Abstract
In this chapter a review of the different uses of agent-based modeling for investigating social behaviors and dynamics is presented. Agent-based modeling is often used to study sociality both from a behavioral and evolutionary perspective.
The aim of this chapter is to present the strengths of this modeling approach, highlighting the usefulness of—and in some scientific domains, the need for—cognitively rich architectures, as, for instance, in studying the emergence of communicative systems and information sharing.
The main reasons supporting this perspective are different. On one hand, social behavior (and not collective behavior) of living organisms in most cases is possible because individuals have cognitive abilities and skills that allow them to interact in complex ways with other individuals and with the environment where they live. On the other hand, while analytical modeling simplifies properties and behaviors of a real system to find regularities and equilibria (and this may limit the applicability of this approach), agent-based modeling adopts the opposite philosophy: agents are heterogeneous—they behave in different ways and they use different strategies to deal with unexpected events and changing environments.
The use of agent-based modeling is consolidated in investigating complex social behaviors and dynamics in some specific scientific domains (e.g., the study of cooperative behavior in living organisms); but potentially, agent-based modeling may represent a useful scientific method and tool in many other scientific fields investigating complex systems and dynamics.
The study of social-ecological systems is presented as one of the main scientific fields where the use of agent-based modeling is gaining more and more attention and where it has already shown itself to be relevant.
Finally, another scientific field—namely, the study of Earth Systems and Dynamics—is presented to highlight the promising applications of agent-based models in helping to better understand complex and nonlinear phenomena (e.g., tipping points) where the social component (i.e., the human component) is crucial and plays a central role.
The scientific journey presented in this chapter should clarify the enormous potential of agent-based modeling from an interdisciplinary and multi-scale perspective, stressing the centrality of individual cognitive skills and abilities.
Access this chapter
Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout
Purchases are for personal use only
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Adger, N. (2000). Social and ecological resilience: Are they related? Progress in Human Geography, 24, 347–364.
Alexander, R. D. (1987). The biology of moral systems. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Alexander, J. C., & Giesen, B. (1987). From reduction to linkage: The long view of the micro-macro link. In J. C. Alexander, B. Giesen, R. Munch, & N. J. Smelser (Eds.), The micro-macro link (pp. 1–42). Berkeley: University of California Press.
Allen, T. F. H., & Starr, T. B. (1982). Hierarchy: Perspectives for ecological complexity. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
André, J. B., & Baumard, N. (2011). Social opportunities and the evolution of fairness. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 289, 128–135.
Axelrod, R. (1984). The evolution of cooperation. New York: Basic Books.
Axelrod, R., & Hamilton, W. D. (1981). The evolution of cooperation. Science, 211, 1390–1396.
Barclay, P. (2011). Competitive helping increases with the size of biological markets and invades defection. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 281, 47–55.
Bateson, G. (1979) Mind and nature: A necessary unit. http://www.oikos.org/mind&nature.htm.
Berkes, F. (1989) Common property resources: Ecology and community-based sustainable development. London: Belhaven Press.
Berkes, F. (1999). Sacred ecology: Traditional ecological knowledge and management systems. Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis.
Berkes, F., & Folke, C. (2002). Back to the future: Ecosystem dynamics and local knowledge. In L. H. Gunderson & C. S. Holling (Eds.), Panarchy: Understanding transformations in human and natural systems (pp. 121–146). Washington, D.C.: Island Press.
Berkes, F., Colding, J., & Folke, C. (2000). Rediscovery of traditional ecological knowledge as adaptive management. Ecological Applications, 10, 1251–1262.
Berkes, F., Colding, J., & Folke, C. (2001). Linking social-ecological systems. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Berkes, F., Colding, J., & Folke, C. (2003). Navigating social-ecological systems: Building resilience for complexity and change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Boekhorst, I. J. A., & Hogeweg, P. (1994a). Effects of tree size on travelband formation in orang-utans: Data-analysis suggested by a model. In R. A. Brooks & P. Maes (Eds.), Artificial life (pp. 119–129). Cambridge: The MIT Press.
