Abstract
Depend on the development of science and technology, the demands for robots are not only limited to the use of functions but also pay more attention to the emotional experience brought by the products. However, as the robot’s appearance approach human-likeness, it makes people uncomfortable, which is called the Uncanny Valley (UV). In this paper, we systematically review the hypothesis and internal mechanisms of UV. Then we focus on the methodological limitations of previous studies, including terms, assessment, and materials. At last, we summarize the applications in interaction design to avoid the uncanny valley and propose future directions.
You have full access to this open access chapter, Download conference paper PDF
Similar content being viewed by others
Keywords
1 Introduction
With the boom of computer technology and the development of related hardware facilities, robots have been used more and more widely in human society and provided many conveniences to people’s life [1]. In the past 20 years, social robots have developed fast and been used to interact with humans in many places, such as homes, hospitals, and shopping malls [1]. In order to improve human-robot interaction, engineers have designed robots that resemble humans highly [2]. There is a positive relationship between the human-likeness of robots and feelings of comfort with them. However, it has a steep dip in comfort and felt eeriness when robots looked almost but not entirely human, which called the “uncanny valley” [3].
The concept of “uncanny valley” was first proposed by Mori in 1970 [4]. In his paper, he envisioned people’s reactions to robots that looked and acted almost like a human and took some examples to verify his thought. He proposed that the level of affinity for the robot increased up with its appearance becoming more humanlike until people perceived the faces as eerie suddenly. However, as the robot’s human-likeness went on increasing, the eeriness reverted to likeability. This concept is useful to design a robot and works as a guide to improve human-robot interaction.
This paper systematically combs the explanation and internal mechanisms of the uncanny valley, the problems and deficiencies in existing research, and its practical applications in interaction design. The paper has the following structure. In Sect. 2, we describe different explanations of the uncanny valley. In Sect. 3, we present the defects of existing research, including the terms, assessments, and materials. In Sect. 4, we summarize the application of the phenomenon in the design of robots to avoid the uncanny valley.
2 Explanations of the Uncanny Valley
Researchers have proposed a variety of explanations to account for the uncanny valley phenomenon [2]. These hypotheses can be mainly divided into two categories. One category explains the phenomenon from an evolutionary psychology perspective that the uncanny feeling comes from facial features themselves, including the Threat Avoidance hypothesis [2, 3, 5, 6] and the Evolutionary Aesthetics hypothesis [2, 5, 6]. The other category interprets the phenomenon based on cognitive conflicts, including the Mind Perception hypothesis [1, 7], the Violation of Expectation hypothesis [1,2,3, 5], and the Categorical Uncertainty hypothesis [1, 2, 8]. Most related empirical studies focus on the latter because the cognitive response is easy to quantify and manipulate. However, the hypothesis of evolutionary psychology has little empirical research.
2.1 Explanations Based on Evolutionary Psychology
Threat Avoidance Hypothesis.
Mori [3] first pointed out that the UV phenomenon “may be important to our self-preservation”. During the process of evolution, diseases and death are two main threats to human beings. Thus, there are two explanations for the uncanny valley stemming from the avoidance of threat. The first explanation is called pathogen avoidance, which indicates that when people perceive the imperfections of humanoid robots, they will associate the defects with diseases [2]. Moreover, because of the high human-likeness, people may consider that humanoid robots are genetically close to humans and are likely to transmit diseases to humans [2, 5, 6]. However, this hypothesis is just an inference based on Rozin’s theory of disgust and has not been tested directly [2, 5]. Another explanation named mortality salience was proposed based on the terror management theory. Hanson [9] indicated that the flaws of humanoid robots combined with a humanlike appearance could remind us of mortality. From the aspect of this explanation, the uncanny feeling is the anxiety for mortality and the fear of death triggered by humanoid robots. People may be reminded of death and consider humanoid robots as dead individuals who come alive [2, 5]. However, there is only one study testing the hypothesis directly and found that the sensitivity to the vulnerability and impermanence of the physical body was significantly correlated with eerie ratings of android [10].
Evolutionary Aesthetics Hypothesis.
