses (330) the measured performance of the user on the test in analysis of cognitive status. For example, the tool assesses: (a) the presence or extent of age-related cognitive decline (e.g., a decline in memory performance or learning performance), (b) presence or extent of pediatric cognitive disability (e.g., a memory performance problem or learning performance problem), (c) presence or extent of progression of Alzheimer's disease, (d) presence of a characteristic of pre-clinical Alzheimer's disease, and/or (e) response of the user to therapeutic intervention to treat cognitive decline. Example therapeutic interventions are presented below. Alternatively, the tool assesses cognitive status for a different type of cognitive assessment. - The cognitive status assessment relates the measured performance to a cognitive status classification. In making the assessment, the tool can compensate for the effects of sex, age and/or learning about the testing on the measured performance. The following sections describe observed correlations between sex, age and learning in example uses of the generalized technique (300), and such correlations can be compensated for during the assessment of cognitive status.
- In some implementations, the user repeatedly takes the NINL test and the performance of the user over time is measured so as to assess changes in cognitive status of the user. Typically, this involves comparing cognitive status assessments from trial to trial for the user. In other implementations, the results of testing are compared for multiple users, e.g., as part of population studies for the efficacy of a therapy.
- The generalized techniques (200, 300) can be used in various scenarios, including but not limited to home use scenarios, professional use scenarios with fMRI equipment, professional use scenarios with MRI equipment, and professional use scenarios with just the testing.
- For example, when used with MRI equipment (or fMRI equipment), the equipment measures neural activity of the user as the user takes the NINL test and/or VR test. A cognitive status assessment for the user can then also be based on the measured neural activity.
- Or, when used in a home use scenario, the user takes the NINL test and/or the VR test on a home computer system such as a desktop or laptop computer. The test can be delivered to the user on a computer-readable medium such as a disk or delivered to the user over a network connection from a server computer system. The user inputs can be received and processed locally to measure performance and assess cognitive status, or information can be forwarded to a remote server computer site to measure performance and/or assess cognitive status.
- Alternatively, the NINL testing and/or VR testing is performed in conjunction with other and/or additional batteries of cognitive tests or physical evaluations of the user.
- Described implementations can be used in a variety of contexts, such as psychological testing or clinical trials involving children and/or the elderly. The NINL object recognition test and the Memory Island spatial navigation test turned out to be sensitive to detect differences in learning and memory performance in these two populations. The sensitivity of these tests is a potential benefit or advantage over other technology. For example, in contrast to established cognitive tests, these tests were shown to be sensitive to effects of APOE ε4 (a risk factor for developing Alzheimer's disease and cognitive impairments following various environmental challenges) in non-demented elderly and children. Other advantages and problems to be solved are presented herein.
- In example implementations, cognitive status assessments are used to make decisions about therapeutic interventions for users (e.g., children or elderly). In general, in this context, a therapeutic intervention is a known or proposed treatment for cognitive decline, such as the cognitive decline caused by normal aging or a pathological process, such as Alzheimer's disease or another condition associated with dementia, such as a neurological disease (for example, Huntington's Disease, Parkinson's disease, Creutzfeldt-Jakob Disease or a brain tumor), a vascular disorder (such as multi-infarct dementia or stroke), an infectious etiology (such as HIV/AIDS, spongiform encephalopathy, or syphilis), a toxic exposure (for example, to lead or alcohol), or an undesired effect of a drug. When the treatment is a proposed treatment, it can be administered as part of a clinical trial, and the response of the subject to the treatment can be assessed by the performance of the subject in the VR environment and/or NINL testing.
- For example, the therapeutic intervention includes a drug therapy, and the cognitive status assessment is used in determining a therapeutic dose of the drug therapy. In the context of treating cognitive decline, for example, the therapeutic intervention is an APOE ε4 inhibitor, an APOE ε3 or APOE ε2 mimetic, a cholinesterase inhibitor, an N-methyl-aspartate receptor antagonist, or a vitamin. Or, the therapeutic intervention includes hormone therapy using testosterone and/or another androgen, or using estrogen.
- This section describes example implementations of NINL tests and VR spatial navigation tests, then details results of performance on the tests in a first series of trials. It includes discussion of specific problems addressed and advantages for the example test implementations in some contexts. Alternatively, implementations of the NINL and VR spatial navigation techniques and tools vary in terms of technical details, specific advantages and/or problems solved.
- A computer-generated VR island environment was developed to mirror the water maze paradigm of spatial learning and memory sensitive to effects of sex on age-related cognitive decline in mouse studies. The participants were trained to navigate to a visible target and subsequently to a hidden target. A joystick was used to control direction and speed of body movement. In addition, potential effects of sex on facial recognition and object recognition were tested using faces and NINL testing, respectively.
- A. Materials and Methods
- 1. Participants
- To determine the effects of sex on cognitive test performance, 27 community college students between 20 and 44 years of age (mean age±S.E.M., 30.3±1.2 years of age; 14 males (mean age±S.E.M., 29.1±1.4 years of age) and 13 females (mean age±S.E.M., 31.5±1.9 years of age)) were tested.
- To determine whether use of a head-mounted display (“HMD”) system (HMD model V8, Virtual Research System Inc., Santa Clara, Calif.) influences performance in a spatial learning and memory test requiring navigation (see below), 24 additional young participants between 17 and 40 years of age (mean age±S.E.M., 30.5±1.2 years of age; 14 males and 10 females) were recruited from the Oregon Health Science University campus community.
- 2. Facial Recognition
- The testing began with two non-computerized memory tests. The first of these was a facial recognition test (Faces I and Faces II), a part of the Wechsler Memory Scale III developed and published by the Psychological Corporation. In this test, the participant was shown a series of 24 faces and asked to remember each one. Immediately after, the participant was shown another series of 48 faces (the 24 original faces plus 24 distracter faces) and asked to indicate whether each face was one of the faces they were directed to remember earlier or not (Faces I score). After an interval of five minutes, the participant was shown a different set of 48 faces (the same 24 original faces plus 24 new distracter faces) and again asked to indicate whether each face was one of the faces they were directed to remember earlier or not (Faces II score). For the Faces I and Faces II scores, the participant received one point for a correct response and zero points for an incorrect response with a maximal total of 48 points.
- 3. Object Recognition
- Following the facial recognition test, an object recognition test entitled Novel Image, Novel Location (“NINL”) test was presented to the study participants. In this test, the participant was presented with a series of 12 panels, one at a time, for eight seconds each. Each panel consisted of four quadrants (A, B, C, and D), with a different image in three of the four quadrants. The images were all similar in complexity but different in content. Positioning of the images within three of the four quadrants varied between panels.
-
FIG. 4 is a diagram showing example panels of the NINL software tool. On the left are panels from the first set. On the right are the corresponding panels from the second set, containing a novel location (A), novel image (B), or no change (C). - For each panel, the participant was asked to remember the images and their positions. After the participant had been presented the first set of 12 panels, they were immediately presented with a second set of 12 panels. After five minutes, they were again presented the second set of 12 panels. In the second set, the panels were either identical to, or slightly different from their counterparts in the first set. The variations in the new panels were either in the positioning of one of the three images (Novel Location) or in that they contained a novel image in the location previously containing one of the three familiar images (Novel Image). Out of the 12 panels in the second set, 4 panels were identical to panels shown in the first set, four panels contained a familiar image in a novel location, and four panels contained a novel image in place of a familiar image. For each of the 12 panels in the second set, the participant was asked to identify the new panel as being either identical to the corresponding panel in the first set (“Yes” answer), or containing a novel image or a novel location of a familiar image (“No” answer), with a maximum score of 12. These answers provided the total score.
- Test performance was also analyzed with four sub-scores. If a panel was identified as containing a novel image, the participant was asked which image on the panel was novel. If a panel was identified as containing a novel location of a familiar image, the participant was asked the novel location of the familiar image on the panel. The “No Change” sub-score (four points max) reflected correct identification of panels identical to those seen in the first set. No points were deducted for incorrect identification of a panel as being identical to one seen in the first set. The “Change” sub-score (eight points max) reflected the ability to identify and characterize the type of anomaly in the panel (novel location or novel image), but not whether the particular image that changed was identified. No points were given when a change was indicated but the type of anomaly (novel location or novel image) was not correctly identified. The final two sub-scores reflected the ability to correctly identify the exact Novel Location (maximal four points) or Novel Image (maximal four points). In preliminary studies involving study participants age-matched to those in the current study, the version of the object recognition test described worked well and did not lead to a ceiling effect in test performance as a result of being too easy.
- 4. Memory Island
- Next, a computer-generated virtual reality world (Memory Island) was used to assess spatial learning and memory. The participants were immersed in a computer-generated three-dimensional environment through a HMD system (HMD model V8, Virtual Research System Inc., Santa Clara, Calif.) comprised of special LCD video goggles and Sennheiser headphones. Inside the visor of the helmet were two video screens, one for each eye, generating a three-dimensional visual experience. Two earphones presented stereo sounds that coincided with the visual images in the visor, further enhancing the immersion experience. A Microsoft Sidewinder joystick determined the direction and speed of movement in the virtual world. As mentioned earlier, to determine whether use of the HMD influences performance in a spatial learning and memory test requiring navigation, an additional cohort of participants was tested with and without the HMD in two subsequent sessions using a counterbalanced design. Each session included four visible target trials, four hidden target trials (target only visible in very close proximity to the target), and a probe trial (no target present). Movement of the participant was tracked and recorded in time-stamped coordinate files, which were used to calculate speed of movement, time to reach the target (latency), and percentage time spent in each quadrant during the visible target session and hidden target session. Percentage time spent in each quadrant is a valuable measure, as it is usually independent of velocity.
- The virtual world simulated an island environment of 347 m×287 m comprised of four quadrants.
FIG. 5 is a diagram showing screen shots of the virtual reality, spatial navigation Memory Island software tool. A flag marks the location of the target during the visible target session (A) while no flag is present during the hidden target session (B). Each quadrant of the island has a different target. The target inquadrant 1 is a fountain (C), in quadrant 2 a piece of moving art (D), in quadrant 3 a seal (E), and in quadrant 4 a seagull (F). - As shown in
FIG. 5 , each quadrant contained a different target item. The participant was first asked to navigate to a target location visibly marked with a flag adjacent to the target (visible target). Targets in all four quadrants were used for visible target training in four consecutive trials. The starting orientation of the participant was varied in each trial, and these variations were kept consistent for all participants. As the starting orientation for a particular trial influenced the difficulty level of that trial, mean performance over the four trials of the visible or hidden target session were used for data analysis rather than performance during individual trials. After training to locate the visible targets, the participant was trained to navigate to a hidden target (here, no flag adjacent to the target, so the participant had to remember where the hidden target was and how to get there). The location of the hidden target was kept constant for each participant. Participants were given four trials with the hidden target. If the participant was unable to locate the target within two minutes, an arrow appeared to guide them to it. Trials in which the target was located within two minutes were defined as successful trials. The percentage of successful trials in the visible and hidden target session was used as an additional performance measure. Following the hidden target trials, the participant received a thirty second probe trial (target removed). - 5. Statistical Analysis
- Statistical differences between groups were determined by ANOVA, with sex as between participant factor, followed by Tukey-Kramer post hoc tests when appropriate. For analyzing probe trial data on Memory Island, the environment was divided into four quadrants and data was analyzed for the percentage of time spent in each quadrant, with the percentage of time spent in each quadrant as a within-participant measure. To assess significance of linear correlations, Pearson correlation calculations with two-tailed p values were used. All these statistics were performed using JMP software (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, N.C.).
- B. Results of the First Series of Trials
- human tests designed to mirror rodent tests of object recognition and spatial navigation were administered to adult cognitively healthy humans. Facial recognition was also assessed. The trial results showed no statistically significant sex difference in facial recognition, consistent with earlier studies. In the object recognition test, the test-retest NINL total scores during the same visit were highly correlated, comparable to the test-retest correlations obtained in the established facial recognition test. No statistically significant effects were identified for sex on object recognition. However, in the spatial navigation test, effects were identified for sex on spatial learning and memory during the session with the hidden, but not visible, target. These tests are useful to compare assessments of object recognition and spatial learning and memory in humans and animal models.
- 1. Facial Recognition Scores
- First, facial recognition was assessed.
FIGS. 6A and 6B are charts showing comparable facial recognition scores in male and female participants, and correlation of the Faces I and Faces II scores, respectively (n=14 males and n=13 females). No statistically significant effect was identified for sex (F=0.5043, p=0.6810,FIG. 6A ) on facial recognition scores. The scores of Faces I and Faces II were highly correlated (r=0.8182, p<0.0001,FIG. 6B ). - 2. Novel Image and Novel Location (NINL)
- Next, participants were tested for object recognition.