Boekhorst, I. J. A. te, & Hogeweg, P. (1994b). Self- structuring in artificial ‘CHIMPS’ offers new hypotheses for male grouping in chimpanzees. Behaviour, 130, 229–52.
Bonabeau, E. (2002). Agent-based modeling: Methods and techniques for simulating human systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 99(3), 7280–7287.
Braeuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Chimpanzees really know what others can see in a competitive situation. Animal Cognition, 10(4), 439–448.
Bryson, J. J., Ando, Y., & Lehmann, H. (2007). Agent-based modelling as scientific method: A case study analysing primate social behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B—Biological Sciences, 362(1485), 1685–1698.
Bshary, R., & Bronstein, J. L. (2011). A general scheme to predict partner control mechanisms in pairwise cooperative interactions between unrelated individuals. Ethology, 117, 271–283.
Bugnyar, T., & Heinrich, B. (2005). Ravens, Corvus corax, differentiate between knowledgeable and ignorant competitors. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B—Biological Sciences, 272, 1641–1646.
Bugnyar, T., & Heinrich, B. (2006). Pilfering ravens, Corvus corax, adjust their behaviour to social context and identity of competitors. Animal Cognition, 9(4), 369–376.
Bull, J. J., & Rice, W. R. (1991). Distinguishing mechanisms for the evolution of cooperation. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 149, 63–74.
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(5), 187–192.
Campennì, M., & Schino, G. (2014). Partner choice promotes cooperation: The two faces of testing with agent-based models. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 344, 49–55.
Cangelosi, A. (2001). Evolution of communication and language using signals, symbols, and words. IEEE Transactions in Evolution Computation, 5(2), 93–101.
Carpenter, S. R., & Gunderson, L. H. (2001). Coping with collapse: Ecological and social dynamics in ecosystem management. BioScience, 51, 451–457.
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., Minch, E., & Mountain, J. L. (1992). Coevolution of genes and languages revisited. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 89(12), 5620–5624.
Charrier, I., & Sturdy, C. B. (2005). Call-based species recognition in the black-capped chickadees. Behavioural Processes, 70(3), 271–281.
Cheney, D. L. (2011). Extent and limits of cooperation in animals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 108, 10902–10909.
Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (1990). How monkeys see the world: Inside the mind of another species. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Clayton, N. S., Dally, J. M., & Emery, N. J. (2007). Social cognition by food-caching corvids. The western scrub-jay as a natural psychologist. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B—Biological Sciences, 362(1480), 507–522.
Clutton-Brock, T. H. (2009). Cooperation between non-kin in animal societies. Nature, 462, 51–57.
Costanza, R., Low, B. S., Ostrom, E., & Wilson, J. (2001). Institutions, ecosystems, and sustainability. Boca Raton: Lewis.
Cumming, G. S. (2011). Spatial resilience in social-ecological systems. London: Springer.
Dally, J. M., Emery, N. J., & Clayton, N. S. (2005). Cache protection strategies by western scrub- jays, Aphelocoma californica: Implications for social cognition. Animal Behaviour, 70(6), 1251–1263.
Dally, J. M., Emery, N. J., & Clayton, N. S. (2006). Food-caching western scrub-jays keep track of who was watching when. Science, 312(5780), 1662–1665.
Darwin, C. (1964). The origin of species. Cambridge: Harvard University Press (reprinted).
DeAngelis, D. L., Godbout, L., & Shuter, B. J. (1991). An individual-based approach to predict- ing density-dependent dynamics in smallmouth bass populations. Ecological Modelling, 57(1–2), 91–115.
Doebeli, M., & Hauert, C. (2005). Models of cooperation based on the prisoner’s dilemma and the snowdrift game. Ecology Letters, 8(7), 748–766.
Dugatkin, L. A. (1997). Cooperation among animals: An evolutionary perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Dunbar, R. I. M., & Shultz, S. (2007). Evolution in the social brain. Science, 317, 1344–1347.
Dyer, F. C., & Seeley, T. D. (1991). Dance dialects and foraging range in three asian honey bee species. Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology, 28(4), 227–233.