The hypothesis pays attention to the attractiveness of physical features and regards the uncanny feeling as an aversion to unattractive individuals. By morphing the images of abstract robots and realistic robots or real humans, Hanson’s research [9] found that the high-attractive images were consistently rated low in eeriness. Attractiveness is judged based on specific external characteristics that humans are sensitive to, such as bilateral symmetry, facial proportions, and skin quality [6]. These traits are associated with health, fertility, and other aspects that are close to the reproduction, and we inherit the preference for these traits from our ancestors who successfully reproduced under the selection pressure [2, 5, 6]. In a word, aesthetic properties are shaped by natural selection and determine the feeling of humanoid robots potentially.
These hypotheses explain the uncanny valley from the perspective of evolutionary psychology. Although they focus on various mechanisms to suggest the explanations, the essence is to achieve self-preservation and successful reproduction, which is the core of evolutionary psychology. However, the empirical studies supporting these hypotheses are still insufficient [2].
2.2 Explanations Based on Cognitive Conflicts
Mind Perception Hypothesis.
Gray and Wegner [7] proposed that humanoid robots are uncanny because they are so realistic that people may ascribe to them the capacity to feel and sense. However, this capacity is considered as the unique characteristic of humans, which is not expected to emerge on the robots [2, 7]. People are happy to have robots do works as human, but not have feelings like humans.
Violation of Expectation Hypothesis.
This hypothesis expands the mind perception hypothesis and believes that people will elicit specific expectations of the humanoid robots whose appearance resembles that of humans. For example, humanoid robots are expected to perform movements or speak as smoothly as humans. However, the robots often violate these expectations: the movements may perform mechanically, and the voice may be synthetic [2, 5]. The mismatch between expectations and reality results in negative emotional appraisal and avoidance behaviors, and leads to the feelings of eeriness and coldness [1, 11].
Categorical Uncertainty Hypothesis.
The hypothesis emphasizes that the feeling of eeriness is caused by the ambiguous boundary of categories [2, 5, 6]. There are many empirical studies on this hypothesis, but the results are quite controversial. Some studies support the Mori’s uncanny valley that the most humanlike robots are perceived as the robots. This perception blurs the category boundary between humans and machines to the greatest extent [12]. However, Ferrey, Burleigh, & Fenske’s study [13] employed human-robot and human-animal morphing images, and found that the negative peak is not always close to the human end (Line 1 in Fig. 1). The perceptual ambiguity was maximum at the midpoint of each continuum (Boundary 1 in Fig. 1). Furthermore, recent research found that the location of the category boundary did not coincide with the classic uncanny valley either (Boundary 2 in Fig. 1), and the negative peak was near the machine end (Line 2 in Fig. 1) [14].
These hypotheses interpret the uncanny valley based on cognitive conflicts. The conflict may exist between deduction and stereotype, between expectation and reality, or between different categories. Although there are many related empirical studies because the cognitive response is easy to quantify and manipulate, the explanation of the uncanny valley is still controversial.
3 Defects of Existing Research
At present, the related research of the uncanny valley involves computer science, psychology, material science, and other fields. Researchers studied the feelings of eeriness from various groups of users [15, 16], and explore the methods to improve the design of androids or computer-animated characters [14, 17, 18]. However, there are some problems in the existing studies, which may lead to inconsistent findings.
The classic uncanny valley is proposed by Mori. Line 1 is proposed by Ferrey, Burleigh, & Fenske (2015). Line 2 is proposed by Mathur, Reichling, & Lunardini, et al. (2020). Boundary 1 and 2 exhibit the category boundary in Ferrari et al. and Mathur et al.’s study, respectively.
3.1 Terms
Firstly, the absence of a clear definition of uncanny feelings may be a major cause of the controversial findings [19,20,21], especially the inconsistency of the translation [1]. Mori [4] used “shinwakan” or “bukimi” to represent the feelings when people faced different human replicas (e.g., androids or artifacts), when the feelings changed against human-likeness [22]. The original Japanese term “bukimi” was translated clearly into eeriness. However, the word “shinwakan” was first translated into familiarity, which was not equivalent and proved complex to define, partly because of its two meanings in English-a sense of closeness or lack of novelty [22,23,24,25]. Thus, it is no surprise that Mori’s original items have been extended to various interpretations and used in numerous studies. Realizing that, Mori et al. [3] revised the translation of familiarity into affinity, which refers to novelty or strangeness. Unfortunately, according to the literature review recently, although affinity has been used in some research, it is still not accepted and used consistently (Table 1).