FIGS. 7A-7F are charts showing (A) NINL total scores of male and female participants, (B) correlation of NINL I and NINL II, (C) scores indicating ability to detect a change, (D) scores indicating ability to detect a novel image, (E) scores indicating ability to detect a novel location of a familiar image, and (F) correlation of combined NINL total scores and combined facial recognition total scores, respectively (n=14 males and n=13 females). - As with the facial recognition test, no statistically significant effect was identified for sex on NINL total scores (F=0.5805, p=0.6305,
FIG. 7A ). The scores ofNINL trials FIG. 7B ). Interestingly, the combined total scores for facial recognition and NINL total scores were also highly correlated (r=0.5228, p<0.005,FIG. 7F ). - With regard to the sub-scores, male and female participants showed no difference in their ability to detect a change (F=0.3183, p=0.8121,
FIG. 7C ), a novel image (F=0.6360, p=0.5952,FIG. 7D ) or novel location (F=0.4148, p=0.7431,FIG. 7E ). - 3. Spatial Learning and Memory Requiring Navigation (Memory Island)
- Finally, spatial learning and memory requiring navigation were assessed on Memory Island.
FIGS. 8A-8E are charts showing, for males and females tested with a virtual reality, spatial navigation software tool in hidden target and visible target trials, (A) results for latency to reach the target with (+) or without (−) wearing a HMD, (B) velocity, (C) latency to reach the target, (D) percentage time in the target quadrant, (E) percentage of successful trials, respectively.FIG. 8F is a chart showing, for males and females in a probe trial, percentage time in four quadrants. InFIG. 8A , n=14 males and n=10 females for (A). InFIGS. 8B-8F , n=14 males and n=13 females. - The participants were first trained to locate a visible target in four trials (visible target session). Subsequently, they were trained to locate a hidden target in four trials (hidden target session). No statistically significant effect was identified for the use of the HMD to perform this task on time to locate the target (latency) (F=0.92, p=0.5150,
FIG. 8A ), velocity (F=1.24, p=0.5300,FIG. 8B ) or percentage time spent in the target quadrant (F=2.14, p=0.323,FIG. 8D ) during the visible or hidden target session. - In both the visible target session and the hidden target session, the female participants moved slower (lower velocities) (F=15.59, p<0.0002,
FIG. 8B ) than the male participants. Analyzing the visible and hidden target sessions combined by repeated measures, the female participants showed higher latencies (F=19.22, p<0.0001,FIG. 8C ) than the male participants and there was a sex×session interaction (F=7.80, p=0.008). In the hidden target session, but not the visible target session, the females showed higher latencies than the males (FIG. 8C ). As the female participants moved slower than the male participants in both the visible and hidden target session and the magnitude of this sex difference was comparable in the visible and the hidden target session (FIG. 8B ), the sex difference in moving speeds did not account for the sex difference in latencies in the hidden target session (FIG. 8C ). - Percentage time in the target quadrant, which is typically not affected by velocity, was also measured. In the visible target session, female and male participants had no difficulty in locating the target and spent close to 100% of their time searching in the target quadrant (
FIG. 8D ). In contrast, in the hidden target session, female participants spent less time in the target quadrant than male participants (F=12.27, p<0.001). - Additionally, the percentage of “successful” trials for each participant was measured (
FIG. 8E ). A successful trial was defined as a trial in which the target was located within 120 seconds. Female, participants had fewer successful trials than male participants in the hidden target session (F=10.29, p=0.0021), but not the visible target session (F=1.94, p=0.2652). - Following the hidden target session, the participants performed a 30-second probe trial in which there was no target present. The participants were unaware of the absence of the target during the probe trial, and were asked to perform one last trial with the hidden target. Both females and males spent most of their time searching in the target quadrant (
FIG. 8F ). There was a trend towards a sex difference with the males spending more time in the target quadrant than the females, but that did not reach significance (F=3.49, p=0.0715). - 4. Sex and Performance
- Since there were sex differences in spatial navigation measures on Memory Island, these measures were examined for correlation with performance on the other behavioral tests.
FIGS. 9A-9C are charts showing correlation between NINL total scores and latency to reach the target during a visible target session with a virtual reality, spatial navigation software tool, correlation between NINL total scores and latency to reach the target during a hidden target session with a virtual reality, spatial navigation software tool, and correlation between NINL total scores and percentage of time spent in the target quadrant during a probe trial, respectively. - In female, but not male, participants the combined NINL total scores correlated with average time to reach the target during the visible target session of Memory Island (r=−0.6736, p<0.01,
FIG. 9A ), with average time to reach the target during the hidden target session of Memory Island (r=−0.6005, p<0.03,FIG. 9B ), and with percentage of time spent in the target quadrant in the probe trial (r=0.7217, p<0.01,FIG. 9C ). - D. Discussion
- In the object recognition test, the test-retest NINL total scores during the same visit were highly correlated, comparable to the test-retest correlations obtained in the established facial recognition test. In the spatial navigation test, effects were identified for sex on spatial learning and memory during the session with the hidden, but not visible, target. No statistically significant effects were identified for sex on object recognition.
- There was a sex difference in the percentage of time in the target quadrant during the hidden target, but not visible target, session. This measure is independent of velocity and not biased by start location, as all participants started out in the center of the island. Therefore, these data tend to show a sex difference in ability to locate the hidden target per se, rather than in general ability to perform this task regardless of whether the target was visible or hidden.
- The identified sex differences in spatial learning and memory on Memory Island are consistent with sex differences in visual spatial perception and in spatial learning and memory in real and other virtual environment navigation tasks. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (“fMRI”) during navigational tasks has shown that women recruit the right parietal and right prefrontal area, whereas men recruit the left hippocampal area, which may relate to the predominant use of landmark cues by women and geometric and landmark cues by men. However, it might be more complex. The Memory Island test environment contains landmarks predicting the target location and still showed sex differences in performance. These data tend to show that the sex differences in spatial memory do not require the exclusion of stable landmarks.
- The Memory Island test can be distinguished from prior studies that have adapted the water maze test to study spatial learning and memory in humans using VR. Programs designed to mirror the water maze test in rodent studies might lack elements found in real world situations. Compared to the prior studies, Memory Island involves a higher degree of immersion into the virtual environment. For example, Memory Island also contains environmental sounds (e.g., birds). While not a water maze environment, the design and analysis of the water maze test was incorporated into the design of Memory Island. Importantly, none of the participants experienced nausea or dizziness on Memory Island, while 10% of the participants experienced these symptoms after exposure to a virtual environment of interconnected hallways and other virtual environments in some prior studies.
- In contrast to Memory Island, in NINL testing, no statistically significant effects were identified for sex on object recognition.
- These tests are useful in comparing assessments of object recognition and spatial learning and memory in humans and animal models.
- After describing example implementations of NINL tests and VR spatial navigation tests, this section details results of performance on the tests in a second series of trials. It includes discussion of specific problems addressed and advantages for the example test implementations in some contexts. Alternatively, implementations of the NINL and VR spatial navigation techniques and tools vary in terms of technical details, specific advantages and/or problems solved.
- In the second series of trials using example implementations of NINL tests and VR tests, to determine effects of APOE ε4 (ε4) on cognitive performance of healthy elderly, 115 non-demented elders (mean age 81 years) were cognitive tested. The established tests Faces, Family Pictures, Spatial Span Forward and Backward, as well as the object recognition and spatial navigation tests described herein, were used as cognitive tests. Salivary samples were collected to determine APOE genotype and salivary testosterone and cortisol levels.
- Non-ε4- and ε4-carrying men and women did not differ in age, or Mini-Mental State Examination, Wide Range Achievement Test-Reading, Beck Anxiety Inventory, or reaction time scores. In the second series of trials, an effect was identified for ε4 on the object recognition and spatial navigation tests, however, with non-ε4 carriers outperforming ε4 carriers, but not in the other cognitive tests. No relationship was found for sex and ε4 status or sex and performance during the hidden target session of Memory Island. In men, salivary cortisol levels correlated with object recognition. These results show that object recognition and spatial navigation tests are useful to assess cognitive function in the elderly.
- A. Procedures
- 1. Study Participants
- To determine the effects of sex and ε4 on cognitive performance in the non-demented elderly, people ranging in age from 62 to 92 (mean age±S.E.M., 81.60±0.57 years) were tested. The inclusion criteria were: 1) age 55 and over; and 2) stable medical conditions. Exclusion criteria were vision or hearing deficits severe enough to interfere with cognitive testing. Participants were given a Mini-Mental State Examination (“MMSE”), a short questionnaire that tests different areas of cognitive function, with a maximum score of 30. (See Kurlowicz et al., “The Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE),” Try This: Best Practices in Nursing Care to Older Adults, Hartford Institute for Geriatric Nursing, no. 3 (January 1999).)
- All participants had MMSE scores equal or greater than 22 (see below).
- The final sample was composed of 115 participants, all whites. The sample was divided into two APOE genotype groups, ε4 carriers and non-ε4 carriers. Those in the non-ε4 carriers group represented ε3/ε3 homozygotes and ε2/ε3 heterozygotes. Those in the ε4 carriers group represented ε4/ε4 homozygotes, ε2/ε4, and ε3/ε4 heterozygotes (Table 1).
-
TABLE 1 APOE genotype distribution of study participants. Genotype Women Men ε2/ε3 13 (15.1%) 2 (6.9%) ε2/ε4 1 (1.2%) 0 (0.0%) ε3/ε3 52 (59.3%) 22 (75.9%) ε3/ε4 18 (20.9) 5 (17.2%) ε4/ε4 2 (2.3%) 0 (0.0%) Values are presented as N (%) of women and men for each genotype. - The group of women consisted of ε6 individuals (mean age±S.E.M., 81.2±0.7 years of age), among them 65 non-ε4 carriers and 21 ε4 carriers. The group of men consisted of 29 individuals (mean age±S.E.M., 82.9±0.9 years of age), among them 24 non-ε4 carriers and five ε4 carriers. There was no significant sex difference in the proportion of ε4 carriers among men and women. When cognitive status of the participants was assessed using the MMSE, 111 participants had a MMSE score greater than 23 which corresponds to a cutoff score for cognitively healthy people. The four participants who obtained a MMSE score below 24 (three scored 23, one scored 22) performed well on the other cognitive tests. As MMSE scores can be affected by other conditions such as hearing impairment, the data were analyzed with and without these four individuals included. Both analyses revealed a similar pattern of results. Therefore, these four participants were not excluded from the study.
- Premorbid intellectual functioning general intelligence levels were evaluated using the Wide Range Achievement Test-Reading (“WRAT-R”) instead of years of formal education. As anxiety levels and reaction times can influence performance on cognitive tests, they were analyzed as well. Levels of anxiety were assessed using the Beck Anxiety Inventory (“BAI”). Reaction times were measured by presenting (on a computer screen) a series of colored ellipses at varying time intervals and asking the participants to press a button as soon as the ellipse appeared (Gary Darby, “Reaction Times,” http://www.delphiforfun.org/Programs/Reaction_times.htm (©2000-2007)). The amount of time between the appearance of the stimulus and the time the button was pressed was recorded. No statistically significant differences were identified for age; cognitive status, pre-morbid intellectual functioning, anxiety levels or reaction times between men and women or non-ε4 and ε4-carrying study participants, respectively. (See Table 2.) The person testing the study participants was blinded to APOE genotype.
-
TABLE 2 Demography of study participants. Sex ε4 Status Measure Women Men Non-ε4 ε4 Subjects 86 (74.8%) 29 (25.2%) 89 (77.4%) 26 (22.6%) [N (%)] Mean age 81.2 ± 0.7 82.9 ± 0.9 81.9 ± 6.4 80.5 ± 1.2 (years) WRAT-R 57.6 ± 1.0a,b 57.5 ± 1.6c 58.2 ± 1.0a,c 55.3 ± 1.8b MMSE 27.3 ± 0.2 27.0 ± 0.3 27.3 ± 0.2 27.1 ± 0.4 BAI 4.2 ± 0.4a,b 3.5 ± 0.8c 4.2 ± 0.4a,c 3.6 ± 0.7b Reaction 0.37 ± 0.01 0.40 ± 0.02 0.38 ± 0.01 0.39 ± 0.02 timesd Values are presented as N (%) or adjusted mean ± S.E.M., as indicated. aTwo participants dropped out of the study between the two testing sessions. bThree participants dropped out of the study between the two testing sessions. cOne score missing. dTwo outliers removed from dataset. - 2. Study Design
- For APOE genotyping, samples of saliva were collected at the beginning of an evaluation session for a user. (See Table 3.)