Emery, N. J., & Clayton, N. S. (2001). Effects of experience and social context on prospective caching strategies by scrub jays. Nature, 414, 443–446.
Epstein, J. M., & Axtell, R. (1996). Growing artificial societies. Social science from the bottom up. Washington, D.C.: Brookings Institution Press and Cambridge, MIT Press.
Eshel, I., & Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1982). Assortment of encounters and evolution of cooperativeness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 79, 1331–1335.
Evans, J. (2011). Environmental governance. London: Routledge.
Evans, C. S., Evans, C. L., & Marler, P. (1993). On the meaning of alarm calls: Functional reference in an avian vocal system. Animal Behaviour, 46(1), 23–38.
Fitch, W. T. (2008). Glossogeny and phylogeny: Cultural evolution meets genetic evolution. Trends in Genetics, 24(8), 373–374.
Fitch, W. T. (2010). The evolution of language. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Fitch, W. T., Huber, L., & Bugnyar, T. (2010). Social cognition and the evolution of language: Constructing a cognitive phylogeny. Neuron, 65(6), 795–814.
Folke, C. (2006). Resilience: The emergence of a perspective for social-ecological systems analysis. Global Environmental Change, 16, 253–267.
Folke, C., Carpenter, S., Elmqvist, T., Gunderson, L., Holling, C., & Walker, B. (2002). Resilience and sustainable development: Building adaptive capacity in a world of transformations. Ambio, 31, 437–440.
Frank, R. H. (1989). Passions within reason. New York: W.W. Norton & Company.
Fruteau, C., Lemoine, S., Hellard, E., Van Damme, E., & Noë, R. (2011). When females trade grooming for grooming: Testing partner control and partner choice models of cooperation in two primate species. Animal Behaviour, 81, 1223–1230.
Galef, B. G. J., & Laland, K. N. (2005). Social learning in animals: Empirical studies and theoretical models. BioScience, 55(6), 488–489.
Gilbert, N., & Troitzsch, K. (2005). Simulation for the social scientist (2nd edn). England: Open University Press.
Green, E., & Maegner, T. (1998). Red squirrels, Tamiasciurus hudsonicus, produce predator-class specific alarm calls. Animal Behaviour, 55(3), 511–518.
Greenberg, J. B., & Park, T. K. (1994). Political ecology. Journal of Political Ecology, 1, 1–12.
Gunderson, L. H., & Holling, C. S. (2002). Panarchy: Understanding transformations in human and natural systems. Washington, D.C.: Island Press.
Hailman, J., Ficken, M., & Ficken, R. (1985). The chick-a-dee calls of Parus atricapillus. Semi- otica, 56(3–4):191–224.
Hamilton, W. D. (1963). The evolution of altruistic behaviour. American Naturalist, 97(896), 354–356.
Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7(1), 17–52.
Hammerstein, P. (Ed.). (2003). Genetic and cultural evolution of cooperation. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Hare, B. (2001). Can competitive paradigms increase the validity of experiments on primate social cognition? Animal Cognition, 4(3–4), 269–280.
Hare, B., Call, J., Agnetta, B., & Tomasello, M. (2000). Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see. Animal Behaviour, 59(4), 771–785.
Hauert, C. (2001). Fundamental clusters in spatial 2 × 2 games. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B—Biological Sciences, 268(1468), 761–769.
Hemelrijk, C. K. (1996). Dominance interactions, spatial dynamics and emergent reciprocity in a virtual world. In P. Maes, M. J. Mataric, J.-A. Meyer, J. Pollack, & S. W. Wilson (Eds.), Proceedings of the fourth international conference on simulation of adaptive behavior 4 (pp. 545–552). Cambridge: MIT-Press.
Hemelrijk, C. K. (2000). Towards the integration of social dominance and spatial structure. Animal Behaviour, 59(5), 1035–1048.
Heyes, C. M. (1994). Social learning in animals: Categories and mechanisms. Biological Reviews, 69(2), 207–231.