Moreover, the same term can be explained as different connotations in various studies. For instance, “likability” is interpreted as friendly and enjoyable [14, 26], or aesthetic or pleasant appearance of the character [21]. Distinct instructions result in complicated comprehension.
One more reason for the dilemma may be that a single concept could not cover the uncanny feeling. Ho et al. [27] verified that uncanny feeling includes several kinds of emotions, such as fear, disgust, nervousness, dislike, and shock. Future research is encouraged to adopt a universal definition of the original term “shinwakan”, such as affinity [28], as well as confirm its boundaries and content compositions.
3.2 Assessments
Self-report questionnaires are widely used in previous studies. Gray and Wegner [7] used the Likert scale to collect the participants’ feelings of uneasy, unnerved, and creepy. Meanwhile, different scales were employed, such as a visual analog scale [14, 26], single-target IAT [29], and semantic differential scale [30, 31]. However, there are several potential limitations. Firstly, the construct validity of these questionnaires and scales are still questioned. For example, some dimensions include only one item, and some dimensions are highly correlated [2, 26, 32, 33]. Secondly, there are few suitable external calibrations to test whether the items measure the putative inner constructs (emotions). The assessment of uncanny feelings is subjective and lacks objective indexes [2]. Thirdly, psychometric noise will also bring an impact on the effectiveness of subjective rating [2]. Subjects may also give socially desirable responses [33].
Recently, objective indicators with high sensitivity, such as reaction times, pupillary responses, EMG (facial electromyography), and brain activity (ERPs and fMRI), are gradually adopted in this area [34,35,36,37,38,39]. For example, an fMRI study found that VMPFC (the ventromedial prefrontal cortex) integrates likability and human-likeness to an explicit UV reaction [39]. The fMRI technology used to explore uncanny feelings could go back to 2011 [40], while eye-tracking data collected firstly to study monkeys’ uncanny feelings in 2009 [41]. Thus, objective indexes and measurements are expected to determine the occurrence and operation mechanisms of uncanny feelings.
3.3 Materials
The selection criteria of experimental materials are not consistent [10, 26]. Similar to uncanny feelings, human-likeness is also a complex variable without a unified definition [24]. Therefore, various stimuli used in previous research induce irrelevant variables that may lead to confounding results. Table 2 shows the stimuli used in the experiments which aim to verify the UV effect in the past five years.
Participants were asked to make evaluations based on different forms of stimuli, such as videos, pictures, descriptions, words, or even interactions [14, 17, 21, 24, 29, 42, 43]. However, few studies compared the uncanny feelings evoked by these various mediums directly. Moreover, it is also difficult to infer whether people had similar feelings when they only see a part of the robots (e.g., face, head, or body), even all of them are displayed as static graphs [14, 26, 39, 44]. Furthermore, a small number of discontinuous stimuli could not reflect the continuous axis of human-likeness correctly. Bartneck et al. [24] got a result against Mori’s prediction, but the author pointed out that by using one human and his robotic copy as the stimuli was unable to confirm or disconfirm the Mori’s hypothesis. If the stimuli are arbitrarily or subjectively selected, then researchers would not be possible to obtain reliable conclusions of the UV effect [25].
Additionally, morphing artifact becomes one of the common methods to manipulate stimuli [20]. Following the guidelines that endpoint images should be similar to each other to reduce morphing artifacts [45], using similar source images of humans and robots for morphing restrict the generated range of human-likeness [31]. Even if the morphing artifacts controlled perfectly, it is still questioned whether the objectively manipulated human-likeness percentages are equal to perceived human-likeness [31, 45].