-
TABLE 3 Sequence of cognitive testing. Test stagea Description 1 Collection of a saliva sample b2 Facial Recognition Immediate [Faces I] 5 min delay Face Recognition Delayed [Faces II] 3 NINL I 5 min delay NINL II 4 Reaction times 5 Memory Island: 3 Visible trials (three different targets: Seagull Art Piece Seal) 3 Hidden trials (Seagull Target) Probe trial 6 MMSE 7 FP I 5 min delay FP II 8 OraGene DNA Testc aThree months following this session, additional tests such as BAI, the WAIS SSF and SSB, and the WRAT-R were conducted. These tests were not included in the first visit to minimize the length of the visit and potential fatigue. bSaliva samples were used to determine salivary cortisol and testosterone levels. cOraGene tests were used to determine APOE genotype. - The neuropsychological tests were administered in a designated apartment of the retirement community in two visits lasting around two hours and one hour, respectively. Table 3 illustrates the sequence of neuropsychological testing in both visits and the salivary collection. To control for circadian variations in hormone levels, all examinees were tested in the morning starting at 8:30 a.m.
- 3. Cognitive Tests
- a. Facial Recognition
- For Faces I (immediate) and Faces II (delayed) tests (as in Section VI) to reduce the overall testing time and potential problems with fatigue, an interval of five minutes instead of 25 minutes was used between test and re-test for the facial recognition. A five-minute delay was also used for family pictures and object recognition tests. Performance on facial recognition was analyzed using Faces I and Faces II scores.
- b. Family Pictures
- Family Pictures I (FP I, immediate) and Family Pictures II (FP II, delayed) are also part of the Wechsler Memory Scale III developed and published by the Psychological Corporation. In the FP tests, the participants were asked to memorize as many details as they could from four different cartoon-like family scenes. After the four scenes were displayed for 10 seconds each, the examinees were prompted to describe which characters were in each scene, where they were positioned in the scene, and what they were doing. After an interval of five minutes, the study participants were asked the same question as in FP I. Each correct answer was scored as one point. Performance was analyzed using FP I and FP II scores.
- c. Object Recognition
- The example implementation of Novel Image Novel Location (“NINL”) test described in Section VI was used in the second series of trials. Briefly, this example NINL test consists of two sets of three-image panels (12 panels in each set). The three pictures on each panel are similar in complexity but different in content and how they are randomly located in three of the four quadrants of the panel (A, B, C, and D). (See
FIG. 1 .) The first set of 12 panels is the reference set (here, the set participants are asked to memorize). The second set is the test set; panels are either identical to, or slightly different from, their counterparts in the first set. The variations in the second set of panels are either in the positioning of one of the three images or in that they contain a novel image instead of one of the three familiar images. Out of the 12 panels in the second set, four panels are identical to panels shown in the first set, four panels contain a familiar image in a novel location, and four panels contain a novel image in the original position of a familiar image. - The participants were first read the instructions and shown an example of what was expected from them in this test. Then they were presented with the first set of 12 panels (reference set), one at a time, for eight seconds each and asked to memorize the images and their positions. Without delay, they were presented with the second set of 12 panels and were prompted to identify each panel as being either identical to the corresponding panel in the first set (“No Change score”), or containing a novel image (“Novel Image score”) or a novel location of a familiar image (“Novel Location score”). Their answers provided the total NINL immediate score, with a maximum of 12 points (“NINL I”). After five minutes (and without seeing the reference set again), participants were presented with the second set and asked the same questions. These answers provided the total NINL delayed score, with a maximum of 12 points (“NINL II”). Test performance was analyzed using NINL I, NINL II and three sub-scores for each. The Novel Location and Novel Image sub-scores reflected the ability to correctly identify the exact Novel Location and Novel Image, respectively (maximal four points each). The No Change sub-score reflected correct identification of panels identical to those seen in the first set (maximal four points).
- d. Spatial Span Forward (SSF) and Spatial Span Backward (SSB)
- SSF and SSB are also part of the Wechsler Memory Scale III developed and published by the Psychological Corporation. They provide a nonverbal measure of immediate memory. Both tests involve a board containing ten randomly anchored cubes. The participants were asked to watch the investigator tap the cubes (1 cube/second) in a prearranged order and to reproduce these tapping sequences. In the SSF test the tapping is performed in order of the example (forward), while in the SSB test it is performed in reverse order compared with the example (backward). The difficulty increases with the number of cubes tapped; from two cubes up to nine cubes. A correct sequence is scored as one point. Testing ended when the participants failed to reproduce the sequence correctly after two trials or when the pair of nine cubes sequence was tapped correctly.
- e. Memory Island
- Memory Island, a computer-generated virtual reality (“VR”) world described in Section VI, was used in the second series of trials to assess spatial learning and memory requiring navigation. Briefly, the participants were immersed through a high quality 19-in. Dell computer monitor and a Harmon Kardon HK395 stereo speaker system with subwoofer. A Microsoft Sidewinder joystick was used to determine the direction and speed of movement in the virtual world. The virtual world simulated an island environment of 347×287 m2 composed of four quadrants, each containing a different target item.
- The participants were first asked to navigate to a target location visibly marked with a flag adjacent to the target (visible target session). Targets in all four quadrants were used for visible target training in three trials. The starting orientation of the participant was varied in each trial, and these variations were kept consistent for all participants, as the starting orientation for a particular trial influenced the difficulty level of that trial. After being trained to locate the visible targets, the participants were trained to navigate to a hidden target (here, no flag adjacent to the target) in three trials. In this part of the test, the participants had to remember where the hidden target was and how to get there (hidden target session). The location of the hidden target was kept constant for all participants (Seagull target). In each trial of the visible or hidden target session, if the participant was unable to locate the target within two minutes, a directional arrow appeared to guide them to the target. Following the hidden target trials, the participant performed a 30 second probe trial (target removed). In each trial, movement of the participants was tracked and recorded in time-stamped coordinate files, which were used to calculate total distance moved (feet), velocity (feet per second), latency (seconds), cumulative distance to the target (feet) and percentage time spent in the target quadrant until it was located or up to 120 seconds, whichever came first. For the probe trial, the percentage time spent in each quadrant was analyzed. Trials in which the target was located within two minutes were defined as successful trials. The percentage of successful trials in the visible and hidden target session was used as an additional measure of performance.
- 4. Salivary Cortisol and Testosterone
- For assessment of salivary cortisol and testosterone levels and to assess potential correlations of these levels with measures of cognitive performance, samples of saliva were collected at the beginning of the session. (See Table 3.) Salivary cortisol and testosterone levels were determined using commercial kits by the General Clinical Research Center at Oregon Health Science University.
- 5. Statistical Analysis
- As ε2 might provide protection against age-related cognitive decline, possible effects of ε2 on cognitive performance of the study participants were assessed. As no such effects were found, the study participants were divided in ε4 carriers and non-ε4 carriers. The lack of an effect of ε2 on cognitive performance of the study participants might have been caused by the relatively low number of ε2 carriers in this sample. Because performance on immediate and delayed parts of the cognitive tests from the same subjects is likely to be correlated, a mixed effects model (repeated measures design) was used to evaluate the change of cognitive performance test for recall parts (Family Pictures and Faces). This model was also adopted for analyzing performance across trials in Memory Island. Based on the Bayesian information criterion, a compound symmetrical structure as the variance-covariance matrix was selected.
- In addition, generalized estimating equation (“GEE”) regression for the object recognition outcomes, such as Novel Image, Novel Location, No Change, and Total NINL scores was used to obtain estimates clustered by study participant, and odds ratios were estimated for significant comparisons. The method of GEE is often used to analyze longitudinal and other correlated response data.
- When tests did not contain a delayed recall part, statistical differences between ε4 and non-ε4-carrying groups as well as between men and women were determined using analyses of covariance (“ANCOVA”) with, in both cases, age as a covariate. Regarding the probe trial, Mann-Whitney U tests were conducted to assess effects of sex and ε4 on time spent in the target quadrant. A Friedman test and when appropriate follow-up pairwise comparisons using Wilcoxon signed rank-tests were used to assess effects of sex and ε4 on the percentage of time spent in each quadrant of Memory Island. To assess significance of linear correlations, Pearson correlation calculations with two-tailed p-values were used. To limit the risk of violation of a normal distribution, outliers were removed from testosterone, cortisol and reaction times. Adjustment for multiple testing was handled by using repeated-measures analyses. Age was used as a covariate in all analyses.
- Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS software (SPSS version 14.0: SPSS Inc., Chicago, Ill., USA) and Statistical Analysis System version 9.1 (SAS Institute, Cary, N.C., USA).
- B. Results of Second Series of Trials
- 1. Facial Recognition
- Performance was slightly better on Faces II than Faces I (F=6.91, p=0.010). The estimated mean for the Faces II was 35.70, while the estimated mean for the Faces I was 33.95 with a standard error of 0.53.
- 2. Family Pictures
-
FIG. 10A is a chart showing, for elderly women and men, an effect of sex on “Family Pictures” test scores. There was strong evidence that women (estimated mean±S.E.M., 34.58±1.30) performed better than men (estimated mean±S.E.M., 23.84±2.09) in the Family Pictures test (F=22.52, p<0.001). - 3. Object Recognition
-
FIG. 10B is a chart showing, for elderly women and men, an effect of ε4 on NINL total scores (combined immediate and delayed scores). An effect was identified for ε4 on NINL total scores (combined immediate and delayed scores) (χ2=4.23, p=0.040). At any given age, non-ε4 carriers had a higher estimated NINL total score than 4 carriers. Mean NINL scores for non-ε4 and ε4 carriers were 9.01±0.25 and 8.10±0.39, respectively. There was a trial by ε4 interaction (χ2=4.93, p=0.026,FIG. 7B ); comparing the immediate and delayed object recognition test total scores, ε4 carriers showed a larger decline in performance than non-ε4 carriers. With regard to the sub-scores, effects were identified for sex and ε4 on the novel location sub-scores (combined immediate and delayed scores).FIGS. 10C and 10D are charts showing, for elderly women and men, effect of ε4 on Novel Location sub-scores (combined immediate and delayed scores), and effect of sex on Novel Location sub-scores, respectively. There was suggestive evidence of non-ε4 carriers performing better than ε4 carriers (χ2=4.01, p=0.045,FIG. 10C ) and of women performing better than men (χ2=4.82, p=0.028,FIG. 10D ). For the no change sub-scores, interactive trial by ε4 effects were found (χ2=7.64, p=0.006). In contrast to the novel location sub-scores, the analyses of the novel image sub-scores yielded that performance was lower in the delayed score compared with the immediate recall (χ2=5.30, p=0.021). - 4. Spatial Span
- No statistically significant effects were identified for sex (F=1.21, p=0.273) or ε4 (F=0.79, p=0.375) on SSF test performance. SSB test performance showed no effects of ε4 (F=0.71, p=0.791), or effect of sex (F=1.60, p=0.209).
- 5. Memory Island
- Spatial learning and memory requiring navigation were assessed using a virtual reality, spatial navigation (Memory Island) test.