Heyes, C. (2009). Evolution, development and intentional control of imitation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B—Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 2293–2298.
Hoffman, M., Yoeli, E., & Nowak, M. A. (2015). Cooperate without looking: Why we care what people think and not just what they do. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(6), 1727–1732.
Holling, C. S. (1986). The resilience of terrestrial ecosystems: Local surprise and global change. Sustainable Development of The Biosphere, 292–317.
Holling, C. S. (2001). Understanding the complexity of economic, ecological, and social systems. Ecosystems, 4(5), 390–405.
Huber, L., Range, F., Voelkl, B., Szucsich, A., Viranyi, Z., & Miklosi, A. (2009). The evolution of imitation: What do the capacities of nonhuman animals tell us about the mechanisms of imitation? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B—Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 2299–2309.
Johnstone, R. A., & Bshary, R. (2002). From parasitism to mutualism: Partner control in asymmetric interactions. Ecology letters, 5, 634–639.
Kaiser, D. (2004). Signaling in myxobacteria. Annual Review of Microbiology, 58, 75–98.
Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Chimpanzees know what others know, but not what they believe. Cognition, 109(2), 224–234.
Karin-D’Arcy, M., & Povinelli, D. J. (2002). Do chimpanzees know what each other see? A closer look. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 15, 21–54.
Killingback, T., Doebeli, M., & Knowlton, N. (1999). Variable investment, the continuous prisoner’s dilemma, and the origin of cooperation. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B—Biological Sciences, 266(1430), 1723–1728.
Kim, J. (1992). “Downward causation” in emergentism and non-reductive physicalism. In A. Beckermann, H. Flohr, & J. Kim (Eds.), Emergence or reduction? Essays on the prospects of nonreductive physicalism. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.
Kreft, J. U., Booth, G., & Wimpenny, J. W. T. (1998). Bacsim, a simulator for individual-based modelling of bacterial colony growth. Microbiology, 144(12), 3275–3287.
Krupnik, I., & Jolly, D. (2002). The earth is faster now: Indigenous observation on arctic environmental change. Fairbanks: Arcus.
Leadbeater, E., & Chittka, L. (2007). Social learning in insects—from miniature brains to consensus building. Current Biology, 17(16), 703–713.
Lehmann, L., & Keller, L. (2006). The evolution of cooperation and altruism. A general framework and classification of models. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 19, 1365–1376.
Levin, S. A. (1998). Ecosystems and the biosphere as complex adaptive systems. Ecosystems, 1, 431–436.
Levin, S. A. (1999). Fragile dominion: Complexity and the commons. Reading: Perseus Books.
Lieberman, E., Michel, J.-B., Jackson, J., Tang, T., & Nowak, M. A. (2007). Quantifying the evolutionary dynamics of language. Nature, 449, 713–716.
Machlis, G. E., Force, J. E., & Burch, W. R. Jr. (1997). The human ecosystem part I: The human ecosystem as an organizing concept in ecosystem management. Society and Natural Resources, 10, 347–367.
Mackinson, S., & Nottestad, L. (1998). Combining local and scientific knowledge. Reviews in Fish Biology and Fisheries, 8, 481–490.
Manser, M., Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2002). Suricate alarm calls signal predator class and urgency. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(2), 55–57.
McCay, B. J., & Acheson, J. M. (1987). The question of the cotntnons. The culture and ecology of comtnunal resources. Tucson: The University of Arizona Press.
McLain, R., & Lee, R. (1996). Adaptive management: Promises and pitfalls. Journal of Environmental Management, 20, 437–448.
Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Laland, K. N. (2004). Perspective: Is human cultural evolution darwinian? Evidence reviewed from the perspective of the origin of species. Evolution, 58(1), 1–11.
Gilbert, N. (2008). Agent-based models (quantitative applications in the social sciences). Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications, Inc.
Noble, J. (1999). Cooperation, conflict and the evolution of communication. Adaptive Behavior, 7(3–4), 349–369.
Noble, J., de Ruiter, J. P., & Arnold, K. (2010). From monkey alarm calls to human language: How simulations can fill the gap. Adaptive Behavior, 18(1), 66–82.
