Europe PMC
Nothing Special   »   [go: up one dir, main page]

Europe PMC requires Javascript to function effectively.

Either your web browser doesn't support Javascript or it is currently turned off. In the latter case, please turn on Javascript support in your web browser and reload this page.

This website requires cookies, and the limited processing of your personal data in order to function. By using the site you are agreeing to this as outlined in our privacy notice and cookie policy.

Abstract 


Purpose

Mindfulness has emerged as an important health concept based on evidence that mindfulness interventions reduce symptoms and improve health-related quality of life. The objectives of this study were to systematically assess and compare the properties of instruments to measure self-reported mindfulness.

Methods

Ovid Medline(®), CINAHL(®), and PsycINFO(®) were searched through May 2012, and articles were selected if their primary purpose was development or evaluation of the measurement properties (validity, reliability, responsiveness) of a self-report mindfulness scale. Two reviewers independently evaluated the methodological quality of the selected studies using the COnsensus-based Standards for the selection of health status Measurement INstruments checklist. Discrepancies were discussed with a third reviewer and scored by consensus. Finally, a level of evidence approach was used to synthesize the results and study quality.

Results

Our search strategy identified a total of 2,588 articles. Forty-six articles, reporting 79 unique studies, met inclusion criteria. Ten instruments quantifying mindfulness as a unidimensional scale (n = 5) or as a set of 2-5 subscales (n = 5) were reviewed. The Mindful Attention Awareness Scale was evaluated by the most studies (n = 27) and had positive overall quality ratings for most of the psychometric properties reviewed. The Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire received the highest possible rating ("consistent findings in multiple studies of good methodological quality") for two properties, internal consistency and construct validation by hypothesis testing. However, none of the instruments had sufficient evidence of content validity. Comprehensiveness of construct coverage had not been assessed; qualitative methods to confirm understanding and relevance were absent. In addition, estimates of test-retest reliability, responsiveness, or measurement error to guide users in protocol development or interpretation of scores were lacking.

Conclusions

Current mindfulness scales have important conceptual differences, and none can be strongly recommended based solely on superior psychometric properties. Important limitations in the field are the absence of qualitative evaluations and accepted external referents to support construct validity. Investigators need to proceed cautiously before optimizing any mindfulness intervention based on the existing scales.

Free full text 


Logo of nihpaLink to Publisher's site
Qual Life Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 Dec 1.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC3745812
NIHMSID: NIHMS461799
PMID: 23539467

Mindfulness: A systematic review of instruments to measure an emergent patientreported outcome (PRO)

Abstract

Purpose

Mindfulness has emerged as an important health concept based on evidence that mindfulness interventions reduce symptoms and improve health-related quality of life. The objectives of this study were to systematically assess and compare the properties of instruments to measure self-reported mindfulness.

Methods

Ovid Medline®, CINAHL®, and PsycINFO® were searched through May 2012, and articles were selected if their primary purpose was development or evaluation of the measurement properties (validity, reliability, responsiveness) of a self-report mindfulness scale. Two reviewers independently evaluated the methodological quality of the selected studies using the COnsensus-based Standards for the selection of health status Measurement INstruments (COSMIN) checklist. Discrepancies were discussed with a third reviewer, and scored by consensus. Finally, a level of evidence approach was used to synthesize results and study quality.

Results

Our search strategy identified a total of 2,588 articles. Forty-six articles, reporting 79 unique studies, met inclusion criteria. Ten instruments quantifying mindfulness as a unidimensional scale (n=5) or as a set of 2 to 5 subscales (n=5) were reviewed. The Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS) was evaluated by the most studies (n=27), and had positive overall quality ratings for most of the psychometric properties reviewed. The Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ) received the highest possible rating (“consistent findings in multiple studies of good methodological quality”) for two properties, internal consistency and construct validation by hypothesis testing. However, none of the instruments had sufficient evidence of content validity. Comprehensiveness of construct coverage had not been assessed; qualitative methods to confirm understanding and relevance were absent. In addition, estimates of test-retest reliability, responsiveness, or measurement error to guide users in protocol development or interpretation of scores were lacking.

Conclusions

Current mindfulness scales have important conceptual differences, and none can be strongly recommended based solely on superior psychometric properties. Important limitations in the field are the absence of qualitative evaluations and accepted external referents to support construct validity. Investigators need to proceed cautiously before optimizing any mindfulness intervention based on the existing scales.

Keywords: Mindfulness, Instruments, Psychometrics, Measurement properties, COSMIN checklist, Systematic review

Introduction

Mindfulness has emerged as an important concept in health and outcomes research, driven by a rapidly growing body of evidence that mindfulness training reduces symptoms and improves quality of life. Mindfulness training is the basis for widely accepted interventions in psychosomatic medicine and psychology [1]. Recent reviews have summarized evidence of the efficacy of these mindfulness interventions for persons with cancer [2], chronic medical conditions [3], and psychological disorders [4]. Clinical efficacy and durability have been shown for depression relapse prevention, anxiety reduction, and insomnia [4-7]. Recent meta-analyses estimated small to medium-sized treatment effects for the impact of mindfulness training on symptoms of stress, anxiety and depression [4,7,8]. Clinical trials of mindfulness training with health providers and community samples demonstrate significant improvements in stress management and enhanced well-being [9,10]. Mindfulness training has also been shown to improve biomarkers of glycemic control in diabetes [11,12], enhance immune response [13], and accelerate skin healing in psoriasis [14].

Mindfulness can be a dynamically changing state, a trait that differs between persons, and a skill that can be enhanced through training [15]. Drawing upon sources in Buddhist psychology, mindfulness has been described as arising from the intentional deployment of a triad of intertwined “behaviors of the mind” - attention, awareness and attachment; and defined as “the active maximizing of the breadth and clarity of awareness” [16]. Adapting mindfulness for use in health interventions for patients with cancer, Susan Bauer-Wu explains mindfulness as “Our capacity to intentionally bring awareness to present-moment experience with an attitude of openness and curiosity. It is being awake to the fullness of our lives right now, through engaging the five senses and noticing the changing landscapes of our minds without holding on to or pushing away from any of it” [17]. In preparing this review, we were guided by the two-part model of mindfulness proposed by Bishop and colleagues following a series of discussions among an interdisciplinary group of researchers [18]. This consensus model of mindfulness encompasses two components, attention and acceptance. The attention component pertains to maintaining awareness of present moment experience, and the acceptance component relates to the quality of relationship to experience (e.g., attitudes of openness and curiosity). This two-part conceptualization of mindfulness has been widely cited, and attention and acceptance are common elements across most definitions used in the construction of self-reports [19].

Although the mechanisms responsible for the health benefits of mindfulness are not known, clinical, experimental and brain imaging studies suggest increased symptom awareness, reduced emotional arousal, and greater engagement in health-promoting behaviors are involved [20,21]. Measuring mindfulness is important for research aimed at understanding its role in helping people to deal with emotional and physical health problems, and to guide refinements of mindfulness interventions to optimize health benefits [22,23]. It is appropriate to evaluate measures of mindfulness using a framework designed for patient reported outcomes (PROs), as large numbers of patients are being asked to complete mindfulness self-assessments in the context of health care and outcomes research [2,3,8]. Other personal factors highly salient to health maintenance and disease prevention which are not conventionally considered outcomes, such as self-efficacy, self-esteem, and perceived social support, have been included in popular frameworks for PROs and self-report instruments to measure these health-related factors have been developed using PRO guidelines (http://www.nihpromis.org/).