4 Practical Applications
The relevant research results of the uncanny valley, which involve users’ attitudes and concepts towards humanoid robots, play a significant role in the field of human-computer interaction, especially in interaction design. The development and innovation of humanoid robot design are trying to reduce the negative impact of the uncanny valley. From the perspective of a human, the question is whether the individual differences among the users can predict sensitivity to the uncanny valley and acceptance to the humanoid robots [10, 46]. From the view of the robot, the question is what kind of design is more acceptable to the majority of users [9, 46]. Therefore, in order to avoid the uncanny valley, there are two directions to improve the design of robots.
One way is to pursuit a nonhuman design deliberately so that the robots can lie at the first peak of affinity. Find a moderate degree of human likeness and a considerable sense of affinity, rather than taking the risk to increase the degree of human likeness continually [3]. There are two suggestions:
-
(1) Keep the balance between humanness and machine-like. The existence of the nose, eyelids, and mouth can increase the perception of humanness. Several design suggestions are proposed, for example, four or more features on the head, wide head with wide eyes, details in the eyes, or complex curves in the forehead [47].
-
(2) Design the robots for target users. For example, children rate human-machine like robots as the most positive, and they prefer cartoon-like and mechanical features, such as exaggerated facial features and wheels [48,49,50]. Elderly users have their preferences as well [46].
The other way is to reach the second peak and increase the level of human-likeness to step over the uncanny valley. The main idea of this way is to narrow the gap between robots and humans from various aspects:
-
(1) Make robots alive. Hanson [9] indicated that people feel unease because robots seem partly-dead. For example, robots shut down instead of going to sleep like humans. Thus, it is better to remove these flaws to make robots alive, friendly, and attractive.
-
(2) Express emotions. The addition of emotion display (e.g., emotional expressions, gait, voice, or gestures) can decrease the sense of uncanniness successfully [18, 51]. These emotion displays narrow the gap between the expectation the design raises about human nature and the perception of it, achieving a harmonious interaction.
5 Conclusions and Future Directions
Robots are becoming increasingly prevalent in everyday life. Humanoid robots are expected to be used more friendly and experienced more comfortably. Therefore, how to define and design the best appearances of humanoid robots is a critical question to be answered. In summary, decades of research develop two main explanations of the uncanny valley effect from the views of evolutionary psychology and cognitive conflict. The inconsistency of previous studies may be due to the absence of a unified definition, robust measure, and the representativeness of materials. Practically, pursuit a nonhuman design and increase the rate of human-likeness as high as possible are both helpful to avoid uncanny feelings. Future research is encouraged to reach a consensus on how to define the uncanny feelings, no matter it is a single item or complex emotions. Moreover, creating a sizeable and diverse database of images (or videos) covers a continuous series of human-likeness, as created by Mathur et al. [14], could avoid manipulation defects such as heterogeneous or discontinuous stimuli. Finally, considering most of the previous studies focus on young adults, future research is expected to test the uncanny valley in a more diverse user group.
References
Broadbent, E.: Interactions with robots: the truths we reveal about ourselves. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 68, 627–652 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-043958
Wang, S., Lilienfeld, S.O., Rochat, P.: The uncanny valley: existence and explanations. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 19(4), 393–407 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000056
Mori, M., MacDorman, K., Kageki, N.: The uncanny valley [from the field]. IEEE Robot. Autom. Mag. 19(2), 98–100 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1109/mra.2012.2192811
Mori, M.: The uncanny valley. Energy 7(4), 33–35 (1970)
MacDorman, K.F., Ishiguro, H.: The uncanny advantage of using androids in cognitive and social science research. Interact. Stud. 7(3), 297–337 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1075/is.7.3.03mac