FIGS. 11A and 11B are charts showing, for elderly women and men, effect of sex on velocity during visible target trials, and effect of sex on velocity during hidden target trials, respectively.FIGS. 12A-12F are charts showing, for elderly women and men, (A) effect of ε4 on velocity during a visible target session, (B) effect of ε4 on velocity during a hidden target session, (C) effect of ε4 on cumulative distance during a visible target session, (D) effect of ε4 on cumulative distance during a hidden target session, (E) effect of ε4 on latency to reach target during a visible target session, and (F) effect of ε4 on latency to reach target during a hidden target session (P<0.05), respectively. - During the visible target trials, an effect was identified for sex (F=8.15, p=0.005,
FIG. 11A ) on velocity. Therefore, average velocity during the visible target session was used as an additional covariate in all repeated-measures analyses of covariance (except for percentage time spent in the target quadrant as it is typically not related to speed of movement). Velocity also increased significantly across the visible trials (F=3.58, p=0.036). Learning curves during the visible trials (as shown inFIGS. 12A , 12B, 12C and 12D) demonstrate that the participants understood and could navigate in the three dimensional virtual environment. As shown inFIG. 12A , overall velocity increased across trials during the visible target session (P<0.05). During the hidden target session, effects of ε4 were found on cumulative distance to the target (F=5.14, p=0.026,FIG. 12D ) and on latency (F=6.17, p=0.015,FIG. 12F ). For example, ε4 carriers showed larger cumulative distance to the target at 120 seconds compared with non-ε4 carriers (P<0.05). In contrast, no statistically significant effects were identified for ε4 on cumulative distance (FIG. 12C ) or latency to the target during the visible target session (FIG. 12E ). -
FIGS. 13A and 13B are charts showing, for elderly women and men tested with a virtual reality, spatial navigation software tool in a probe trial (target removed), effect of sex on percentage of time spent in four quadrants, and effect of ε4 on percentage of time spent in four quadrants, respectively. Women spent more time in the right quadrant (P<0.01) compared with the target quadrant and more time in the target quadrant compared with the left (P<0.05) and to the opposite quadrant (P<0.01) while men spent more time in the target quadrant compared with the left quadrant (P<0.05). Non-ε4 carriers spent more time in the target compared with the left quadrant (P<0.001) and the opposite quadrant (P<0.05) while ε4 carriers spent more time in the right quadrant compared with the target quadrant (P<0.01). - In particular, women spent more time in the right quadrant than the target quadrant (z=−2.68, p=0.007), and more time in the target quadrant than the left (z=−2.92, p=0.004) and opposite quadrant (z=−2.07, p=0.039), while men spent only more time in the target quadrant than the left quadrant (z=−2.45, p=0.014) (
FIG. 13A ). Non-ε4 carriers spent more time in the target quadrant than the left (z=−3.59, P<0.001) and the opposite quadrant (z=−2.54, p=0.011) while ε4 carriers spent more time in the right quadrant than the target quadrant (z=−2.74, p=0.006,FIG. 13B ). Comparatively, non-ε4 carriers spent more time in the target quadrant (31%) than ε4 carriers (15%) (z=−2.03, p=0.042,FIG. 13B ). - 6. Salivary Cortisol and Testosterone Levels and Correlations with Cognitive Performance
-
FIG. 14 is a chart showing, for elderly women and men, effect of ε4 on salivary testosterone levels. Effects were identified for sex (F=3.944, p=0.05 and F=4.245, p=0.04 after controlling for age,FIG. 14 ) and ε4 (F=4.520, p=0.04 and F 4.036, p=0.06 after controlling for age,FIG. 14 ) on salivary testosterone levels. Women carrying ε4 had lower testosterone levels compared with non-carriers while men carrying ε4 had higher testosterone levels compared with non-carriers. Age was included as a covariate in the analysis. - Based on this result and to avoid spurious conclusions, potential correlations of salivary testosterone levels with cognitive measures in the complete sample were not considered. Only in men, salivary cortisol levels correlated with some cognitive measures. (See Table 4.)
-
TABLE 4 Correlations of salivary cortisol levels with cognitive measures. Cortisol Level Measure Women Men Non ε4 ε4 MMSEa −0.05 −0.32 −0.05 −0.22 BAI 0.05 0.12 0.03 0.39 WRAT-R −0.08 0.03 −0.03 −0.14 Reaction 0.16 −0.33 0.05 −0.10 timesb Spatial SSF 0.00 0.27 0.03 0.03 spanc SSB −0.03 −0.11 −0.11 0.05 Facesd Faces immediate 0.04 −0.22 0.03 −0.17 Faces delayed −0.11 −0.23 −0.04 −0.49$ Family Family Pictures −0.08 −0.07 0.05 −0.23 Pictures immediate Family Pictures −0.03 −0.15 0.04 −0.15 delayed NINLa No change immediate −0.08 −0.06 −0.09 0.02 No change delayed −0.09 −0.02 −0.08 −0.07 Novel image −0.05 −0.45* −0.12 −0.14 immediate Novel image delayed −0.01 −0.37+ −0.07 −0.05 Novel Location −0.02 −0.28 0.06 −0.38 immediate Novel Location 0.02 −0.12 0.08 −0.14 delayed Total NINL I −0.07 −0.004* −0.08 −0.31 Total NINL II −0.03 −0.33 −0.04 −0.14 Memory Average velocity −0.05 0.10 −0.03 −0.16 Island Total distance −0.21 0.36 −0.03 −0.30 visible % Successful trials 0.11 0.05 0.07 0.20 target % In target quadrant 0.20 −0.09 0.14 0.04 sessione Cumulative distance −0.08 0.01 −0.06 0.03 Memory Average velocity −0.09 0.05 −0.13 0.00 Island Total distance 0.09 0.21 0.09 0.22 hidden % Successful trials −0.18 −0.24 −0.19 −0.41 target % In target quadrant −0.21 −0.11 −0.19 −0.30 sessionf Cumulative distance 0.13 0.32 0.17 0.43 aN = 109; missing four cortisol samples, two outliers removed. bN = 107; two outliers removed. cN = 104; five participants dropped out of the study between the two testing times, one missing score. dN = 108; missing one score. eN = 103; six participants did not complete the visible target session. fN = 80; 29 did not complete the hidden target session. *Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (two-tailed). $While p < 0.05, closer examination showed it was driven by a single datapoint (P = 0.187 without). +p = 0.051. -
FIGS. 15A and 15B are charts showing, for elderly men, correlation of salivary cortisol levels with NINL I novel image recognition, and correlation of salivary cortisol levels with NINL II novel image recognition, respectively. In the object recognition tests, Novel Image sub-scores correlated with salivary cortisol levels; immediate scores (r=−0.45, p=0.05,FIG. 12A ), and delayed scores (r=−0.37, p=0.05,FIG. 15B ). Total NINL scores correlated also with salivary cortisol levels (r=−0.44, p=0.05). No such correlations were found in women. -
TABLE 5 Effects of ε4 and sex on cognitive measures (repeated-measures analyses)a Within Interaction ε4 Sex subjectb ε4 × Seq Sex × Seq Measure N F P F P F P F P F P Facesc 114 0.01 0.943 1.00 0.320 6.91 0.010 0.15 0.696 0.90 0.344 Family 115 2.50 0.117 22.57 <.0001 0.12 0.733 1.73 0.191 3.64 0.059 Pictures Visible 109 0.31 0.861 8.15 0.005 3.58 0.036 1.82 0.170 2.11 0.131 Velocity Visible 109 0.77 0.781 3.24 0.075 0.50 0.535 0.11 0.818 0.61 0.486 Total Distance Visible 109 3.26 0.074 0.65 0.421 0.33 0.639 2.14 0.138 0.50 0.537 Latency Visible % 109 1.07 0.303 3.73 0.056 2.18 0.117 0.93 0.392 0.69 0.496 Time spent in Target Visible % 109 2.06 0.154 0.05 0.821 1.03 0.353 1.14 0.317 0.01 0.987 Successful trials Visible 109 0.18 0.670 0.55 0.462 0.34 0.695 2.00 0.142 0.69 0.493 Cumulative Distance Hidden 84 2.37 0.128 3.40 0.069 2.89 0.059 0.10 0.905 1.91 0.151 Velocity Hidden 84 1.10 0.297 1.00 0.321 0.02 0.983 0.84 0.433 1.12 0.331 Total Distance Hidden 84 6.17 0.015 0.69 0.410 1.01 0.366 0.83 0.438 2.40 0.094 Latency Hidden % 84 3.48 0.066 0.76 0.385 1.33 0.268 1.25 0.288 1.18 0.311 Time spent in Target Hidden % 84 2.06 0.154 0.05 0.821 1.03 0.353 1.14 0.317 0.01 0.987 Successful trials Hidden 84 5.14 0.026 0.62 0.433 0.73 0.482 0.21 0.809 2.60 0.078 Cumulative Distance aThere were 19 models in Table 5. Since there were 19 separate models for different outcomes (response variables), a multiple test procedure for controlling the family-wise error rate was not applied when testing general hypotheses defined in terms of sub-models. Age was used as a covariate in all analyses. bRepeated measures analyses were used to assess potential within-subjects effects. cFaces represented a combined measure of “Faces I and Faces II.” -
TABLE 6 Effects of ε4 and sex on cognitive measures (GEE regression analysis)a Within Interaction ε4 Sex subjectb ε4 × Seqc Sex × Seq Measure N X2 P X2 P X2 P X2 P X2 P NINL 115 4.23 0.040 2.51 0.113 3.48 0.062 4.93 0.026 0.09 0.767 total Novel 115 4.01 0.045 4.82 0.028 0.09 0.763 1.82 0.178 0.52 0.472 location Novel 115 1.66 0.198 0.25 0.615 5.30 0.021 0.65 0.419 0.70 0.403 image No 115 0.00 0.968 0.13 0.723 0.00 0.980 7.64 0.006 1.44 0.230 change aAdjustment for multiple testing was handled using repeated-measures analyses. Age was used as a covariate in all analyses. bRepeated-measures analyses were used to assess potential within-subject effects. cSequential, used for the assessments of potential interactions of ε4 or sex with the repeated measure of performance. - C. Discussion
- The established cognitive tests Faces, Family Pictures, SSF and SSB were administered along with the object recognition and spatial navigation tests to non-demented elderly. Women performed better than men in the family pictures test (
FIG. 10A ). This sex difference in performance is consistent with the better performance of women than men in recognizing pictures containing natural categories. Of the cognitive tests used, only the object recognition and spatial navigation tests were sensitive to effects of ε4 on cognitive performance. The lack of ε4 effects in the established cognitive tests is consistent with other studies showing no effects of ε4 on cognitive performance in the non-demented elderly. Deficits in episodic memory, memory for specific experiences that can be defined in terms of time and space, are often the first symptoms experienced by patients with AD. Therefore, poor performance on episodic memory tests such as the object recognition and spatial navigation tests can indicate pre-clinical phases of cognitive impairment. - In the object recognition test, an effect was identified for ε4 (
FIG. 10C ) on the novel location score (combined immediate and delayed score) with ε4 carriers performing worse on novel location, but not on novel object, scores than non-ε4 carriers. These data indicate that it is more difficult for ε4 carriers to accurately recall location. Together with the poorer performance of ε4 than non-ε4 carriers on the spatial navigation test, these data support the proposition that ε4 carriers are particularly susceptible to spatial memory impairments. - In the object recognition test, there was also an effect of sex (
FIG. 10D ) on the novel location score (combined immediate and delayed score) with women performing better than men, and there were no effects of sex on spatial navigation in the Memory Island test. Using the same object recognition test, for cognitive testing of healthy adult humans, no sex differences in performance were seen. In the first series of trials, using the same spatial navigation test used in the current study, adult men performed better than adult women. - In the probe trial, elderly women and men did not spend most of their time in the target quadrant. (See
FIG. 13A .) In contrast, younger adult women and men (20-44 years of age) did spend most of their time in the target quadrant in the probe trial. (Section VI.) These data are consistent with the effect of age on spatial memory retention in young (25-45 years of age), middle-aged (45-65 years of age), and old (65-93 years of age) humans shown in other studies, with the older study participants showing lower measures of spatial memory retention in the probe trial than the younger study participants. In the probe trial, ε4 carriers spent more time in the right quadrant than the target quadrant (FIG. 13B ). This was not seen in non-ε4 carriers. This is an important finding in that, based on the starting orientation in the probe trial, entering the right quadrant indicates that the study participants navigated straight rather than effectively searching for the target location. - For salivary testosterone levels, there was a sex×ε4 interaction, with higher salivary testosterone levels in ε4-carrying than non-ε4-carrying men and lower salivary testosterone levels in ε4-carrying than non-ε4-carrying women. (See
FIG. 14 .) In men, the difference between immediate and delayed novel image recognition scores correlated with salivary testosterone levels (r=0.473, p=0.015) and at each performance level difference between immediate and delayed recognition performance, ε4-carrying men had the highest salivary testosterone levels. Similarly, during the hidden target session of Memory Island, salivary testosterone levels in male ε4 carriers correlated with latency (r=−0.92, p=0.03), cumulative distance to the target location (r=0.87, p=0.06), percentage of successful trials (r=0.85, p=0.07), and percentage of time spent in the target quadrant (r=0.89, p=0.04). - The correlations of salivary cortisol levels with cognitive measures were sensitive to sex. Only in men, salivary cortisol levels correlated with immediate total NINL scores, and immediate Novel Image sub-scores. (See Table 6.) The association of higher cortisol levels with poorer cognitive performance in the elderly is consistent with other studies. However, the second series of trials underlines the importance of considering ε4 and sex in assessing potential correlations of cortisol with cognitive measures.
- There was no significant sex difference in the proportion of ε4 carriers. The lack of such a difference might have been due to the sex difference in sample size with more women than men participating in the study, which in turn might relate to sex differences in longevity.
- D. Conclusion
- The object recognition and spatial navigation tests were sensitive to effects of ε4 in the elderly. Differences in neuroanatomy, brain glucose metabolism during mental activity, and brain activation in memory tasks between non-demented non-ε4 and ε4 carriers might contribute to these effects. As the mean age of the participants of this study was 82, these data indicate that while the ε4-associated risk to develop AD is age-dependent and maximal before this age, effects of ε4 on cognitive performance can be revealed in the old-old using episodic memory tests.