Numerous studies have evaluated self-report instruments to quantify mindfulness [23-25], however a comprehensive, systematic review of these instruments has not been conducted using a level of evidence approach. The level of evidence approach relies upon systematically ranking studies by the rigor of their methods so that final recommendations reflect results from the most methodologically sound studies. Conclusions from a level of evidence approach consider both the consistency of findings across studies and the rigor of those studies. The strongest evidence derives from consistent findings from multiple studies judged to have good or excellent methods. The aim of this study is to critically appraise and summarize the quality of the measurement properties of all published self-report mindfulness instruments using a level of evidence approach and the COnsensus-based Standards for the selection of health Measurement INstruments (COSMIN) guidelines [26]. COSMIN uses a taxonomy of measurement properties selected for relevance to health-related PROs based on the consensus of an international team of experts in health outcomes research. COSMIN includes uniform definitions and standards for the evaluation of methodological quality of studies to be reviewed, and has been used in more than a dozen systematic reviews published in peer-reviewed journals.

Methods

Search strategy

The electronic databases Ovid Medline® (1949 through May 2012), CINAHL® (1981 through May 2012), and PsycINFO® (1806 through May 2012) were searched using mindfulness index terms in combination with psychometric terms as described in Appendix 1. A manual search of the references of the included studies was conducted to supplement the electronic search. The search was limited to articles published in the English language.

Selection criteria

Articles were selected if their primary purpose was to develop or evaluate the measurement properties of an original version of a mindfulness instrument. The instrument had to quantify mindfulness, and be developed for self-administration by adults. Instruments that were program-specific were excluded as were instruments that did not measure mindfulness per se. Therefore, instruments to measure mindful eating [27], mindful coping [28], meditation experience [29], mindfulness practice [30], self-compassion [31], and mindfulness-based relapse prevention adherence and competence (MBRP-AC) [32] were excluded. Articles were excluded if they were not full-text, original articles (e.g., reviews, commentaries, or dissertations) or if they were designed to create a brief, translated, or adolescent/child’s version of another mindfulness scale. Articles about mindfulness instruments originally developed in any language other than English were initially excluded, however, after review of the collected articles, an exception to this rule was made to include the Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory because of its importance to the field as the first insight meditation-inspired self-report measure of mindfulness. Articles were also excluded if the primary aim was to test the efficacy of a mindfulness intervention. The decision to exclude efficacy trials was based on the recommendation for the conduct of systematic reviews from the text by De Vet et al. [33]. These authors note that efficacy studies generally provide only indirect evidence on the measurement properties of an instrument and this evidence is often difficult to interpret. Efficacy trials have been the focus of a growing number of meta-analyses.

One reviewer (T.P.) conducted the initial screening of titles and abstracts for all articles retrieved by the literature search, and identified candidate articles. Two reviewers (T.P. and C.R.G.) assessed the full text of the candidate articles, and jointly made decisions regarding article inclusion.

Measurement properties

The COSMIN taxonomy groups psychometric properties into three domains: reliability, validity, and responsiveness [34]. Reliability, the degree to which an instrument is free from measurement error, includes three properties: internal consistency, measurement error, and reliability. Internal consistency is the degree of the interrelatedness among the items in an instrument, and is typically assessed by Cronbach’s alpha. Measurement error is the systematic and random error that is not attributed to true changes in the underlying construct, and it is adequate if the smallest detectable change (SDC) on the instrument is less than the minimal important change (MIC) [35]. Reliability is the proportion of the total variance reflecting true differences between persons, assessed by intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs), Cohen’s Kappa, or test-retest correlations. Validity is the extent to which an instrument measures the construct it purports to measure. COSMIN groups three properties in the validity domain: content validity, construct validity, and criterion validity. Content validity includes face validity, comprehensiveness, and relevance of the items in an instrument for its target population and purpose. Structural validity, hypothesis testing, and cross-cultural validity are aspects of construct validity. Structural validity is the evidence to support the dimensionality of an instrument, and hypothesis testing is the degree to which relationships between an instrument and other measures conform to expectations, including differences between known groups. Relationships are often assessed by the Pearson correlation coefficient (r). Criterion validity, the extent that an instrument correlates with an accepted “gold standard” was not applicable for this review, because there is no gold standard for mindfulness. Cross-cultural validity, the extent that items of a translated or adapted version of an instrument perform as items on the original perform, was also not evaluated for this review. The final domain is responsiveness, the ability of an instrument to detect change in the underlying construct. To assess responsiveness, investigators pose hypotheses about expected correlations between the change score on the target instrument and change scores on other instruments for the same or other constructs. This is essentially validity in a longitudinal context. Responsiveness is not assessed by treatment effect size in COSMIN. This approach is consistent with the approach of Brown & Ryan, who noted that “the present study was not designed to test the efficacy of the intervention per se, but rather to examine whether mindfulness and changes in it were related to well-being outcomes and changes in them” [15].

The COSMIN checklist and study quality assessment

An evidence-based approach requires that results be relied upon only when they are produced by methodologically sound studies. The COSMIN checklist contains 98 items to assess whether a study of measurement properties meets quality standards [36]. Study quality is determined separately for each measurement property, using 5 to 18 items each rated as poor, fair, good, or excellent. The final quality rating for a property is the lowest rating of any item pertinent to that property (worst rating counts) [36].

Two reviewers (T.P. and C.R.G.) independently extracted data from the selected articles and evaluated methodological quality using the COSMIN checklist as a guide. Discrepancies were discussed with a third reviewer (M.R.-S.) to reach consensus. Where a single article presented multiple studies, each study was separately evaluated and rated for every measurement property it addressed.

Best evidence synthesis

Study findings were rated as “positive,” “negative,” or “indeterminate” for each measurement property based on criteria proposed by Terwee et al (Table 1) [37]. Summaries for each instrument were prepared showing how many studies of excellent, good, fair, or poor quality provided positive, negative, or indeterminate results by property. Overall ratings were then synthesized for each instrument across studies using a level of evidence approach that considered the number and methodological quality of the studies, and the consistency of their findings (Table 2) [38]. Findings from poor quality studies received no weight in the final synthesis.

Table 1

Quality criteria for measurement properties adapted from Terwee et al. [37]

PropertyRatingQuality Criteriaa
Reliability
 Internal consistency+(Sub)scale unidimensional AND Cronbach’s alpha(s) ≥0.70
?Dimensionality not known OR Cronbach’s alpha not determined
(Sub)scale not unidimensional OR Cronbach’s alpha(s) < 0.70
 Measurement error+MIC > SDC OR MIC outside the LOA
?MIC not defined
MIC ≤ SDC OR MIC equals or inside LOA
 Reliability+ICC/weighted Kappa ≥ 0.70 OR Pearson’s r ≥ 0.80
?Neither ICC/weighted Kappa, nor Pearson’s r determined
ICC/weighted Kappa < 0.70 OR Pearson’s r < 0.80
Validity
 Content validity+The target population considers all items in the questionnaire to be relevant AND considers the questionnaire to be complete
?No target population involvement OR no assessment of completeness or comprehensivenessa
The target population considers items in the questionnaire to be irrelevant OR considers the questionnaire to be incomplete
Construct validity
 Structural validity+Factors should explain at least 50% of the variance OR Good or adequate fit by goodness-of-fit criteria for a CFA or EFAa, b
?Explained variance not mentioned OR equivocal fit by goodness-of-fit criteria for a CFA or EFAa, b
Factors explain < 50% of the variance OR Poor fit by goodness-of-fit criteria for a CFA or EFAa, b
 Hypothesis testing+Correlation with an instrument measuring the same construct ≥ 0.50 OR at least 75% of the results are in accordance with the hypotheses AND correlation with related constructs is higher than with unrelated constructs OR No evidence of DIFa
?Solely correlations determined with unrelated constructs OR ≥ 50% but < 75% of the results are in accordance with the hypothesesa OR possible DIFa
Correlation with an instrument measuring the same construct < 0.50 OR < 50% of the results are in accordance with the hypothesesa OR correlation with related constructs is lower than with unrelated constructs OR notable evidence of DIFa
Responsiveness
 Responsiveness+Correlation of changes with an instrument measuring change in the same construct ≥ 0.50 OR at least 75% of the results are in accordance with the hypotheses OR AUC ≥ 0.70 AND correlation of changes with related constructs is higher than with unrelated constructs
?Solely correlations determined with unrelated constructs
Correlation of changes with an instrument measuring change in the same construct < 0.50 OR < 75% of the results are in accordance with the hypotheses OR AUC < 0.70 OR correlation of changes with related constructs is lower than with unrelated constructs