MacDorman, K.F., Green, R.D., Ho, C.C., Koch, C.T.: Too real for comfort? Uncanny responses to computer generated faces. Comput. Hum. Behav. 25(3), 695–710 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2008.12.026
Gray, K., Wegner, D.M.: Feeling robots and human zombies: mind perception and the uncanny valley. Cognition 125(1), 125–130 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.06.007
Yamada, Y., Kawabe, T., Ihaya, K.: Categorization difficulty is associated with negative evaluation in the “uncanny valley” phenomenon. Jpn. Psychol. Res. 55(1), 20–32 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5884.2012.00538.x
Hanson, D.: Expanding the aesthetic possibilities for humanoid robots. In: IEEE-RAS International Conference on Humanoid Robots (2005)
MacDorman, K.F., Entezari, S.O.: Individual differences predict sensitivity to the uncanny valley. Interact. Stud. 16(2), 141–172 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1075/is.16.2.01mac
MacDorman, K.F., Chattopadhyay, D.: Reducing consistency in human realism increases the uncanny valley effect; increasing category uncertainty does not. Cognition 146, 190–205 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.09.019
Ferrari, F., Paladino, M.P., Jetten, J.: Blurring human-machine distinctions: anthropomorphic appearance in social robots as a threat to human distinctiveness. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 8(2), 287–302 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12369-016-0338-y
Ferrey, A.E., T.J. Burleigh, M.J. Fenske: Stimulus-category competition, inhibition, and affective devaluation: a novel account of the uncanny valley. Front. Psychol. 6(249) (2015). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00249
Mathur, M.B., Reichling, D.B., Lunardini, F., et al.: Uncanny but not confusing: multisite study of perceptual category confusion in the uncanny valley. Comput. Hum. Behav. 103, 21–30 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.08.029
Buckingham, G., Parr, J., Wood, G., et al.: Upper- and lower-limb amputees show reduced levels of eeriness for images of prosthetic hands. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 26(4), 1295–1302 (2019). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-019-01612-x
Destephe, M., Zecca, M.., Hashimoto, K., Takanishi, A.: Uncanny valley, robot and autism: perception of the uncanniness in an emotional gait. In: Proceedings of the 2014 IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Biomimetics, pp. 1152–1157. IEEE, Bali (2014). https://doi.org/10.1109/robio.2014.7090488
Ho, C.-C., MacDorman, K.F.: Measuring the uncanny valley effect. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 9(1), 129–139 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12369-016-0380-9
Koschate, M., Potter, R., Bremner, P., Levine, M.: Overcoming the uncanny valley: displays of emotions reduce the uncanniness of humanlike robots. In: 11th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI), pp. 359–366. IEEE, Christchurch (2016). https://doi.org/10.1109/hri.2016.7451773
Olivera-La Rosa, A.: Wrong outside, wrong inside: a social functionalist approach to the uncanny feeling. New Ideas Psychol. 50, 38–47 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2018.03.004
Katsyri, J., Forger, K., Makarainen, M., Takala, T.: A review of empirical evidence on different uncanny valley hypotheses: support for perceptual mismatch as one road to the valley of eeriness. Front. Psychol. 6(390) (2015). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00390
Katsyri, J., Makarainen, M., Takala, T.: Testing the ‘uncanny valley’ hypothesis in semirealistic computer-animated film characters: an empirical evaluation of natural film stimuli. Int. J. Hum Comput Stud. 97, 149–161 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhcs.2016.09.010
Ho, C.-C., MacDorman, K.F.: Revisiting the uncanny valley theory: developing and validating an alternative to the Godspeed indices. Comput. Hum. Behav. 26(6), 1508–1518 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2010.05.015
Bartneck, C., Kanda, T., Ishiguro, H., Hagita, N..: Is the uncanny valley an uncanny cliff? In: Proceedings of the 16th IEEE International Conference on Robot & Human Interactive Communication, pp. 368–373. IEEE, Jeju (2007)
Bartneck, C., Kanda, T., Ishiguro, H., Hagita, N.: My Robotic Doppelgänger – a critical look at the uncanny valley. In: 18th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication, pp. 269–276. IEEE, Toyama (2009)
Lay, S., Brace, N., Pike, G.: Circling around the uncanny valley: design principles for research into the relation between human likeness and eeriness. i-Perception 1–11 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1177/2041669516681309