- This section describes example implementations as well as discussion of specific problems addressed and advantages for those implementations in some contexts. Alternatively, implementations of the preceding techniques and tools vary in terms of technical details, specific advantages and/or problems solved.
- Compared to APOE ε3, APOE ε4 is a risk factor for age-related cognitive decline and cognitive impairments following environmental challenges. To assess whether APOE ε4 has effects on cognitive performance in children, they were given standardized cognitive tests as well as an object recognition test and a spatial navigation test sensitive to effects of APOE ε4 in the elderly. Children with APOE ε4 showed reduced novel location recognition, reduced ability to navigate to a visible target, and reduced spatial memory retention. The early effect of APOE ε4 on cognition indicates predisposition to cognitive impairments later in life.
- Of the three major human APOE isoforms, which play roles in cholesterol metabolism and are encoded by distinct alleles (ε2, ε3, and ε4), ε4 increases the risk of age-related cognitive decline and cognitive injury following environmental insults. To assess potential effects of ε4 on cognition in children, 55 healthy 7-10 year-olds (girls: 17 non-ε4 and eight-ε4; boys: 24 non-ε4 and six-ε4) were given established cognitive tests, as well as an object recognition test and a spatial navigation test requiring navigation sensitive to effects of ε4 in non-demented elderly, and provided saliva samples for APOE genotyping and cortisol levels.
- The inclusion criteria were healthy boys and girls 7-10 years of age. The exclusion criteria were children whose birth mother or legal guardian could not be interviewed, children with severe visual impairments, children born more than 35 weeks premature, children with epilepsy, head injury, Tourette's syndrome, cerebral palsy, congenital abnormalities, severe brain trauma, diagnosed with leukemia or any other medical condition that could interfere with cognitive ability, or children exposed to elicit drugs during pregnancy. Children were from middle/upper class families and 84% Caucasian, 7% Hispanic, 3.5% African American, and 3.5% Pacific Islander. During pregnancy the average age of the mother's was 29 years old with 12.0% reporting smoking and 17.2% reporting at least some alcohol consumption during pregnancy.
- Average cortisol levels did not correlate with any cognitive measure, but did correlate with anxiety scores (Pearson Correlation r=0.293, p<0.05). (See Table 7, below.) Thus, self-reported anxiety levels were likely not a confounding factor for assessing cognitive measures. Each child was given cognitive tests during a 1.5 hr session. (See Table 7, below.)
-
TABLE 7 Cognitive measures in non-ε4 and ε4 carrying 7-10 year-old boys and girls. ANCOVA or REM Immediate Delay (Geno)1 Task Non-ε4 ε4 carrier Non-ε4 ε4 carrier F p Dot 10.4 + 0.5 11.6 + 0.9 1.37 0.25 Locationa - Learning Dot Location - 11.01 + 0.5 12.4 + 0.9 1.95 0.17 Total Score Dot Location - 11.6 + 0.3 11.8 + 0.6 0.05 0.81 Long Delay Dot Location - 11.3 + 0.4 12.0 + 0.7 0.67 0.41 Short Delay CPTb Non- 56.6 + 3.1 55.1 + 5.4 0.05 0.81 clinical score CPT % 49.6 + 2.2 51.8 + 3.8 0.24 0.62 Omissions CPT % 49.6 + 1.9 45.9 + 3.3 0.98 0.32 Commissions CPT Hit Rate 47.6 + 1.8 52.0 + 3.1 1.47 0.23 MASCc - 45.5 + 2.3 47.4 + 3.9 0.17 0.67 Total Score WASId 56.9 + 1.8 57.3 + 3.1 0.01 0.91 Vocabulary WASI Block 56.2 + 1.6 60.5 + 2.8 1.64 0.20 Design Family 11.8 + 0.4 11.9 + 0.5 10.0 + 0.8 10.2 + 0.9 3.39 0.07 Pictures NINL - 27.4 + 1.0 26.0 + 1.0 25.5 + 1.8 23.7 + 1.8 1.19 0.28 Total Score NINL - 5.1 + 0.4 4.2 + 0.5 5.2 + 0.7 4.8 + 0.8 0.10 0.74 Novel Image NINL - 6.5 + 0.4 6.1 + 0.4 5.3 + 0.6 4.3 + 0.7 4.77e 0.03e Novel Location Average 0.07 + 0.01 0.08 + 0.02 0.80 0.38 Cortisol levels (ug/dL) aChildrens' Memory Scale, Dot Location test. bConners' Continuous Performance Test (“CPT”) is an attention test used in ADHD research and clinical assessments. cMulti-dimensional Anxiety Scale for Children. dWechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. eParticularly significant effects - As attention (CPT, overall non-clinical score) correlated with performance in most cognitive tests (p<0.05), it was included as a covariate in all analyses. Age was used as a covariate if age correction tables were not available. The data were analyzed using ANCOVAs or repeated measures (REM) with trial as the with-in subject variable and genotype as the fixed factor. As no statistically significant effects were identified for sex on any cognitive measure, boys and girls were combined for the analysis. ε4 did not affect general cognitive ability in children (Table 7), consistent with earlier reports. However, an effect was identified for ε4 on novel location recognition (p<0.04) with lower delayed novel location recognition scores in ε4 carriers (p<0.03). As such an effect of ε4 was also seen in non-demented healthy elderly, novel location recognition can be used to detect effects of ε4 throughout life.
- Spatial learning and memory requiring navigation were assessed using the virtual reality Memory Island test.
FIGS. 16A , 16B and 16C show effects of ε4 on Memory Island performance in 7-10 year-old boys and girls.FIG. 16A shows effects of sex on velocity during the visible and hidden target trials (*p<0.04).FIG. 16B shows effects of ε4 on cumulative distance to the target in the visible (*p<0.005), but not hidden, target trials.FIG. 16C shows percentage of time spend in quadrants for ε4 and non-ε4 children (*p<0.04 for the target quadrant versus all other quadrants). Compared to the left and right quadrants, the children tended to spend more time in the opposite quadrant. This might be due to the fact that the start orientation in the probe trial faces the opposite quadrant. - The velocity of boys was higher than girls during the trials with the visible (F=4.75; p<0.04) and hidden (REM ANCOVAs F=7.90; p<0.007) target. (See
FIG. 16A .) Therefore, velocity was used as covariate for the analyses of performance on Memory Island. Non-ε4 carriers outperformed ε4 carriers during the trials with the visible (F=4.53; p<0.04), but not hidden (F=0.11; p=0.74), target by navigating closer to the visible target location. (SeeFIG. 16B .) - Fifteen minutes following the last hidden trial, non-ε4 carriers showed spatial memory retention in the probe trial (no target present) and searched most of the time in the quadrant previously containing the target (p<0.04) but ε4 carriers did not. (See
FIG. 16C .) Similarly, while 75.6% of non-ε4 carriers showed target preference, only 43% of ε4 carriers did (p<0.04, Fisher's exact test). - Thus, effects of ε4 on spatial learning and memory are already detected in 7-10 year-old children and indicate predisposition to cognitive injury following environmental challenges and/or age-related cognitive decline.
- In this example, the cognitive status of an elderly subject suspected to have dementia (e.g., an age-associated dementia, such as Alzheimer's disease) is assessed. The subject is given the Memory Island and NINL tests described herein. The subject's performance on the Memory Island test (e.g., distance traversed, time elapsed before reaching target, percentage of successful trials, time spent in target area, velocity of movement) and NINL tests (e.g., Novel Location score, Novel Image score, No Change score, total NINL score) is compared with previously measured performance benchmarks for non-demented individuals of the same sex and similar age.
- In this example, the cognitive status of an elderly subject suspected to have age-associated dementia is assessed over time. The subject is initially given Memory Island and NINL tests described herein. The subject's performance on the Memory Island and NINL tests is compared with previously measured performance benchmarks for non-demented individuals of the same sex and similar age. One year later, the subject is given a second round of Memory Island and NINL tests. The subject's performance on the Memory Island and NINL tests is compared with the subject's own performance the previous year.
- In this example, the cognitive status of a child subject suspected to have a genetic predisposition toward dementia later in life (for example an age-associated dementia, such as Alzheimer's disease) is assessed. The child subject is given the Memory Island and NINL tests described herein. The child subject's performance on the Memory Island and NINL tests is compared with previously measured performance benchmarks for non-demented individuals of the same sex and similar age.
- In this example, a subject who has been diagnosed with dementia (for example an age-associated dementia, such as Alzheimer's disease) is given the Memory Island and NINL tests described herein. Baseline measures are determined (for Memory Island: distance traversed, time elapsed before reaching target, percentage of successful trials, time spent in target area, velocity of movement; for NINL: Novel Location score, Novel Image score, No Change score, total NINL score), and the subject is given an anti-Alzheimer's drug (such as Donepezil (ARICEPT) or anti-APOE ε4 drug) as a therapeutic intervention.
- For a subject given Donepezil, the subject is treated with 5 mg per day of the drug for a period of at least four to six weeks, and then the subject is once again given the Memory Island and NINL tests. If a desired improvement in scores is not obtained, then the dose of the drug is increased to 10 mg per day. Testing with the Memory Island and NINL tests is then repeated to assess the response to the drug, where a change in test scores indicates a response to the drug.
- In this example, a subject who has been diagnosed with dementia (for example an age-associated dementia, such as Alzheimer's disease) is given the Memory Island and NINL tests described herein. Baseline measures are determined as described in Example 4, and the subject is given an anti-Alzheimer's drug (such as Donepezil (ARICEPT) or anti-APOE ε4 drug) as a therapeutic intervention.
- For a subject given Donepezil, the subject is treated with 5 mg per day of the drug for a period of at least four to six weeks, and then the subject is once again given the Memory Island and NINL tests. If a desired improvement in scores is not obtained, the subject is given a different drug. Testing with the Memory Island and NINL tests is then repeated to assess the response to the different drug, where a change in test scores indicates a response to the different drug.
- Having described and illustrated the principles of my invention with reference to various described embodiments, it will be recognized that the described embodiments can be modified in arrangement and detail without departing from such principles. It should be understood that the programs, processes, or methods described herein are not related or limited to any particular type of computing or clinical environment, unless indicated otherwise. Various types of general purpose or specialized computing environments may be used with or perform operations in accordance with the teachings described herein. Elements of the described embodiments shown in software may be implemented in hardware and vice versa.
- In view of the many possible embodiments to which the principles of my invention may be applied, I claim as my invention all such embodiments as may come within the scope and spirit of the following claims and equivalents thereto.
Claims (36)
1. A method comprising:
receiving input from a user as the user takes a test in which the user identifies novel images, if any, among plural images presented to the user and identifies novel locations, if any, among the plural images presented to the user;
measuring performance of the user in the test based at least in part upon the received input; and
using the measured performance of the user on the test in analysis of cognitive status.
2. The method of claim 1 , wherein the test includes
presenting a first set of images organized by location to the user; and
presenting a second set of images organized by location to the user, wherein one or more images of the second set of images differs in image content and/or image location relative to the first set of images.
3. The method of claim 1 , wherein the measured performance includes a novel location score indicating performance of the user in identifying changes in locations of images.
4. The method of claim 1 , wherein the measured performance includes a novel image score indicating performance of the user in identifying new images in a second set of one or more images relative to a first set of one or more images.
5. The method of claim 1 , wherein the measured performance includes a no change score indicating performance of the user in identifying situations with no change in image content or image location for a second set of one or more images relative to a first set of one or more images.
6. The method of claim 1 , wherein the measured performance includes:
immediate performance results indicating performance of the user in a first round of the test, the first round occurring a first delay period after reviewing a training set of one or more images for the test; and
delayed performance results indicating performance of the user in a second round of the test, the second round occurring a second delay period after the first round of the test.
7. The method of claim 6 wherein the first delay period is less than one minute and the second delay period is approximately five minutes.
8. The method of claim 6 wherein the second delay period is set to measure memory retention performance of the user.
9. The method of claim 1 , further comprising:
measuring neural activity of the user with a magnetic resonance imaging tool as the user takes the test, wherein the analysis of cognitive status is also based at least in part on the measured neural activity.
10. The method of claim 1 , further comprising:
measuring neural activity of the user with a functional magnetic resonance imaging tool as the user takes the test, wherein the analysis of cognitive status is based at least in part on the measured neural activity.
11. The method of claim 1 , wherein software implements the test, coordinates presentation of the test to the user, and coordinates receipt of the input from the user.
12. The method of claim 1 , wherein a personal computer system presents the test to the user.
13. The method of claim 12 , wherein a server computer system provides the test to the personal computer system over a network connection.
14. The method of claim 1 , wherein the input includes keyboard input, touchpad input, or voice input.
15. The method of claim 1 , wherein the input is an indication of no change, change in image or change in location between a second set of one or more images and a first set of or more images.