MIC: Minimal important change, SDC: Smallest detectable change, LOA: Limits of agreement, ICC: Intraclass correlation coefficient, DIF: Differential item functioning, AUC: Area under the curve

+ positive rating, ? indeterminate rating, - negative rating

aQuality criteria are from [37], with modifications by the authors indicated by italics.
bGood or Adequate fit: comparative fit index (CFI) ≥ 0.90, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) ≤ 0.08, standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) < 0.10 [83-85]; Inadequate fit: CFI ≤ 0.85, RMSEA ≥ 0.10, SRMR ≥ 0.10; Indeterminate fit: the values of fit indexes ranged in between the adequate criteria and inadequate criteria.

Table 2

Levels of evidence for the overall quality of the measurement properties

LevelRatingCriteria
Strong+++ or − − −Consistent findings in multiple studies of good methodological quality OR in one study of excellent methodological quality
Moderate++ or − −Consistent findings in multiple studies of fair methodological quality OR in one study of good methodological quality
Limited+ or −One study of fair methodological quality
Conflicting±Conflicting findings from studies of comparable quality
Indeterminate?Findings from excellent, good or fair studies were not definitively positive or negative
NonenaFindings from excellent, good or fair were not available

Table adapted from Van Tulder et al. [38]: + positive result; - negative result; ± both positive and negative findings have been reported by studies of adequate quality; ? findings from studies of adequate quality were not definitively positive or negative; na findings from studies of adequate quality were not available

Results

The study selection process is presented in Figure 1. A total of 2,588 unique articles were identified using the search strategy; of these, 146 articles were selected based on their title and abstract. For further assessment, the full text of these articles was examined resulting in the exclusion of 67 articles. As shown in Figure 1, most of these were excluded because evaluation of psychometric properties was not the primary focus of the article. Another 33 articles were then excluded because they addressed a translated, short or modified version of the original instruments, leaving a total of 46 articles for inclusion in our review. These 46 articles contained 79 separate studies, and evaluated 10 different mindfulness instruments. Several studies evaluated multiple instruments. Table 3 shows the characteristics of the included studies, Table 4 presents the characteristics of the included instruments, and Table 5 shows the COSMIN ratings of the methodological quality of these studies, by measurement property. No selected article was completely excluded for poor methodological quality. Our synthesis of the results and level of evidence for the properties of each mindfulness instrument is presented in Table 6. Results for each instrument are summarized below. In these summaries we use the following conventions for describing correlations: correlations are considered strong if |r| is between 0.7 and 1.0, moderate if 0.4 ≤ |r| < 0.7, and weak if 0 < |r| < 0.4. Results from studies of poor methodological quality are not included in these summaries.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is nihms461799f1.jpg

Flowchart of the search strategy and selection of articles

a Several papers included both translated and created a short/or modified version.

b Several papers reviewed multiple scales and/or contained multiple studies.

Table 3

Characteristics of the included studies

StudyPopulationSample sizeAge, mean (SD)Female (%)Country
Argus and Thomson (2008) [86]Patients with depression14143 (14)68Australia
Baer et al. (2004) [50]Substudy 1: Psychologists/Doctoral students in clinical psychology5/6nr mostly 18-22 (nr)/nr/36 (nr)100/50 about 60/nr/96US
Substudy 2 and substudy 6: Undergraduates (sample 1)/Undergraduates (sample 2)/Adults with borderline personality disorder (sample 3)205/215/26US
Substudy 3: Subset of sample 249nrnrUS
Substudy 4: Subset of sample 113020 (nr)56US
Substudy 5: Undergraduates115nrnrUS
Baer et al. (2006) [44]Substudy 1 and substudy 2: Undergraduates61321 (nr)70US
Substudy 3: Undergraduates26819 (nr)77US
Substudy 4: Samples from substudy 1, substudy 2, and substudy 388120 (nr)72US
Baer et al. (2008) [59]Undergraduates/Community participants/Nonmeditators/Meditators259/293/252/21319 (3)/50 (7)/44 (12)/49 (13)78/60/58/68US/UK/US/US
Baer et al. (2011) [68]Meditators11546 (12)73US
Nonmeditators11544 (12)63US
Barnes and Lynn (2010-2011) [63]Undergraduates14519 (2)69US
Baum et al. (2010) [87]Patients with recurrent depression10048 (11)77England
Bernstein et al. (2011) [88]Adults with traumatic events7630 (13)46US
Brown and Ryan (2003) [15]Study: Undergraduates/Undergraduates/General adults/Undergraduates313/327/239/6020 (nr)/20 (nr)/43 (nr)/19 (nr)66/64/66/57US
Substudy 1: Undergraduates, general adults74 to 1,04619 to 23 (nr)55 to 66US
Substudy 2: Community meditators and nonmeditators10041 (nr)29US
Substudy 3: Undergraduates9020 (nr)66US
Substudy 4: Community participants/Undergraduates74/9238 (nr)/20 (nr)55/74US
Substudy 5: Patients with breast or prostate cancer4155 (10)78US
Buchheld et al. (2001) [53]Retreat participants11543 (nr)69German
Cardaciotto et al. (2008) [73]Substudy 1: Mindfulness experts6nr33US
Substudy 2: Undergraduates20422 (4)52US
Substudy 3: Undergraduates55920 (4)51US
Substudy 4: Outpatients in psychiatry5241 (12)56US
Substudy 5: Inpatients with eating disorders3030 (11)90US
Substudy 6: Graduate students seeking psychotherapy7826 (8)89US
Carlson and Brown (2005) [39]Outpatients with cancer12250 (13)67Canada
Community participants12248 (16)67Canada
Cash and Whittingham (2010) [64]Community participants10636 (16)59Australia
Chadwick et al. (2008) [58]Community participants: Nonmeditators5147 (nr)75England
Community participants: Meditators8347 (nr)60England
Patients with psychosis12231 (nr)36England
Christopher et al. (2009)a [41]Undergraduates36522 (6)71US
Christopher and Gilbert (2010) [45]Undergraduates36522 (6)71US
Christopher et al. (2012) [60]Adults online (meditators and nonmeditators)34932 (12)75US
Cordon and Finney (2008) [43]Securely attached undergraduates22819 (1)77US
Insecurely attached undergraduates26719 (1)77US
Davis et al. (2009) [70]Community participants36940 (14)65Australia
Undergraduates9222 (7)80Australia
Emanuel et al. (2010) [89]Undergraduates: online responders10923 (nr)78US
Undergraduates: paper responders111nrnrUS
Feldman et al. (2007) [57]Substudy 1: Undergraduates/Undergraduates250/29819 (3)/19 (2)64/61US
Substudy 2: Undergraduates21219 (2)60US
Fernandez et al. (2010) [61]Undergraduates31622 (0.4)92US
Fisak and von Lehe (2012) [66]Undergraduates40022 (5)69US
Fresco et al. (2007) [71]Substudy 1: Undergraduates/Undergraduates1,150/51919 (4)/19 (2)67/65US/US
Substudy 2: Undergraduates6120 (3)56US
Substudy 3: Patients with depression vs. Healthy control220 vs. 5044 (10) vs. 45 (9)75 vs. 74England, Wales, Canada vs. England
Frewen et al. (2008) [47]Substudy 1: Undergraduates64nr73Canada
Substudy 2: Undergraduates43nr70Canada
Ghorbani et al. (2009)a [46]Undergraduates: three samples256/298/34620 (5)/19 (3)/20 (3)37/68/54US
Haigh et al. (2011) [72]Undergraduates (sample 1)/Undergraduates (sample 2)/ Undergraduates (subset of sample 1 + subset of sample 2)582/457/45119 (4)/23 (10)/22 (nr)69/60/69US
Herndon (2008) [90]Undergraduates142nrnrUS
Hollis-Walker and Colosimo (2011) [62]Undergraduates and demographically similar community participants12321 (nr)78Canada
Lau et al. (2006) [69]Substudy 1: General adults (Meditators + Nonmeditators)390 (232+158)41 (13)55Canada
Substudy 2: Patients with psychiatric or medical conditions12347 (13)68Canada
Lavender et al. (2011) [65]Undergraduate women27620 (3)100US
Leigh et al. (2005) [55]Undergraduates196nr63US
MacKillop and Anderson (2007) [40]Undergraduates711Mostly 18-19 (nr)53US
McCracken et al. (2007) [91]Patients with pain10547 (13)60UK
McCracken and Thompson (2009) [92]Patients with pain150b47 (13)64UK
McKee et al. (2007) [93]Community participants15422 (8)57US
Roemer et al. (2009) [94]Substudy 1: University commuters41123 (nr)64US
Substudy 2: Clinical sample diagnosed with generalized anxiety disorder vs. control16 vs.1633 (12) vs. 31 (9)69 vs. 69US
Schmertz et al. (2009) [49]Undergraduates5020 (3)82US
Van Dam et al. (2009) [67]Undergraduates: Nonmeditators/Meditators263/5819 (1)/48 (14)48/64US
Van Dam et al. (2010) [42]Undergraduates41420 (3)67US
Vujanovic et al. (2007) [95]Community participants24822 (8)55US
Vujanovic et al. (2009) [51]Adults with traumatic life events23923 (10)54US
Vujanovic et al. (2010) [52]Non-clinical community young adults19324 (10)55US
Walach et al. (2006) [54]Meditators/General adults/Clinical sample with psychiatric conditions85/85/11744 (9)/34 (12)/nr66/55/nrGerman
Waters et al. (2009) [48]Adult smokers15844 (12)45US
Zvolensky et al. (2006) [96]Community participants17022 (8)56US