Noë, R. (2006). Cooperation experiments: Coordination through communication versus acting apart together. Animal Behaviour, 71, 1–18.
Noë, R., & Hammerstein, P. (1994). Biological markets: Supply and demand determine the effect of partner choice in cooperation, mutualism and mating. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 35, 1–11.
Norberg, J., & Cumming, G. S. (2008). Complexity theory for a sustainable future. New York: Columbia University Press.
Nowak, M. A. (2006). Five rules for the evolution of cooperation. Science, 314, 1560–1563.
Nowak, M. A., & May, R. M. (1992). Evolutionary games and spatial chaos. Nature, 359, 826–829.
Nowak, M. A., & Sigmund, K. (1998). Evolution of indirect reciprocity by image scoring. Nature, 393, 573–577.
Nowak, M. A., Tarnita, C. E., & Wilson, E. O. (2010). The evolution of eusociality. Nature, 466, 1057–1062.
Nunn, C. L., & Lewis, R. J. (2001). Cooperation and collective action in animal behavior. In R. Noë, van J. A. R. A. M. Hooff, & P. Hammerstein (Eds.), Economics in nature (pp. 42–66). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Pagel, M., Atkinson, Q. D., & Meade, A. (2007). Frequency of word-use predicts rates of lexical evolution throughout indo-european history. Nature, 449, 717–720.
Paolucci, M., Conte, R., & Di Tosto, G. (2006). A model of social organization and the evolution of food sharing in vampire bats. Adaptive Behavior, 14(3), 223–239.
Petschel-Held, G., et al. (1999). Syndromes of global change—A qualitative modelling approach to assist global environmental management. Environmental Modeling and Assessment, 4, 295–314.
Petschel-Held, G., & Reusswig, F. (1999). Climate change and global change—The syndrome concept. In J. Hacker & A. Pelchen (Eds.), Goals and economic instruments for the achievement of global warming mitigation in Europe (pp. 79–95). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Petschel-Held, G., Block, A., & Schellnhuber, H.-J. (1995). Syndrome des Globalen Wandels—ein systemarer Ansatz für Sustainable-Development-Indikatoren. GEOwissenschaften, 3, 81–87.
Povinelli, D. J., & Eddy, T. J. (1996). Chimpanzees: Joint visual attention. Psychological Science, 7(3), 129–135.
Povinelli, D. J., & Vonk, J. (2003). Chimpanzee minds: Suspiciously human? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(4), 157–160.
Povinelli, D. J., Nelson, K. E., & Boysen, S. T. (1990). Inferences about guessing and knowing by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 104(3), 203–210.
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515–526.
Pretty, J., & Ward, H. (2001). Social capital and the environment. World Development, 29, 209–227.
Redman, C., Grove, M. J., & Kuby, L. (2004). Integrating social science into the Long Term Ecological Research (LTER) Network: Social dimensions of ecological change and ecological dimensions of social change. Ecosystems, 7(2), 161–171.
Reynolds, C. W. (1987). Flocks, herds, and schools: A distributed behavioral model. Computer Graphics, 21(4), 25–34 (SIGGRAPH ’87 Conference Proceedings).
Roberts, G. (1998). Competitive altruism: From reciprocity to the handicap principle. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 265, 427–431.
Sachs, J. L., Mueller, U. G., Wilcox, T. P., & Bull, J. J. (2004). The evolution of cooperation. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 79, 135–160.
Schauder, S., & Bassler, B. L. (2001). The languages of bacteria. Genes & Development, 15, 1468–1480.
Schellnhuber, H.-J., et al. (1997). Syndromes of global change. GAIA, 6, 19–34.
Schermerhorn, P., & Scheutz, M. (2007). Investigating the adaptiveness of communication in multi-agent behavior coordination. Adaptive Behavior, 15(4), 423–445.
Schino, G. (2007). Grooming and agonistic support: A meta-analysis of primate reciprocal altruism. Behavioural Ecology, 18, 115–120.