Mathur, M.B., Reichling, D.B.: Navigating a social world with robot partners: a quantitative cartography of the Uncanny Valley. Cognition 146, 22–32 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.09.008
Ho, C.-C., MacDorman, K.F., Pramono, Z.A.D.: Human emotion and the uncanny valley: a GLM, MDS, and isomap analysis of robot video ratings. In: 3rd ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI), pp. 169–176. IEEE, Amsterdam (2008)
Wang, S., Rochat, P.: Human perception of animacy in light of the uncanny valley phenomenon. Perception 46(12), 1386–1411 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1177/0301006617722742
Villacampa, J., Ingram, G.P.D., Corradi, G., Rosa, A.O.-L.: Applying an implicit approach to research on the uncanny feeling. J. Artic. Support Null Hypothesis 16(1), 11–22 (2019)
MacDorman, K.F., Chattopadhyay, D.: Categorization-based stranger avoidance does not explain the uncanny valley effect. Cognition 161, 132–135 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.01.009
Katsyri, J., de Gelder, B., Takala, T.: Virtual faces evoke only a weak uncanny valley effect: an empirical investigation with controlled virtual face images. Perception 48(10), 968–991 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1177/0301006619869134
MacDorman, K.F.: Subjective ratings of robot video clips for human likeness, familiarity, and eeriness: an exploration of the uncanny valley. In: Proceedings of the ICCS/CogSci 2006 Long Symposium ‘Toward Social Mechanisms of Android Science’, pp. 26–29 (2006)
Bartneck, C., Kulić, D., Croft, E., Zoghbi, S.: Measurement instruments for the anthropomorphism, animacy, likeability, perceived intelligence, and perceived safety of robots. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 1, 71–81 (2009).https://doi.org/10.1007/s12369-008-0001-3
Saygin, A.P., Chaminade, T., Ishiguro, H., Driver, J., Frith, C.: The thing that should not be: predictive coding and the uncanny valley in perceiving human and humanoid robot actions. SCAN 7, 413–422 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr025
Strait, M., Scheutz, M.: Measuring users’ responses to humans, robots, and human-like robots with functional near infrared spectroscopy. In: 23rd IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication, pp. 1128–1133. IEEE, Edinburgh (2014)
Cheetham, M., Wu, L., Pauli, P., Jancke, L.: Arousal, valence, and the uncanny valley: psychophysiological and self-report findings. Front. Psychol. 6(981) (2015). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00981
Strait, M., Vujovic, L., Floerke, V., Scheutz, M., Urry, H.: Too much humanness for human-robot interaction. In: Proceedings of the 33rd Annual ACM Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems - CHI 2015, Seoul, pp. 3593–3602 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1145/2702123.2702415
Reuten, A., van Dam, M., Naber, M.: Pupillary responses to robotic and human emotions: the uncanny valley and media equation confirmed. Front. Psychol. 9(774) (2018). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00774
Rosenthal-von der Puetten, A.M., Kraemer, N.C., Maderwald, S., Brand, M., Grabenhorst, F.: Neural mechanisms for accepting and rejecting artificial social partners in the uncanny valley. J. Neurosci. 39(33), 6555–6570 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.2956-18.2019
Cheetham, M., Suter, P., Jäncke, L.: The human likeness dimension of the “uncanny valley hypothesis”: behavioral and functional MRI findings. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5(126) (2011). https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2011.00126
Steckenfinger, S.A., Ghazanfar, A.A.: Monkey visual behavior falls into the uncanny valley. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106(43), 18362–18366 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0910063106
Ramey, C.H.: An inventory of reported characteristics for home computers, robots, and human beings: applications for android science and the uncanny valley. In: Proceedings of the ICCS/CogSci 2006 Long Symposium ‘Toward Social Mechanisms of Android Science’ (2006)
Appel, M., Izydorczyk, D., Weber, S., Mara, M., Lischetzke, T.: The uncanny of mind in a machine: humanoid robots as tools, agents, and experiencers. Comput. Hum. Behav. 102, 274–286 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.07.031
Strait, M.K., Floerke, V.A., Ju, W., et al.: Understanding the uncanny: both atypical features and category ambiguity provoke aversion toward humanlike robots. Front. Psychol. 8(1366) (2017). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01366