nr: Not reported

aStudies were limited to English speaking students.
bIncludes the sample from McCracken et al. 2007; the 2009 findings addressed in this review were limited to properties not assessed in the 2007 report.

Table 4

Characteristics of the included instruments

InstrumentConstruct assessedaRec all periodDimensions (number of items)Number of subscalesResponse options (range)Ease of scoring and administration (Range of scores)Sample items
MAASDispositional MindfulnessNoneOne dimension Total (15)None6-point scale (1 = almost always to 6 = almost never)Easyb (1-6)“I find myself doing things without paying attention,” and “I do jobs or tasks automatically, without being aware of what I’m doing.”
KIMSMindfulness SkillsNoneObserve (12)45-point scale (1 = never or very rarely true to 6 = almost always or always true)Easy (Observe: 12-72, Describe: 8-48, Act with awareness: 10-60, Accept without judgment: 9-54)“I notice the smells and aromas of things” (Observe); “I’m good at finding the words to describe my feelings” (Describe); “When I do things, my mind wanders off and I’m easily distracted” (Act with Awareness); and “I tell myself that I shouldn’t be feeling the way I’m feeling” (Accept without Judgment).
Describe (8)
Act with awareness (10)
Accept without judgment (9)
Total (39)
FMIMindfulnessTime frame to be set by userPresent-moment disidentifying attention (12)None4-point scale (1 = almost never to 4 = almost always)Easy (30-120)“I am open to the experience of the present moment,” and “I perceive my feelings and emotions without having to react to them.”
Nonjudgmental, nonevaluative attitude toward self and others (7)
Openness to negative mind states (7)
Process-oriented, insightful understanding (4)
Total (30)
CAMS-RMindfulnessNoneAttention (3)None4-point scale (1 = rarely/not at all to 4 = almost always)Easy (12-48)“It is easy for me to concentrate on what I am doing,” and “I am able to focus on the present moment.”
Present-focus (3)
Awareness (3)
Acceptance/non-judgment (3)
Total (12)
SMQMindfulnessNoneOne dimension Total (16)None7-point scale (0 = strongly disagree to 6 = strongly agree)Easy (0-96)All items start with “Usually when I experience distressing thoughts and images...” and items include, “I am able just to notice them without reacting,” and “I am able to accept the experience.”
FFMQMindfulnessNoneObserving (8)55-point scale (1 = never or very rarely true to 5 = very often or always true)Easy (Observing: 8-40, Describing: 8-40, Acting with awareness: 8-40, Nonjudging of experience: 8-40, Nonreactivity to experience: 7-35)“I pay attention to sensations such as the wind in my hair or sun on my face” (Observing); “I have trouble thinking of the right words to express how I feel about things” (Describing); “I rush through activities without being really attentive to them” (Acting with awareness); “I make judgments about whether my thoughts are good or bad” (Nonjudgjng of experience); and “I watch my feelings without getting lost in them” (Nonreactivity to experience).
Describing (8)
Acting with awareness (8)
Nonjudgjng of experience (8)
Nonreactivity to experience (7)
Total (39)
TMSMindfulnessRespondents are instructed to rate a 15 minute meditation experienceCuriosity (6)25-point scale (0 = not at all to 4 = very much)Moderatec (Curiosity: 0-24, Decentering: 0-28)“I was curious about my reactions to things,” (Curiosity) and “I was aware of my thoughts and feelings without over- identifying with them” (Decentering).
Decentering (7)
EQDecenteringNoneOne dimension Total (11)None5-point scale (1 = never to 5 = all the time)Easy (11-55)“I am better able to accept myself as I am,” and “I can observe unpleasant feelings without being drawn into them.”
MMSMindfulnessNoneNovelty seeking (6)47-point scale (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree)Easy (Novelty seeking: 6-42, Novelty producing: 6-42, Engagement: 5-35, Flexibility: 4-28)“I like to investigate things” (Novelty seeking); “I try to think of new ways of doing things” (Novelty producing); “I get involved in almost everything I do” (Engagement); and “I stay with the old tried and true ways of doing things” (Flexibility).
Novelty producing (6)
Engagement (5)
Flexibility (4)
Total (21)
PHLMSMindfulnessOne weekAwareness (10)25-point scale (1 = never to 5 = very often)Awareness: easy (10-50)“When I am startled, I notice what is going on inside my body,” (Awareness) and “There are things I try not to think about” (Acceptance).
Acceptance (10)Acceptance: easyd (10-50)
aHigher scores represent higher levels of construct.
bAverage of total summed items
cIt requires a delivery of 15 minute meditation period prior to administration.
dSum of the reversed scores