Schino, G., & Aureli, F. (2008). Grooming reciprocation among female primates: A meta-analysis. Biology Letters, 4, 9–11.
Schino, G., & Aureli, F. (2009). Reciprocal altruism in primates: Partner choice, cognition, and emotions. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 39, 45–69.
Schuster, S., Woehl, S., Griebsch, M., & Klostermeier, I. (2006). Animal cognition: How archer fish learn to down rapidly moving targets. Current Biology, 16(4), 378–383.
Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (1990). The assessment by vervet monkeys of their own and other species’ alarm calls. Animal Behaviour, 40(4), 754–764.
Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2012). The evolutionary origins of friendship. The Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 153–177.
Sherratt, T. N., & Roberts, G. (1998). The evolution of generosity and choosiness in cooperative exchanges. Journal of theoretical biology, 193, 167–177.
Sigmund, K., Hauert, C., & Nowak, M. A. (2001). Reward and punishment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 98(19), 10757–10762.
Sigmund, K., Fehr, E., & Nowak, M. A. (2002). The economics of fair play. Scientific American, 286, 82–87.
Skyrms, B. (2009). Evolution of signalling systems with multiple senders and receivers. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B—Biological Sciences, 364(1518), 771–779.
Smith, K. (2002). Natural selection and cultural selection in the evolution of communication. Adaptive Behavior, 10(1), 25–45.
Sober, E., & Wilson, D. S. (1998). Unto others: The evolution and psychology of unselfish behavior. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Striedter, G. F. (2004). Principles of brain evolution. Sunderland: Sinauer.
Taga, M. E., & Bassler, B. L. (2003). Chemical communication among bacteria. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 100(2), 14549–14554.
Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Ratcheting up the ratchet: On the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B—Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 2405–2415.
Tiddi, B., Aureli, F., Polizzi di Sorrentino, E., Janson, C. H., & Schino, G. (2011). Grooming for tolerance? Two mechanisms of exchange in wild tufted capuchin monkeys. Behavioural Ecology, 22, 663–669.
Tomasello, M., Call, J., & Hare, B. (2003). Chimpanzees understand psychological states—The question is which ones and to what extent. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(4), 153–156.
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35–57.
Wagner, K., Reggia, J. A., Uriagereka, J., & Wilkinson, G. S. (2003). Progress in the simulation of emergent communication and language. Adaptive Behavior, 11(1), 37–69.
Walker, B. H., Gunderson, L. H., Kinzig, A. P., Folke, C., Carpenter, S. R., & Schultz, L. (2006). A handful of heuristics and some propositions for understanding resilience in social-ecological systems. Ecology and Society, 11(1), 13. http://www.ecologyandsociety.org/vol11/iss1/art13/.
Warren, D. M., Slikkerveer, L. J., & Brokensha, D. (1995). The cultural dimension of development: Indigenous knowledge system. London: Intermediate Technology Publications.
West, S. A., Griffin, A. S., & Gardner, A. (2007). Evolutionary explanations for cooperation. Current Biology, 17, 661–672.
Wilkinson, A., Kuenstner, K., Mueller, J., & Huber, L. (2010). Social learning in a non-social reptile (Geochelone carbonaria). Biology Letters, 6(5), 614–616.
Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Wilson, D. S., & Sober, E. (1994). Reintroducing group selection to the human behavioral sciences. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 17(4), 585–654.
Zentall, T. R. (2004). Action imitation in birds. Learning & Behavior, 32(1), 15–23.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Editor information
Editors and Affiliations
Rights and permissions
Copyright information
© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland
About this chapter
Cite this chapter
Campennì, M. (2016). Cognitively Rich Architectures for Agent-Based Models of Social Behaviors and Dynamics: A Multi-Scale Perspective. In: Cecconi, F. (eds) New Frontiers in the Study of Social Phenomena. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-23938-5_2
Download citation
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-23938-5_2
Published:
Publisher Name: Springer, Cham
Print ISBN: 978-3-319-23936-1
Online ISBN: 978-3-319-23938-5
eBook Packages: Physics and AstronomyPhysics and Astronomy (R0)