16. The method of claim 1 , wherein the analysis of cognitive status is used to detect presence or extent of age-related cognitive decline.
17. The method of claim 16 , wherein the age-related cognitive decline is a decline in memory performance or learning performance.
18. The method of claim 1 , wherein the analysis of cognitive status is used to detect presence or extent of pediatric cognitive disability.
19. The method of claim 1 , wherein the pediatric cognitive disability is a memory performance problem or learning performance problem.
20. The method of claim 1 , wherein the analysis of cognitive status is used to detect presence or extent of progression of Alzheimer's disease.
21. The method of claim 1 , wherein the analysis of cognitive status is used to detect presence of a characteristic of pre-clinical Alzheimer's disease.
22. The method of claim 1 , wherein the analysis of cognitive status is used to measure responsiveness of the user to treatment of Alzheimer's disease.
23. The method of claim 1 , further comprising:
setting or adjusting a treatment regimen for the user based at least in part on the analysis of cognitive status.
24. The method of claim 1 further comprising:
comparing the analysis of cognitive status to one or more prior analyses of cognitive status to assess change in cognitive status of the user.
25. The method of claim 1 , wherein the analysis comprises adjusting for effects of sex and/or age on the measured performance.
26. The method of claim 1 , wherein the analysis comprises adjusting for effects of learning the test on the measured performance.
27. The method of claim 1 , wherein analysis of cognitive status further comprises analysis of a response of the user to a therapeutic intervention to treat cognitive decline.
28. The method of claim 27 , wherein the therapeutic intervention comprises a drug therapy.
29. The method of claim 28 , wherein the analysis of a response of the user further comprises determining a therapeutic dose of the drug therapy.
30. The method of claim 27 , wherein the therapeutic intervention is a proposed intervention that is being studied for efficacy.
31. The method of claim 27 , wherein the therapeutic intervention is an APOE ε3 or APOE ε2 mimetic, a cholinesterase inhibitor, an N-methyl-aspartate receptor antagonist, hormone therapy, or a vitamin.
32. The method of claim 1 wherein the analysis is performed by a software tool that includes pattern recognition or other artificial intelligence aspects.
33. The method of claim 1 wherein the analysis and the measuring performance are performed by an integrated software tool.
34. A method comprising:
receiving input from a user as the user takes a test in which the user identifies novel images, if any, among plural images presented to the user and identifies novel locations, if any, among the plural images presented to the user;
measuring performance of the user in the test based at least in part upon the received input; and
using the measured performance of the user on the test in analysis of cognitive status;
wherein the measured performance includes one or more of a novel location score indicating performance of the user in identifying changes in locations of images, a novel image score indicating performance of the user in identifying new images in a second set of one or more images relative to a first set of one or more images, and a no change score indicating performance of the user in identifying situations with no change in image content or image location for a second set of one or more images relative to a first set of one or more images.
35. A method comprising:
receiving input from a user known to have a first genotype as the user takes a test in which the user identifies novel images, if any, among plural images presented to the user and identifies novel locations, if any, among the plural images presented to the user;
measuring performance of the user in the test based at least in part upon the received input; and
comparing the measured performance of the user on the test to measured performances for a user group for the first genotype.
36. The method of claim 35 wherein the first genotype is an APOE genotype.
Priority Applications (2)
Application Number | Priority Date | Filing Date | Title |
---|---|---|---|
US11/888,091 US20090298025A1 (en) | 2007-05-09 | 2007-07-30 | Object recognition testing tools and techniques for measuring cognitive ability and cognitive impairment |
US13/434,297 US8475171B2 (en) | 2007-05-09 | 2012-03-29 | Object recognition testing tools and techniques for measuring cognitive ability and cognitive impairment |
Applications Claiming Priority (2)
Application Number | Priority Date | Filing Date | Title |
---|---|---|---|
US92857707P | 2007-05-09 | 2007-05-09 | |
US11/888,091 US20090298025A1 (en) | 2007-05-09 | 2007-07-30 | Object recognition testing tools and techniques for measuring cognitive ability and cognitive impairment |
Related Child Applications (1)
Application Number | Title | Priority Date | Filing Date |
---|---|---|---|
US13/434,297 Division US8475171B2 (en) | 2007-05-09 | 2012-03-29 | Object recognition testing tools and techniques for measuring cognitive ability and cognitive impairment |
Publications (1)
Publication Number | Publication Date |
---|---|
US20090298025A1 true US20090298025A1 (en) | 2009-12-03 |
Family
ID=39969879
Family Applications (3)
Application Number | Title | Priority Date | Filing Date |
---|---|---|---|
US11/888,109 Abandoned US20080280276A1 (en) | 2007-05-09 | 2007-07-30 | Virtual reality tools and techniques for measuring cognitive ability and cognitive impairment |
US11/888,091 Abandoned US20090298025A1 (en) | 2007-05-09 | 2007-07-30 | Object recognition testing tools and techniques for measuring cognitive ability and cognitive impairment |
US13/434,297 Active - Reinstated US8475171B2 (en) | 2007-05-09 | 2012-03-29 | Object recognition testing tools and techniques for measuring cognitive ability and cognitive impairment |
Family Applications Before (1)
Application Number | Title | Priority Date | Filing Date |
---|---|---|---|
US11/888,109 Abandoned US20080280276A1 (en) | 2007-05-09 | 2007-07-30 | Virtual reality tools and techniques for measuring cognitive ability and cognitive impairment |
Family Applications After (1)
Application Number | Title | Priority Date | Filing Date |
---|---|---|---|
US13/434,297 Active - Reinstated US8475171B2 (en) | 2007-05-09 | 2012-03-29 | Object recognition testing tools and techniques for measuring cognitive ability and cognitive impairment |
Country Status (1)
Country | Link |
---|---|
US (3) | US20080280276A1 (en) |
Cited By (16)
Publication number | Priority date | Publication date | Assignee | Title |
---|---|---|---|---|
US20130345524A1 (en) * | 2012-06-22 | 2013-12-26 | Integrated Deficit Examinations, LLC | Device and methods for mobile monitoring and assessment of clinical function through sensors and interactive patient responses |
WO2015120481A1 (en) * | 2014-02-10 | 2015-08-13 | Medical Care Corporation | Assessing cognition using item-recall trials with accounting for item position |
US9629543B2 (en) | 2009-03-17 | 2017-04-25 | Emory University | Internet-based cognitive diagnostics using visual paired comparison task |
WO2017105907A1 (en) * | 2015-12-14 | 2017-06-22 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Systems and methods for acquiring and employing resiliency data for leadership development |
US9805339B2 (en) | 2011-07-05 | 2017-10-31 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Method for monitoring and improving health and productivity of employees using a computer mouse system |
US9808156B2 (en) | 2011-07-05 | 2017-11-07 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Systems, computer medium and computer-implemented methods for monitoring and improving biomechanical health of employees |
US9844344B2 (en) | 2011-07-05 | 2017-12-19 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Systems and method to monitor health of employee when positioned in association with a workstation |
US9889311B2 (en) | 2015-12-04 | 2018-02-13 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Systems, protective casings for smartphones, and associated methods to enhance use of an automated external defibrillator (AED) device |
US9949640B2 (en) | 2011-07-05 | 2018-04-24 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | System for monitoring employee health |
US10058285B2 (en) | 2011-07-05 | 2018-08-28 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Chair pad system and associated, computer medium and computer-implemented methods for monitoring and improving health and productivity of employees |
US10108783B2 (en) | 2011-07-05 | 2018-10-23 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Systems, computer medium and computer-implemented methods for monitoring health of employees using mobile devices |
US10307104B2 (en) | 2011-07-05 | 2019-06-04 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Chair pad system and associated, computer medium and computer-implemented methods for monitoring and improving health and productivity of employees |
US10475351B2 (en) | 2015-12-04 | 2019-11-12 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Systems, computer medium and methods for management training systems |
US10642955B2 (en) | 2015-12-04 | 2020-05-05 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Devices, methods, and computer medium to provide real time 3D visualization bio-feedback |
US10824132B2 (en) | 2017-12-07 | 2020-11-03 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Intelligent personal protective equipment |
EP4216225A1 (en) * | 2022-01-20 | 2023-07-26 | Koninklijke Philips N.V. | Cognitive impairment determination apparatus |
Families Citing this family (57)
Publication number | Priority date | Publication date | Assignee | Title |
---|---|---|---|---|
US9895085B2 (en) | 2007-03-21 | 2018-02-20 | Massachusetts Institute Of Technology | Measuring representational motions in a medical context |
US8339418B1 (en) * | 2007-06-25 | 2012-12-25 | Pacific Arts Corporation | Embedding a real time video into a virtual environment |
US9566029B2 (en) * | 2008-09-30 | 2017-02-14 | Cognisens Inc. | Method and device for assessing, training and improving perceptual-cognitive abilities of individuals |
US20130191153A1 (en) * | 2010-07-09 | 2013-07-25 | Michael D. Lee | Assessing Variation In Clinical Response Data Based On A Computational Representation Of Neural Or Psychological Processes Underlying Performance On A Brain Function Test |
US20120029302A1 (en) * | 2010-07-29 | 2012-02-02 | Shih-Chung Hsu | Apparatus and method for dynamic detection of electrophysiological signals for stillness feedback |
TW201214351A (en) * | 2010-09-23 | 2012-04-01 | Joanne Liang | Interactive learning method by using drawing way |
ES2397031B1 (en) * | 2011-05-10 | 2014-01-27 | Universidade Da Coruña | VIRTUAL REALITY SYSTEM FOR THE EVALUATION AND TREATMENT OF MOTOR DISORDERS ASSOCIATED WITH NEURODEGENERATIVE DISEASES AND AGE. |
US9220453B2 (en) | 2011-10-20 | 2015-12-29 | Cogcubed Corporation | Apparatus for mounting a wireless sensor on a human for diagnosing and treating cognitive disorders |
US9014614B2 (en) | 2011-10-20 | 2015-04-21 | Cogcubed Corporation | Cognitive assessment and treatment platform utilizing a distributed tangible-graphical user interface device |
US9324241B2 (en) | 2011-10-20 | 2016-04-26 | Cogcubed Corporation | Predictive executive functioning models using interactive tangible-graphical interface devices |
CA2857069C (en) * | 2011-10-24 | 2022-03-01 | President And Fellows Of Harvard College | Enhancing diagnosis of disorder through artificial intelligence and mobile health technologies without compromising accuracy |
ES2853550T3 (en) * | 2011-11-16 | 2021-09-16 | Bohbot Veronique Deborah | Computer generated three-dimensional virtual reality environment to improve memory |
WO2013120075A1 (en) * | 2012-02-09 | 2013-08-15 | Anthrotronix, Inc. | Performance assessment tool |
CN104246852B (en) * | 2012-02-22 | 2018-11-16 | 乔塞林.福伯特 | Consciousness-cognition-motor learning system and method |
PL2844146T3 (en) * | 2012-04-24 | 2016-12-30 | System and method of measuring attention | |
GB201300055D0 (en) * | 2013-01-03 | 2013-02-20 | Howard Foundation Holdings Ltd | Identifying subjects in need of treatment |
US9216133B2 (en) * | 2013-01-16 | 2015-12-22 | Elwha Llc | Using a 3D display to train a weak eye |
US20140249377A1 (en) * | 2013-03-01 | 2014-09-04 | Eugen Tarnow | Well defined system and method for monitoring neurological disease onset and progress and onset of the effects of neuropharmacological drugs using the recency-primacy shift and overall free recall differences |
US20150170537A1 (en) * | 2013-12-17 | 2015-06-18 | Selwyn Super | System and method for assessing visual and neuro-cognitive processing |
US20150196242A1 (en) * | 2014-01-13 | 2015-07-16 | Anthrotronix, Inc. | Performance Assessment Tool |