Table 5

Methodological quality of each study per measurement property and instrument

Internal consistencyReliabilityContent validityStructural validityHypotheses testingResponsiveness
MAAS
Brown and Ryan (2003)GoodcGoodcFairaGoodb
Brown and Ryan – substudy 1 (2003)Fairc
Brown and Ryan – substudy 2 (2003)Goodc
Brown and Ryan – substudy 3 (2003)GoodcGoodc
Brown and Ryan – substudy 4 (2003)GoodcGoodc
Brown and Ryan – substudy 5 (2003)FaircFairc
Carlson and Brown (2005)GoodcGoodcGoodc
Baer et al. (2006)GoodcGoodc
Zvolensky et al. (2006)Fairc
MacKillop and Anderson (2007)ExcellentcExcellentcGoodc
McCracken et al. (2007)GoodcGoodc
Vujanovic et al. (2007)Fairc
Argus and Thomson (2008)GoodcFairc
Cordon and Finney (2008)GoodcGoodcFairc
Frewen et al – substudy 1 (2008)GoodcPoorcGoodc
Frewen et al – substudy 2 (2008)FaircPoorcFairc
Herndon (2008)FaircFairc
Christopher et al. (2009)Fairc
Ghorbani et al. (2009)GoodcGoodcGoodc
McCracken and Thompson (2009)Goodc
Roemer et al. – substudy 1 (2009)GoodcGoodc
Roemer et al. – substudy 2 (2009)FaircFairc
Schmertz et al. (2009)GoodcFairc
Waters et al. (2009)Goodc
Christopher and Gilbert (2010)GoodcGoodc
Van Dam et al. (2010)ExcellentcFairc
Bernstein et al. (2011)Goodc
KIMS
Baer et al. – substudy 1 (2004)Faird
Baer et al. – substudy 2 (2004)GoodeGoode
Baer et al. – substudy 3 (2004)Faire
Baer et al. – substudy 4 (2004)Faire
Baer et al. – substudy 5 (2004)Goode
Baer et al. – substudy 6 (2004)Faire
Baer et al. (2006)GoodeGoode
McKee et al. (2007)Goode
Frewen et al – substudy 1 (2008)GoodePooreFaire
Frewen et al – substudy 2 (2008)FairePooreFaire
Christopher et al. (2009)Faire
Vujanovic et al. (2009)Goode
Waters et al. (2009)Goode
Baum et al. (2010)Goode
Christopher and Gilbert (2010)GoodeGoode
Vujanovic et al. (2010)Goode
FMI
Buchheld et al. (2001)GoodFair
Leigh et al. (2005)GoodGoodFair
Baer et al. (2006)GoodGood
Walach et al. (2006)ExcellentGoodGood
CAMS-R
Baer et al. (2006)GoodGood
Feldman et al. – substudy 1 (2007)GoodGood
Feldman et al. – substudy 2 (2007)GoodGood
Schmertz et al. (2009)FairFair
SMQ
Baer et al. (2006)GoodGood
Chadwick et al. (2008)GoodGoodFair
FFMQ
Baer et al. (2006)GoodGoodGood
Baer et al. (2008)GoodGoodGood
Van Dam et al. (2009)PoorFair
Barns and Lynn (2010-2011)GoodFair
Cash and Whittingham (2010)Good
Emanuel et al. (2010)GoodFair
Fernandez et al. (2010)GoodGoodFair
Baer et al. (2011)Good
Hollis-Walker and Colosimo (2011)Good
Lavender et al. (2011)GoodGood
Christopher et al. (2012)GoodGoodFair
Fisak and von Lehe (2012)GoodGood
TMS
Lau et al. (2006)Poor
Lau et al. – substudy 1 (2006)GoodGoodGood
Lau et al. – substudy 2 (2006)GoodGood
Davis et al. (2009)GoodGood
EQ
Fresco et al. – substudy 1 (2007)GoodGood
Fresco et al. – substudy 2 (2007)Good
Fresco et al. – substudy 3 (2007)GoodGoodGood
MMS
Haigh et al. (2011)ExcellentExcellent
PHLMS
Cardaciotto et al. – substudy 1 (2008)Fairf
Cardaciotto et al. – substudy 2 (2008)Goodg
Cardaciotto et al. – substudy 3 (2008)GoodgGoodgGoodg
Cardaciotto et al. – substudy 4 (2008)FairgGoodg
Cardaciotto et al. – substudy 5 (2008)FairgFairg
Cardaciotto et al. – substudy 6 (2008)FairgGoodg

Note: The original COSMIN checklist was modified so that studies could receive an overall rating of good, if the only flaw noted was inadequate reporting of the methods for handling missing data.

aAssessed with a pool of 184 items,
bwith a set of 24 items,
cand with the final 15 items
dAssessed with a pool of 77 items,
eand with the final 39 items
fAssessed with a pool of 105 items,
gand with the final 20 items

Table 6

Quality of measurement properties per instrumenta

InstrumentInternal consistencyReliabilityContent validityStructural validityHypothesis testingResponsiveness
MAAS+++++?±++++
KIMS++++b??++cna
FMI+++nana?±na
CAMS-R++nana++++na
SMQ+++nana- -±na
FFMQ+++nana++++dna
TMS+++na?++++?e
EQ+++nana++++++na
MMS+++fnana- - -nana
PHLMS+++na?++±na
aThere was no evidence to evaluate measurement error for these instruments.
bTest-retest results were adequate for 3 of 4 subscales.
cRating for the global score is ++, ratings for subscales range from +++ for Accept without Judgment to − for Observe.
dRatings for subscales Observe and Describe are less often consistently positive than ratings for the other subscales.
eOne of the two subscales has been shown to be responsive.
fThis was evaluated with the whole MMS score only.

Mindfulness Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS)

The MAAS was the first widely-disseminated measure of mindfulness. It was designed to measure mindfulness as present-centered attention-awareness in everyday experience, a state which varies within and between persons, and an attribute that may be cultivated with practice [15]. This instrument focused on the absence of attention to and awareness of present experience, and was designed to operationalize mindfulness as a single construct. This instrument was intended to be generic, and applicable to persons regardless of experience with meditation. Sample items are shown in Table 4. Most studies confirmed a 1-factor structure for the MAAS [15,39-42]. One study found that some items in the MAAS did not function well as indicators for a single latent construct [43]. There was support for the internal consistency of the MAAS (Cronbach alphas ranging from 0.78 to 0.92) and evidence of test-retest reliability (ICC = 0.81). Correlations between the MAAS and other mindfulness instruments, such as the FMI, CAMS-R, SMQ, KIMS, and MMS, were weak to moderate (r’s = 0.14 to 0.51) [15,44,45]. Consistent with expectations for construct validity, MAAS scores were positively correlated with measures of openness, internal state awareness, positive and pleasant affect, and well-being, and negatively correlated with neuroticism, anxiety, stress, and rumination [15,39,44-47]. MAAS scores were higher for meditators compared to non-meditators [43], but there was no significant difference between novice meditators and non-meditators [40]. Several studies [15,48,49] have compared the MAAS to results on performance-based tasks (e.g., cognitive tests of attention, inhibition) with mixed results.

Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills (KIMS)

The KIMS was designed to assess the tendency to be mindful in daily life in areas corresponding to the skills taught in mindfulness interventions, particularly Dialectical Behavior Therapy [50]. The KIMS consists of 39 items grouped into four subscales: Observe, Describe, Act with Awareness, and Accept without Judgment. The Observe subscale reflects the skill of observing or paying attention to internal (bodily sensations, thoughts and emotions) and external phenomena. The Describe subscale refers to a tendency or ability to put sensations, perceptions, thoughts, feelings, emotions, or experiences into words. The Act with Awareness subscale reflects the ability to focus undivided attention on the present. The Accept without Judgment subscale includes both the act of making judgments and common examples of self-criticism. The 4-factor structure of the KIMS was supported by exploratory factor analysis (EFA); 43% of the variance was accounted for by the 4-factors [50]. Although nearly adequate fit was shown in confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), the analyses used a somewhat controversial “parceling approach” to overcome CFA sample size limitations, and others were unable to replicate the 4-factor solution by EFA [41]. The KIMS (global and subscales) had evidence of internal consistency (Cronbach alphas ranging from 0.72 to 0.97), and test-retest reliability was adequate (r’s ranging from 0.81 to 0.86) for all but the Observe subscale (r = 0.65) [50]. The construct validity of the KIMS global score was supported by moderate correlations (r’s ranging from 0.51 to 0.67) with the MAAS, FMI and CAMS-R and positive correlations with meditation experience [44]. Consistent with expectations for convergent and divergent validity, the global KIMS had positive correlations with openness, emotional intelligence, and self-compassion, and negative correlations with psychological symptoms, neuroticism, alexithymia, dissociation, and absent-mindedness [44]. KIMS subscales had different levels of evidence to support their construct validity. Accept without Judgment has consistently been found to be the most robust subscale, with most a priori relationships with health and quality of life measures confirmed [45,47,50-52]. There was also moderate evidence of the construct validity of the Act with Awareness subscale. Evidence to support the construct validity of the Describe subscale was limited, and relationships with the Observe subscale have been unpredictable. For example, the Observe subscale did not differ between adults with borderline personality disorder and normative student samples [50]. The developers acknowledged limitations in the content coverage of the KIMS, and concerns about integration of the subscales to provide a meaningful global score.

Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory (FMI)

The FMI was originally developed and validated in German, and English translations of FMI items have been incorporated into more recently developed mindfulness instruments [44]. Buddhist psychology guided development of the FMI and its intended target audience was individuals with some knowledge about or familiarity with insight meditation. The FMI was designed to assess mindfulness as “attentional, unbiased observation of any phenomenon in order to perceive and to experience how it truly is, absent of emotional or intellectual distortion” [53]. The developers cited the hallmark of mindfulness as dispassionate, non-manipulative participant observation of ongoing mental states without conceptualizing or forming emotional reactions. EFA identified 4 factors for the FMI, however the structure was not stable across samples and items cross-loaded, which the authors interpreted as support for a single underlying factor [53]. This original 4-factor structure was only approximately replicated in a subsequent study [54], and these authors also favored interpreting the FMI as one general factor reflecting mindfulness. There was evidence to support the internal consistency of the global FMI (Cronbach alphas ranged from 0.80 to 0.94). The FMI had weak to moderate correlations with the MAAS, KIMS, CAMS-R, and SMQ (r’s = 0.31 to 0.60) [44]. As expected, the FMI was positively correlated with openness, self-compassion, and self-knowledge, and negatively correlated with psychological symptoms, neuroticism, difficulties in emotion regulation, alexithymia, dissociation, and distress [44,54]. However, there was an unexpected positive relationship between FMI scores and smoking/frequent binge-drinking among undergraduate college students, suggesting that the FMI may not be valid when completed by persons without some familiarity or experience with insight meditation [55]. This review pooled the findings from the original German version of the FMI [53,54] with those of its English translation [44,55] because of the importance of the FMI as the first insight meditation-inspired self-report measure of mindfulness published.

Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale-Revised (CAMS-R)

The CAMS-R was designed to measure mindfulness in a brief, jargon-free, and conceptually comprehensive way, with the intention that it would be a generic measure appropriate regardless of meditation experience. Based on Kabat-Zinn’s definition [56], “awareness that emerges through paying attention on purpose, in the present moment, and non-judgmentally to the unfolding of experience moment to moment,” the authors conceptualized mindfulness as having four aspects: attention, present-focus, awareness, and acceptance/non-judgment [57]. Factor analyses provided moderate evidence of the predicted four aspects reflecting an overarching construct of mindfulness [57]. There was evidence of the internal consistency of the CAMS-R (Cronbach alphas ranging from 0.61 to 0.81). The CAMS-R had moderate correlations with other measures of mindfulness, including MAAS, FMI, KIMS, and SMQ (r’s = 0.51 to 0.67) [44,57]. Construct validity was supported by positive relationships with measures of adaptive regulation, openness, and well-being, and negative relationships with neuroticism, difficulties in emotion regulation, dissociation, and stagnant deliberation [44]. The CAMS-R, and not the original CAMS, was included in this review, because the developers determined that the CAMS was seriously flawed, and do not support its use [57].

Southampton Mindfulness Questionnaire (SMQ)

The SMQ was designed to assess awareness of distressing thoughts and images defined as a concept consisting of four related constructs: awareness of cognitions as mental events in wider context, allowing attention to remain with difficult conditions, accepting such difficult thoughts and oneself without judging, and letting difficult cognitions pass without reactions such as rumination [58]. Although factor analysis suggested a single factor structure for the SMQ, a single-factor solution explained less than 50% of the variance [58]. There was evidence of the internal consistency of the SMQ (Cronbach alphas ranging from 0.82 to 0.89). Correlations between the SMQ and other measures of mindfulness varied from weak to moderate (r’s = 0.38 to 0.61) [44,58]. Consistent with expectations, the SMQ correlated positively with emotional intelligence and self-compassion, and negatively with neuroticism, difficulties in emotion regulation, alexithymia, dissociation, and negative affect [44,58]. SMQ scores were higher in meditators compared to non-meditators, and in non-clinical samples compared to patients with psychosis [58].

Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ)

The FFMQ was derived from factor analysis of the combined item pool from five independently developed mindfulness instruments: MAAS, KIMS, FMI, CAMS-R, and SMQ [44]. The FFMQ has four facets similar to those of the KIMS (Observing, Describing, Acting with Awareness, and Nonjudging of inner experience) and one more facet comprised of items from the FMI and SMQ (Nonreactivity to inner experience). The authors found that the relationship between the facets and an overarching construct of mindfulness differed based on meditation experience, and that associations with symptoms and other constructs differed by facet. Therefore, they suggested use of the individual subscales may be preferred to use of the total FFMQ score. A 5-factor structure for the FFMQ was suggested by EFA [44] and confirmed by good or acceptable fit indexes in CFA using the same parceling approach for CFA employed in developing the KIMS [50,59]. A recent, standard item-level CFA supported the original 5-factor structure and an over-arching mindfulness factor [60]. Others have shown a modest fit for this structure [61], and hierarchical models that supported only four factors (all but Observe) as facets of an overarching mindfulness construct in student samples [44]. Internal consistency of the FFMQ is adequate with Cronbach alphas for the five subscales ranging from 0.67 to 0.93. Construct validity for the global FFMQ and its subscales has been evidenced by positive correlations with openness, emotional intelligence, self-compassion, and well-being, and negative correlations with neuroticism, depression, anxiety, alexithymia, and dissociation [44,62-66]. Meditators scored higher on the FFMQ than non-meditating students, and meditation history was correlated with a total FFMQ score in meditating samples (r = 0.52) [67]. The FFMQ Observe and Describe subscales were derived largely from the KIMS, and as with the KIMS, relationships with these subscales were less robust and predictable than those with other facets. For example, significant differences in Observe and Describe were not found between high- and low-worry groups [66]. There was little or no evidence for differential item functioning (DIF) between meditators and non-meditators matched for age [68], although the developers previously found that the structure of the FFMQ, particularly with respect to the Observe facet, differed between meditators and non-meditators [44].