Cheetham, M., Jancke, L.: Perceptual and category processing of the uncanny valley hypothesis’ dimension of human likeness: some methodological issues. Jove J. Visual. Exp. (76) (2013). https://doi.org/10.3791/4375
Prakash, A., Rogers, W.A.: Why some humanoid faces are perceived more positively than others: effects of human-likeness and task. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 7(2), 309–331 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12369-014-0269-4
DiSalvo, C.F., Gemperle, F., Forlizzi, J., Kiesler, S.: All robots are not created equal: the design and perception of humanoid robot heads. In: Proceedings of the 4th conference on Designing Interactive Systems: Processes, Practices, Methods, and Techniques, pp. 321–326. ACM Press, London (2002). https://doi.org/10.1145/778712.778756
Woods, S.: Exploring the design space of robots: children’s perspectives. Interact. Comput. 18(6), 1390–1418 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intcom.2006.05.001
Woods, S., Dautenhahn, K., Schulz, J.: The design space of robots: investigating children’s views. In: 13th IEEE International Workshop on Robot and Human Interactive Communication, pp. 47–52. IEEE, Kurashiki (2004). https://doi.org/10.1109/roman.2004.1374728
Lin, W., Yueh, H.-P., Wu, H.-Y., Fu, L.-C.: Developing a service robot for a children’s library: a design-based research approach. J. Assoc. Inf. Sci. Technol. 65(2), 290–301 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1002/asi.22975
Jizheng, Y., Zhiliang, W., Yan, Y.: Humanoid robot head design based on uncanny valley and FACS. J. Robot. (2014). https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/208924
Hanson, D., et al.: Upending the uncanny valley. In: The Twentieth National Conference on Artificial Intelligence and the Seventeenth Innovative Applications of Artificial Intelligence Conference, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, USA, 9–13 July 2005 (2005)
Zibrek, K., Kokkinara, E., McDonnell, R.: The effect of realistic appearance of virtual characters in immersive environments - does the character’s personality play a role? IEEE Trans. Visual Comput. Graphics 24(4), 1681–1690 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1109/tvcg.2018.2794638
Burleigh, T.J., Schoenherr, J.R., Lacroix, G.L.: Does the uncanny valley exist? An empirical test of the relationship between eeriness and the human likeness of digitally created faces. Comput. Hum. Behav. 29(3), 759–771 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.11.021
Chattopadhyay, D., MacDorman, K.F.: Familiar faces rendered strange: Why inconsistent realism drives characters into the uncanny valley. J Vis. 16(11), 7 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1167/16.11.7
Schwind, V., Leicht, K., Jaeger, S., Wolf, K., Henze, N.: Is there an uncanny valley of virtual animals? A quantitative and qualitative investigation. Int. J. Hum Comput Stud. 111, 49–61 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhcs.2017.11.003
Seyama, J.I., Nagayama, R.S.: The uncanny valley: effect of realism on the impression of artificial human faces. Presence Teleop. Virt. Environ. 16(4), 337–351 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1162/pres.16.4.337
Zlotowski, J., Sumioka, H., Nishio, S., Glas, D.F., Bartneck, C., Ishiguro, H.: Persistence of the uncanny valley: the influence of repeated interactions and a robot’s attitude on its perception. Front. Psychol. (2015). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00883
Acknowledgments
Jie Zhang and Shuo Li made equal contributions to this manuscript. This research is supported by fund for building world-class universities (disciplines) of Renmin University of China. Project No. 2018, the Beijing Natural Science Foundation (5184035), and CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Editor information
Editors and Affiliations
Rights and permissions
Copyright information
© 2020 Springer Nature Switzerland AG
About this paper
Cite this paper
Zhang, J., Li, S., Zhang, JY., Du, F., Qi, Y., Liu, X. (2020). A Literature Review of the Research on the Uncanny Valley. In: Rau, PL. (eds) Cross-Cultural Design. User Experience of Products, Services, and Intelligent Environments. HCII 2020. Lecture Notes in Computer Science(), vol 12192. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-49788-0_19
Download citation
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-49788-0_19
Published:
Publisher Name: Springer, Cham
Print ISBN: 978-3-030-49787-3
Online ISBN: 978-3-030-49788-0
eBook Packages: Computer ScienceComputer Science (R0)