WO2015172017A1 (en) | 2014-05-08 | 2015-11-12 | Medical Care Corporation | Systems and methods for assessing human cognition, including a quantitative approach to assessing executive function |
WO2016061322A1 (en) * | 2014-10-16 | 2016-04-21 | Anthrotronix, Inc. | Testing stylus |
US20160125748A1 (en) * | 2014-11-04 | 2016-05-05 | John Wesson Ashford | Memory test for Alzheimer's disease |
US20180070872A1 (en) * | 2015-04-27 | 2018-03-15 | The Regents Of The University Of California | Neurotherapeutic video game for improving spatiotemporal cognition |
CN105852831A (en) * | 2016-05-10 | 2016-08-17 | 华南理工大学 | Equipment based on virtual reality interaction technology and brain function real-time monitoring technology |
KR102369850B1 (en) * | 2016-08-03 | 2022-03-03 | 아킬리 인터랙티브 랩스 인크. | Cognitive platform including computerized associative elements |
WO2018039610A1 (en) | 2016-08-26 | 2018-03-01 | Akili Interactive Labs, Inc. | Cognitive platform coupled with a physiological component |
US10311645B1 (en) | 2016-10-14 | 2019-06-04 | Floreo, Inc. | Methods and systems for treating autism |
WO2018081134A1 (en) | 2016-10-24 | 2018-05-03 | Akili Interactive Labs, Inc. | Cognitive platform configured as a biomarker or other type of marker |
WO2018112103A1 (en) * | 2016-12-13 | 2018-06-21 | Akili Interactive Labs, Inc. | Platform for identification of biomarkers using navigation tasks and treatments using navigation tasks |
CN106821333A (en) * | 2017-03-21 | 2017-06-13 | 黑龙江尔惠科技有限公司 | A kind of cognition dysfunction rehabilitation detection means based on virtual scene, method and therapeutic equipment |
KR101983279B1 (en) * | 2017-03-23 | 2019-05-30 | 사회복지법인 삼성생명공익재단 | Nerve disprder diagnosis apparatus and method using virtual reality |
US10453172B2 (en) | 2017-04-04 | 2019-10-22 | International Business Machines Corporation | Sparse-data generative model for pseudo-puppet memory recast |
CN107186722A (en) * | 2017-04-26 | 2017-09-22 | 天津理工大学 | A kind of robot of prevention senile dementia based on mental exercise |
WO2018222729A1 (en) * | 2017-05-30 | 2018-12-06 | Akili Interactive Labs, Inc. | Platform for identification of biomarkers using navigation tasks and treatments based thereon |
CN110415789A (en) * | 2018-04-27 | 2019-11-05 | 深圳市前海安测信息技术有限公司 | For assisting the wearable device of Alzheimer Disease patient memory training |
CN110448269A (en) * | 2018-05-08 | 2019-11-15 | 深圳市前海安测信息技术有限公司 | AD patient's Auto-Test System and method based on Congnitive scale |
CA3100519A1 (en) * | 2018-05-17 | 2019-11-21 | Rutgers, The State University Of New Jersey | Systems and methods for cognitive diagnostics in connection with major depressive disorder and response to antidepressants |
CN110534189A (en) * | 2018-05-25 | 2019-12-03 | 深圳市前海安测信息技术有限公司 | Alzheimer illness classified estimation model creation method and computer installation |
JP2021531606A (en) * | 2018-07-27 | 2021-11-18 | マファビット,インク. | Systems and methods for treating memory disorders |
EP3621276A1 (en) * | 2018-09-05 | 2020-03-11 | Altoida Ltd. | Apparatus, method and program for determining a cognitive state of a user of a mobile device |
US20200170510A1 (en) * | 2018-10-29 | 2020-06-04 | Apptek, Inc. | Device for sensor based medication delivery based on motor, sensory, cognitive and physiological fluctuations |
US20200337627A1 (en) * | 2019-04-25 | 2020-10-29 | Massachusetts Institute Of Technology | Cognitive health assessment system |
CN110313895A (en) * | 2019-04-28 | 2019-10-11 | 江南大学 | Training of cognitive function method |
JP6784429B1 (en) * | 2019-06-20 | 2020-11-11 | Mig株式会社 | Information processing equipment, information processing methods and information processing programs |
WO2021038109A1 (en) | 2019-08-30 | 2021-03-04 | Metralabs Gmbh Neue Technologien Und Systeme | System for capturing sequences of movements and/or vital parameters of a person |
CN110916690A (en) * | 2019-12-02 | 2020-03-27 | 中国科学院心理研究所 | Method for predicting early symptoms of Alzheimer's disease |
JP6764140B6 (en) * | 2019-12-27 | 2020-10-28 | Mig株式会社 | Information processing equipment, information processing methods and information processing programs |
JP6764141B6 (en) * | 2019-12-27 | 2020-10-28 | Mig株式会社 | Information processing equipment, information processing methods and information processing programs |
JP6764139B6 (en) * | 2019-12-27 | 2020-10-28 | Mig株式会社 | Information processing equipment, information processing methods and information processing programs |
JP6764142B6 (en) * | 2019-12-27 | 2020-10-28 | Mig株式会社 | Information processing equipment, information processing methods and information processing programs |
CN110970130B (en) * | 2019-12-30 | 2023-06-27 | 佛山创视嘉科技有限公司 | Data processing device for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder |
US20210241648A1 (en) * | 2020-01-30 | 2021-08-05 | Workplace Technologies Research, Inc. | Systems and methods to provide mental distress therapy through subject interaction with an interactive space |
US20210248917A1 (en) | 2020-02-07 | 2021-08-12 | University-Industry Cooperation Group Of Kyung Hee University | Method and system of measuring spatial ability required for architecture or interior design |
JP6813919B1 (en) * | 2020-03-25 | 2021-01-13 | Mig株式会社 | Information processing equipment, information processing methods and information processing programs |
JP6820038B1 (en) * | 2020-03-26 | 2021-01-27 | Mig株式会社 | Information processing equipment, information processing methods and information processing programs |
US20240096476A1 (en) * | 2022-09-15 | 2024-03-21 | Taipei Medical University | Method, the computing deivce, and the non-transitory computer-readable recording medium for providing cognitive training |
Citations (31)
Publication number | Priority date | Publication date | Assignee | Title |
---|---|---|---|---|
US6186145B1 (en) * | 1994-05-23 | 2001-02-13 | Health Hero Network, Inc. | Method for diagnosis and treatment of psychological and emotional conditions using a microprocessor-based virtual reality simulator |
US6364845B1 (en) * | 1998-09-17 | 2002-04-02 | University Of Rochester | Methods for diagnosing visuospatial disorientation or assessing visuospatial orientation capacity |
US6425764B1 (en) * | 1997-06-09 | 2002-07-30 | Ralph J. Lamson | Virtual reality immersion therapy for treating psychological, psychiatric, medical, educational and self-help problems |
US20020103429A1 (en) * | 2001-01-30 | 2002-08-01 | Decharms R. Christopher | Methods for physiological monitoring, training, exercise and regulation |
US20020103428A1 (en) * | 2001-01-30 | 2002-08-01 | Decharms R. Christopher | Methods for physiological monitoring, training, exercise and regulation |
US20020116352A1 (en) * | 2000-11-02 | 2002-08-22 | Regents Of The University Of California San Francisco | Computer-implemented methods and apparatus for alleviating abnormal behaviors |
US20020120208A1 (en) * | 2001-02-23 | 2002-08-29 | Sun-Il Kim | System and method of providing service for enhancing attention on internet |
US20020128540A1 (en) * | 2001-02-23 | 2002-09-12 | Sun-Il Kim | System and method of correlating virtual reality with biofeedback for enhancing attention |
US6503085B1 (en) * | 1998-01-29 | 2003-01-07 | James Elkind | Use of virtual reality and desk top computer formats to diagnose executive dysfunctions |
US6632174B1 (en) * | 2000-07-06 | 2003-10-14 | Cognifit Ltd (Naiot) | Method and apparatus for testing and training cognitive ability |
US6688746B2 (en) * | 1999-12-03 | 2004-02-10 | Iouri Malov | Field testing using spread spectrum technique |
US20040092809A1 (en) * | 2002-07-26 | 2004-05-13 | Neurion Inc. | Methods for measurement and analysis of brain activity |
US20040208923A1 (en) * | 2001-03-29 | 2004-10-21 | Michael Davis | Acute pharmacologic augmentation of psyschotherapy with enhancers of learning or conditioning |
US20050019734A1 (en) * | 2001-11-20 | 2005-01-27 | Avi Peled | System and method for diagnosis of mental disorders |
US20050033154A1 (en) * | 2003-06-03 | 2005-02-10 | Decharms Richard Christopher | Methods for measurement of magnetic resonance signal perturbations |
US20050096396A1 (en) * | 2002-03-28 | 2005-05-05 | Emory University | Acute pharmacologic augmentation of psychotherapy with enhancers of learning or conditioning |
US20050175972A1 (en) * | 2004-01-13 | 2005-08-11 | Neuroscience Solutions Corporation | Method for enhancing memory and cognition in aging adults |
US20050197560A1 (en) * | 2004-03-05 | 2005-09-08 | Rao Stephen M. | System for detecting symptoms, determining staging and gauging drug efficacy in cases of Alzheimer's disease |
US20050216243A1 (en) * | 2004-03-02 | 2005-09-29 | Simon Graham | Computer-simulated virtual reality environments for evaluation of neurobehavioral performance |
US20050228785A1 (en) * | 2004-04-02 | 2005-10-13 | Eastman Kodak Company | Method of diagnosing and managing memory impairment using images |
US20050283053A1 (en) * | 2002-01-30 | 2005-12-22 | Decharms Richard C | Methods for physiological monitoring, training, exercise and regulation |
US20060074340A1 (en) * | 2004-09-24 | 2006-04-06 | Tsutomu Murata | Cognitive capacity measurement device and cognitive capacity measurement method |
US20060161218A1 (en) * | 2003-11-26 | 2006-07-20 | Wicab, Inc. | Systems and methods for treating traumatic brain injury |
US7122004B1 (en) * | 1999-08-13 | 2006-10-17 | Interactive Metronome, Inc. | Method and apparatus of enhancing learning capacity |
US20060241718A1 (en) * | 2003-11-26 | 2006-10-26 | Wicab, Inc. | Systems and methods for altering brain and body functions and for treating conditions and diseases of the same |
US20060293617A1 (en) * | 2004-02-05 | 2006-12-28 | Reability Inc. | Methods and apparatuses for rehabilitation and training |
US20070027406A1 (en) * | 2004-02-13 | 2007-02-01 | Georgia Tech Research Corporation | Display enhanced testing for concussions and mild traumatic brain injury |
US20070088404A1 (en) * | 2005-10-19 | 2007-04-19 | Allen Wyler | Methods and systems for improving neural functioning, including cognitive functioning and neglect disorders |
US20070166675A1 (en) * | 2005-12-15 | 2007-07-19 | Posit Science Corporation | Cognitive training using visual stimuli |
US7333963B2 (en) * | 2004-10-07 | 2008-02-19 | Bernard Widrow | Cognitive memory and auto-associative neural network based search engine for computer and network located images and photographs |
US7645140B2 (en) * | 2002-11-05 | 2010-01-12 | University Of Rochester Medical Center | Method for assessing navigational capacity |
Family Cites Families (6)
Publication number | Priority date | Publication date | Assignee | Title |
---|---|---|---|---|
US5911581A (en) * | 1995-02-21 | 1999-06-15 | Braintainment Resources, Inc. | Interactive computer program for measuring and analyzing mental ability |
US7207804B2 (en) * | 1996-03-27 | 2007-04-24 | Michael Hersh | Application of multi-media technology to computer administered vocational personnel assessment |
US6565359B2 (en) * | 1999-01-29 | 2003-05-20 | Scientific Learning Corporation | Remote computer-implemented methods for cognitive and perceptual testing |
US6306086B1 (en) * | 1999-08-06 | 2001-10-23 | Albert Einstein College Of Medicine Of Yeshiva University | Memory tests using item-specific weighted memory measurements and uses thereof |
US6663392B2 (en) * | 2001-04-24 | 2003-12-16 | The Psychological Corporation | Sequential reasoning testing system and method |
US8083675B2 (en) * | 2005-12-08 | 2011-12-27 | Dakim, Inc. | Method and system for providing adaptive rule based cognitive stimulation to a user |
-
2007
- 2007-07-30 US US11/888,109 patent/US20080280276A1/en not_active Abandoned
- 2007-07-30 US US11/888,091 patent/US20090298025A1/en not_active Abandoned
-
2012
- 2012-03-29 US US13/434,297 patent/US8475171B2/en active Active - Reinstated
Patent Citations (31)
Publication number | Priority date | Publication date | Assignee | Title |
---|---|---|---|---|
US6186145B1 (en) * | 1994-05-23 | 2001-02-13 | Health Hero Network, Inc. | Method for diagnosis and treatment of psychological and emotional conditions using a microprocessor-based virtual reality simulator |
US6425764B1 (en) * | 1997-06-09 | 2002-07-30 | Ralph J. Lamson | Virtual reality immersion therapy for treating psychological, psychiatric, medical, educational and self-help problems |
US6503085B1 (en) * | 1998-01-29 | 2003-01-07 | James Elkind | Use of virtual reality and desk top computer formats to diagnose executive dysfunctions |
US6364845B1 (en) * | 1998-09-17 | 2002-04-02 | University Of Rochester | Methods for diagnosing visuospatial disorientation or assessing visuospatial orientation capacity |
US7122004B1 (en) * | 1999-08-13 | 2006-10-17 | Interactive Metronome, Inc. | Method and apparatus of enhancing learning capacity |
US6688746B2 (en) * | 1999-12-03 | 2004-02-10 | Iouri Malov | Field testing using spread spectrum technique |
US6632174B1 (en) * | 2000-07-06 | 2003-10-14 | Cognifit Ltd (Naiot) | Method and apparatus for testing and training cognitive ability |
US20020116352A1 (en) * | 2000-11-02 | 2002-08-22 | Regents Of The University Of California San Francisco | Computer-implemented methods and apparatus for alleviating abnormal behaviors |