Toronto Mindfulness Scale (TMS)

The TMS was designed to assess mindfulness as a “quality maintained when attention is intentionally cultivated with an open, non-judgmental orientation to experience” [69]. The original TMS measures mindfulness as a state-like quality, and not as a trait. The administration of the TMS requires that a brief mindfulness exercise precede self-administration of the instrument, and the TMS items assess the quality of that experience. The TMS is composed of two subscales, Curiosity and Decentering, and a total TMS score is not reported. EFA suggested a 2-factor structure for the TMS, and this was supported by CFA [69]. The TMS had evidence of internal consistency with Cronbach alphas ranging from 0.86 to 0.91, and 0.85 to 0.87 for Curiosity and Decentering, respectively. Correlations for the Decentering subscale with most of the other measures of mindfulness, including MAAS, FMI, CAMS-R, SMQ, KIMS subscales, and FFMQ subscales (r’s = 0.20 to 0.74) were stronger than the correlations between the Curiosity subscale and these measures (r’s = 0.10 to 0.54) [70]. Curiosity and Decentering were positively correlated with absorption, awareness of surroundings, reflective self-awareness, and psychological mindedness. As hypothesized, only Curiosity was correlated with awareness of internal states and self-consciousness (r = 0.41 and 0.31), and only Decentering was correlated with openness and cognitive failures (r = 0.23 and -0.16) [69]. Curiosity and Decentering scores were higher in meditators than non-meditators, and scores for the Decentering subscale were shown to increase with meditation experience [70]. Changes in Decentering were associated with changes in symptoms and stress [69].

Experiences Questionnaire (EQ)

The EQ was designed to measure decentering, a construct described as the ability to adopt a wider perspective where one’s thoughts are viewed as separate from oneself, and not necessarily an objective reflection of reality [71]. Decentering is posited to be a major outcome of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy and a mechanism that enables patients to be resilient to depressive thoughts. The authors did not view decentering as synonymous with mindfulness, but closely related or a component of mindfulness. The EQ was originally designed to have items reflecting decentering and rumination; however, the structure was determined to be unifactorial for the construct of decentering [71]. The EQ had evidence of internal consistency (Cronbach alphas ranging from 0.83 to 0.90), and construct validity was supported by positive correlations with cognitive appraisal (r = 0.25), and negative correlations with experiential avoidance, brooding rumination, emotional suppression, current depression, and anxiety symptoms (|r|’s = 0.31 to 0.49) [71]. Patients with depression had lower levels of decentering compared to healthy controls [71].

Mindfulness/Mindlessness Scale (MMS)

The MMS was designed to assess mindfulness from a cognitive-information processing framework as active awareness of and engagement with the environment [72]. Its Western cognitive derivation distinguishes the MMS from the other measures presented in this review. The MMS is composed of four subscales: Novelty Seeking, Engagement, Novelty Producing, and Flexibility. The 4-factor structure has not been supported, and a 2-factor structure explaining 34% of the variance has been reported [72]. Evidence of internal consistency was positive for the MMS as a single scale with Cronbach alphas ranging from 0.81 to 0.86. Cronbach alphas for the MMS subscales ranged from 0.45 to 0.77. There was mixed evidence regarding the relationships between MMS items and measures of mood.

Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale (PHLMS)

The PHLMS was designed to assess mindfulness defined as “the tendency to be highly aware of one’s internal and external experiences in the context of an accepting, nonjudgmental stance toward those experiences” [73]. This definition was operationalized as two constructs: Awareness - a behavioral tendency of continuously monitoring current experience, and Acceptance - a stance of experiencing events, including cognitions, without judgments and reactions such as interpretation, elaboration or avoidance. The subscales were shown to be uncorrelated, and use of a total PHLMS score is not recommended. A 2-factor structure for the PHLMS was supported by CFA [73]. Internal consistency was also supported with Cronbach alphas ranging from 0.75 to 0.86, and 0.75 to 0.91 for Awareness and Acceptance, respectively. Evidence of construct validity was mixed [73]. For example, the Awareness subscale was strongly correlated with the KIMS Observe subscale (r = 0.83) and the Acceptance subscale was strongly correlated with the KIMS Accept without Judgment subscale (r = 0.79) [73]. However, the correlation between the Awareness subscale and MAAS was weak (r = 0.21) for student samples and moderate (r = 0.40) for psychiatry outpatients. The correlation between the Acceptance subscale and MAAS was also weak (r = 0.32) for the normative student samples. As expected, student samples scored higher on both PHLMS subscales than psychiatry outpatients, and students scored higher on the Acceptance subscale compared to the inpatients with eating disorders (EDs). However, Awareness scores were not significantly different between students and inpatients with EDs.

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to systematically assess and compare the properties of instruments to measure self-reported mindfulness in adults. A comprehensive search strategy identified a total of 2,588 potentially relevant articles. Out of this pool, 46 articles reporting 79 unique studies, met the inclusion criteria for review. Ten instruments quantifying mindfulness as a unidimensional scale (n=5) or as a set of 2 to 5 subscales (n=5) were found. The COSMIN checklist was used to evaluate the methodological quality of each study for six properties in the COSMIN taxonomy: internal consistency, reliability, content validity, structural validity, hypothesis testing and responsiveness. We had initially planned to address measurement error, but no study evaluated this measurement property. The methodological quality of the studies included in this review was mostly good (66%) or fair (26%) across all properties. The majority of the studies were conducted with college undergraduates (48 out of 79 studies). No instrument had evidence to support content validity or adequacy of measurement error. The MAAS was the most frequently evaluated instrument followed by the KIMS. The MAAS was supported by positive evidence for internal consistency, reliability, construct validity by hypothesis testing, and responsiveness. The KIMS was supported by strong evidence for internal consistency, moderate evidence for construct validity by hypothesis testing, and limited evidence for reliability, but the other measurement properties were indeterminate or not available. The results shown in Table 6 provide limited guidance for instrument selection. The MAAS, KIMS, CAMS-R, FFMQ, TMS, EQ and PHLMS were found to have moderate or strong positive results for two or more properties; these measures may be preferred on psychometric grounds over the other instruments. Final instrument selection must consider other factors including the conceptual definition, completion time and target population. Moreover, as described below, there are areas where all the instruments are lacking; therefore caution is advised in using these results.