US20020103428A1 (en) * | 2001-01-30 | 2002-08-01 | Decharms R. Christopher | Methods for physiological monitoring, training, exercise and regulation |
US20020103429A1 (en) * | 2001-01-30 | 2002-08-01 | Decharms R. Christopher | Methods for physiological monitoring, training, exercise and regulation |
US20020128540A1 (en) * | 2001-02-23 | 2002-09-12 | Sun-Il Kim | System and method of correlating virtual reality with biofeedback for enhancing attention |
US20020120208A1 (en) * | 2001-02-23 | 2002-08-29 | Sun-Il Kim | System and method of providing service for enhancing attention on internet |
US20040208923A1 (en) * | 2001-03-29 | 2004-10-21 | Michael Davis | Acute pharmacologic augmentation of psyschotherapy with enhancers of learning or conditioning |
US20050019734A1 (en) * | 2001-11-20 | 2005-01-27 | Avi Peled | System and method for diagnosis of mental disorders |
US20050283053A1 (en) * | 2002-01-30 | 2005-12-22 | Decharms Richard C | Methods for physiological monitoring, training, exercise and regulation |
US20050096396A1 (en) * | 2002-03-28 | 2005-05-05 | Emory University | Acute pharmacologic augmentation of psychotherapy with enhancers of learning or conditioning |
US20040092809A1 (en) * | 2002-07-26 | 2004-05-13 | Neurion Inc. | Methods for measurement and analysis of brain activity |
US7645140B2 (en) * | 2002-11-05 | 2010-01-12 | University Of Rochester Medical Center | Method for assessing navigational capacity |
US20050033154A1 (en) * | 2003-06-03 | 2005-02-10 | Decharms Richard Christopher | Methods for measurement of magnetic resonance signal perturbations |
US20060161218A1 (en) * | 2003-11-26 | 2006-07-20 | Wicab, Inc. | Systems and methods for treating traumatic brain injury |
US20060241718A1 (en) * | 2003-11-26 | 2006-10-26 | Wicab, Inc. | Systems and methods for altering brain and body functions and for treating conditions and diseases of the same |
US20050175972A1 (en) * | 2004-01-13 | 2005-08-11 | Neuroscience Solutions Corporation | Method for enhancing memory and cognition in aging adults |
US20060293617A1 (en) * | 2004-02-05 | 2006-12-28 | Reability Inc. | Methods and apparatuses for rehabilitation and training |
US20070027406A1 (en) * | 2004-02-13 | 2007-02-01 | Georgia Tech Research Corporation | Display enhanced testing for concussions and mild traumatic brain injury |
US20050216243A1 (en) * | 2004-03-02 | 2005-09-29 | Simon Graham | Computer-simulated virtual reality environments for evaluation of neurobehavioral performance |
US20050197560A1 (en) * | 2004-03-05 | 2005-09-08 | Rao Stephen M. | System for detecting symptoms, determining staging and gauging drug efficacy in cases of Alzheimer's disease |
US20050228785A1 (en) * | 2004-04-02 | 2005-10-13 | Eastman Kodak Company | Method of diagnosing and managing memory impairment using images |
US20060074340A1 (en) * | 2004-09-24 | 2006-04-06 | Tsutomu Murata | Cognitive capacity measurement device and cognitive capacity measurement method |
US7333963B2 (en) * | 2004-10-07 | 2008-02-19 | Bernard Widrow | Cognitive memory and auto-associative neural network based search engine for computer and network located images and photographs |
US20070088404A1 (en) * | 2005-10-19 | 2007-04-19 | Allen Wyler | Methods and systems for improving neural functioning, including cognitive functioning and neglect disorders |
US20070166675A1 (en) * | 2005-12-15 | 2007-07-19 | Posit Science Corporation | Cognitive training using visual stimuli |
Cited By (29)
Publication number | Priority date | Publication date | Assignee | Title |
---|---|---|---|---|
US9629543B2 (en) | 2009-03-17 | 2017-04-25 | Emory University | Internet-based cognitive diagnostics using visual paired comparison task |
US11633099B2 (en) | 2009-03-17 | 2023-04-25 | Emory University | Internet-based cognitive diagnostics using visual paired comparison task |
US10694942B2 (en) | 2009-03-17 | 2020-06-30 | Emory University | Internet-based cognitive diagnostics using visual paired comparison task |
US10052023B2 (en) | 2011-07-05 | 2018-08-21 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Floor mat system and associated, computer medium and computer-implemented methods for monitoring and improving health and productivity of employees |
US10108783B2 (en) | 2011-07-05 | 2018-10-23 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Systems, computer medium and computer-implemented methods for monitoring health of employees using mobile devices |
US10307104B2 (en) | 2011-07-05 | 2019-06-04 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Chair pad system and associated, computer medium and computer-implemented methods for monitoring and improving health and productivity of employees |
US9805339B2 (en) | 2011-07-05 | 2017-10-31 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Method for monitoring and improving health and productivity of employees using a computer mouse system |
US9808156B2 (en) | 2011-07-05 | 2017-11-07 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Systems, computer medium and computer-implemented methods for monitoring and improving biomechanical health of employees |
US9830576B2 (en) | 2011-07-05 | 2017-11-28 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Computer mouse for monitoring and improving health and productivity of employees |
US9830577B2 (en) | 2011-07-05 | 2017-11-28 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Computer mouse system and associated computer medium for monitoring and improving health and productivity of employees |
US9844344B2 (en) | 2011-07-05 | 2017-12-19 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Systems and method to monitor health of employee when positioned in association with a workstation |
US10206625B2 (en) | 2011-07-05 | 2019-02-19 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Chair pad system and associated, computer medium and computer-implemented methods for monitoring and improving health and productivity of employees |
US9949640B2 (en) | 2011-07-05 | 2018-04-24 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | System for monitoring employee health |
US9962083B2 (en) | 2011-07-05 | 2018-05-08 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Systems, computer medium and computer-implemented methods for monitoring and improving biomechanical health of employees |
US10058285B2 (en) | 2011-07-05 | 2018-08-28 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Chair pad system and associated, computer medium and computer-implemented methods for monitoring and improving health and productivity of employees |
US9619613B2 (en) * | 2012-06-22 | 2017-04-11 | Integrated Deficit Examinations, LLC | Device and methods for mobile monitoring and assessment of clinical function through sensors and interactive patient responses |
US20130345524A1 (en) * | 2012-06-22 | 2013-12-26 | Integrated Deficit Examinations, LLC | Device and methods for mobile monitoring and assessment of clinical function through sensors and interactive patient responses |
US9171131B2 (en) * | 2012-06-22 | 2015-10-27 | Integrated Deficit Examinations, LLC | Device and methods for mobile monitoring and assessment of clinical function through sensors and interactive patient responses |
US10791978B2 (en) | 2014-02-10 | 2020-10-06 | Medical Care Corporation | Classifying individuals using finite mixture markov modelling and test trials with accounting for item position |
US10335079B2 (en) | 2014-02-10 | 2019-07-02 | Medical Care Corporation | Assessing cognition using item-recall trials with accounting for item position |
WO2015120481A1 (en) * | 2014-02-10 | 2015-08-13 | Medical Care Corporation | Assessing cognition using item-recall trials with accounting for item position |
US9889311B2 (en) | 2015-12-04 | 2018-02-13 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Systems, protective casings for smartphones, and associated methods to enhance use of an automated external defibrillator (AED) device |
US10475351B2 (en) | 2015-12-04 | 2019-11-12 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Systems, computer medium and methods for management training systems |
US10642955B2 (en) | 2015-12-04 | 2020-05-05 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Devices, methods, and computer medium to provide real time 3D visualization bio-feedback |
WO2017105907A1 (en) * | 2015-12-14 | 2017-06-22 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Systems and methods for acquiring and employing resiliency data for leadership development |
US10628770B2 (en) | 2015-12-14 | 2020-04-21 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Systems and methods for acquiring and employing resiliency data for leadership development |
US10824132B2 (en) | 2017-12-07 | 2020-11-03 | Saudi Arabian Oil Company | Intelligent personal protective equipment |
EP4216225A1 (en) * | 2022-01-20 | 2023-07-26 | Koninklijke Philips N.V. | Cognitive impairment determination apparatus |
WO2023139006A1 (en) * | 2022-01-20 | 2023-07-27 | Koninklijke Philips N.V. | Cognitive impairment determination apparatus |
Also Published As
Publication number | Publication date |
---|---|
US20080280276A1 (en) | 2008-11-13 |
US8475171B2 (en) | 2013-07-02 |
US20120190968A1 (en) | 2012-07-26 |
Similar Documents
Publication | Publication Date | Title |
---|---|---|
US8475171B2 (en) | Object recognition testing tools and techniques for measuring cognitive ability and cognitive impairment | |
US11696720B2 (en) | Processor implemented systems and methods for measuring cognitive abilities | |
US20160125748A1 (en) | Memory test for Alzheimer's disease | |
US9483613B2 (en) | Determination of neuropsychiatric therapy mechanisms of action | |
Macintyre-Béon et al. | Dorsal stream dysfunction in children. A review and an approach to diagnosis and management | |
Groen et al. | Where were those rabbits? A new paradigm to determine cerebral lateralisation of visuospatial memory function in children | |
Terruzzi et al. | The neuropsychological assessment of unilateral spatial neglect through computerized and virtual reality tools: a scoping review | |
Adabla et al. | A scoping review of virtual reality interventions for youth with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder | |
Finn et al. | A model of anxious arousal for public speaking | |
Horowitz-Kraus et al. | Fluent contextual reading is associated with greater synchronization of the visual and auditory networks, fluent reading and better speed of processing in children with dyslexia | |
Bangirana et al. | Computerized cognitive rehabilitation therapy (CCRT) for African Children: Evidence for neuropsychological benefit and future directions | |
Marek et al. | Advances in understanding human performance: Neuroergonomics, human factors design, and special populations | |
Serrao et al. | Performance of a sample of patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI), Alzheimer's Disease (AD) and healthy elderly on a lexical decision test (LDT) as a measure of pre-morbid intelligence | |
De Leon et al. | Comparison Of goal achievement when transitioning from in-person therapy to teletherapy in Westchester County early intervention program due to the COVID-19 pandemic | |
Sarker et al. | Analysis of smooth pursuit assessment in virtual reality and concussion detection using bilstm | |
Misawa et al. | A telerehabilitation program to improve visual perception in children and adolescents with hemianopia consecutive to a brain tumour: a single-arm feasibility and proof-of-concept trial. | |
Korhonen et al. | High Support Need and Minimally Verbal Children with Autism Playing a Preference Based Computer Game: A Pilot Eye-Tracking Study of Four Individual's Attendance to Eyes. | |
Guimarães et al. | Using shoe-mounted inertial sensors and stepping exergames to assess the motor-cognitive status of older adults: A correlational study | |
Enders et al. | Identification of Target Objects from Gaze Behavior during a Virtual Navigation Task | |
Ni | Investigating the feasibility of neuro-cognitive games for detecting the onset of dementia using a phantom arm compared to touchscreen version | |
Vestito et al. | STORMS: A Pilot Feasibility Study for Occupational TeleRehabilitation in Multiple Sclerosis | |
Crivelli et al. | Introducing Valutazione Neuropsicologica Interattiva in Telemedicina: A battery of tests designed for a videoconference-based tele-neuropsychological assessment. | |
Raulin et al. | Conducting research in the field of psychopathology | |
Dalansi | Reliability of a Virtual Reality Tool for the Assessment of Concussion | |
RANGE | WOODCOCK-JOHNSON IV TESTS OF COGNITIVE ABILITIES (WJ IV COG) |
Legal Events
Date | Code | Title | Description |
---|---|---|---|
STCB | Information on status: application discontinuation |
Free format text: EXPRESSLY ABANDONED -- DURING EXAMINATION |
|
AS | Assignment |
Owner name: NATIONAL INSTITUTES OF HEALTH - DIRECTOR DEITR, MARYLAND Free format text: CONFIRMATORY LICENSE;ASSIGNOR:OREGON HEALTH & SCIENCE UNIVERSTIY;REEL/FRAME:055705/0629 Effective date: 20210324 |