Descriptive critiques of mindfulness instruments have identified key problems including: 1) important differences in conceptual definitions of mindfulness; 2) no confirmation of respondent understanding of items; 3) absence of investigation of the potential discrepancies between self-reports and external referents (e.g., indicators of mindfulness experimentally tested or observed by others); and 4) conflation of the effects of learning the language of mindfulness or valuing mindfulness with actual increases in mindfulness per se [24,25]. To a great extent these problems are direct consequences of inadequate content validation. As documented by this systematic review, there was no engagement with members of a target population for item development and pre-testing. Cognitive interviews or focus groups to evaluate understanding and relevance to the target population or comprehensiveness of the items for the construct of mindfulness were not conducted. Neither was there any exploration of potential “response shift” in understanding of the construct of mindfulness following meditation training [74]. Moreover, the lack of diversity among the samples used in psychometric testing severely restricted the capacity of developers to detect potentially important differences among persons. It is unknown if items have very different semantic interpretations depending on the respondent’s characteristics, e.g., health status, age, race. Conceptual differences and lack of content validity were evidenced by weak to modest correlations among these measures of mindfulness, and among similarly titled mindfulness subscales. These gaps are consistent with a general lack of empirical studies comparing the psychometric performances of competing patient-reported outcome (PRO) measures within a complementary and alternative medicine (CAM) setting [75]. As no degree of superlative performance on other psychometric properties can compensate for poor content validity [76], none of the measures evaluated can be strongly recommended as a PRO at this time.

It is not clear as to which mindfulness instrument represents all the essential aspects of mindfulness. Some facets or dimensions of mindfulness may be more tractable to self-reports, and facets vary in their relationships with clinically relevant outcomes. “Summing up” purported facets of mindfulness as often done with the KIMS or FFMQ is likely to be problematic since some individuals who appear to possess higher levels of mindfulness could actually have a “toxic” combination of mental behaviors, such as being highly aware and very judgmental [16,25,77]. Although cogent arguments have been made for the utility of a brief, all-inclusive measure of mindfulness (e.g., CAMS-R) for clinical use, others have urged that instruments address specific sub-domains and be re-titled to better reflect their contents, and avoid having a multiplicity of instruments with very different content all claiming to measure mindfulness [25,78].

Utility of Mindfulness Scales

There is a surprising lack of information to guide users of these instruments. Few instruments had information on test-retest reliability or responsiveness, and none provided evidence of the adequacy of measurement error or estimated a minimally important difference. Floor or ceiling effects, rates of missing data, average completion time, and skewness of distributions were mentioned rarely or not at all. For the instruments with subscales, additional guidance regarding whether or not subscales should or should not be combined and how those scores should be labeled (e.g., total or global) and reported (e.g., mean or sum) would promote consistency and facilitate comparisons across studies.

Use of COSMIN and Quality Criteria

The COSMIN checklist is a useful guide, but has shortcomings. First, benchmarks for sample size are not helpful for CFA, since they are based on number of items and not number of parameters to estimate, and do not account for approaches such as bundling items into parcels to overcome sample size limitations. We also found it necessary to better define thresholds for adequate fit of CFA. These are listed in the footnote to Table 1. Second, COSMIN weights reporting and handling of missing data very heavily. Studies that do not provide clear information about rates of missing data and explain how missing data were handled are rated as having no more than fair methodological quality on missing data items. We initially followed this guideline and it resulted in 71% of all studies receiving overall fair ratings for the property. We felt this under-represented the overall quality of the studies in this review. We therefore used a modified guideline to allow an overall rating of good, if the only flaw noted was inadequate reporting of missing data. These are the ratings shown in Table 5. Nevertheless, inadequate reporting and handling of missing data is problematic [79], and developers should be strongly encouraged to report rates of missing data and use robust methods for imputation.

It would be timely to update the quality criteria to assess measurement properties. These criteria derive from a 2007 paper by Terwee et al. [37], rely heavily upon classical test theory, and lack sufficient guidance for integrating findings from item response theory (IRT) into its quality ratings. For example, is an instrument to be down-graded for construct validity if several of its items are shown to have differential item functioning (DIF), and if so, how many items with DIF would result in a downgrade?

There are limitations to this study. First, only one investigator conducted the first stage review of the over 2,500 titles and abstracts identified by our search strategy. To verify the completeness of the initial selection, we relied upon our search of the references of the selected articles and investigation of citations for the selected instruments through the Web of Science. Second, selection bias may be introduced by including only studies published in English. We initially excluded all mindfulness instruments not developed in English, and then changed our criteria to include the German language FMI because of its importance to the field. We have noted where psychometric findings from the German and English versions have been pooled. Short forms of these mindfulness instruments were not included [80-82]. We cannot recommend use of any short form where the longer version lacks evidence of content validity; reducing the number of items will not overcome this serious flaw. Translated instruments were also not included. These instruments warrant a separate review to adequately address issues of meaning and cross-cultural validity.

In conclusion, self-reports of mindfulness have the potential to be an important means of assessing the mechanisms and outcomes of mindfulness-based therapies. There is a great need to establish the content validity of the extant measures of mindfulness using qualitative methods such as semi structured interviews and focus groups with novice and experienced meditators, diverse populations, and clinical populations with acute and chronic illnesses. Further explication of the construct of mindfulness, its facets and consequences, and pre-testing of items with diverse target populations to ensure comprehensiveness of content coverage, clarity, and relevance are needed. Items prone to bias from learning the language of mindfulness and recognizing its value should be eliminated. It is timely to devise external referents to validate these self-reports. External referents may take the form of neuropsychological or other performance tests, evaluations by third-parties, such as teachers, spouses, or other family members, biomarkers or imaging studies. Several of the brief, royalty-free tests in the cognitive domain of the newly developed NIH Toolbox for Assessment of Neurological and Behavioral Function (www.nihtoolbox.org) may be useful external referents for mindfulness. Content validation should take precedence over efforts to optimize reliability and create short forms. Researchers using current mindfulness instruments are encouraged to report frequencies of skipped items to aid in identifying poor items for clinical samples, and estimate test-retest reliability, responsiveness, adequacy of measurement error, and minimally important differences. Use of mindfulness-based interventions continues to grow, with target populations and use of novel technologies for training rapidly expanding. Research to establish the best approaches for mindfulness training and target those most likely to benefit will be facilitated by valid and reliable self-reported mindfulness instruments.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported in part by National Institutes of Health, National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney diseases grant P01 DK 13083.

APPENDIX 1. Full search strategy

Ovid Medline®, CINAHL®, and PsycINFO® were searched using the following search string:

  1. mindful* OR vipassana OR zen meditation OR insight mediation OR theravada OR Buddhist meditation

  2. research measurement OR questionnaire* OR scale* OR instrument* OR methods OR outcome assessment OR outcome measure OR psychometr* OR reliab* OR valid* OR internal consistency OR (cronbach* AND (alpha OR alphas)) OR (item AND (correlation* OR selection* OR reduction*)) OR (intraclass AND correlation*) OR interscale correlation* OR agreement OR stability OR generaliza* OR concordance OR variability OR kappa OR kappa’s OR factor analysis OR factor analyses OR factor structure OR dimension OR subscale* OR standard error of measurement OR test-retest OR (test AND retest) OR sensitiv* OR responsive* OR reproducib* OR repeatab* OR replica* OR ((minimal OR minimally OR clinical OR clinically) AND (important OR significant OR detectable) AND (change OR difference)) OR interpretab* OR item response OR IRT OR Rasch OR differential item functioning OR ceiling effect* OR floor effect*

  3. 1 and 2

  4. limit to English language only.

References

1. Baer RA. Mindfulness-based treatment approaches: clinician’s guide to evidence base and applications. Massachusetts: Academic Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
2. Shennan C, Payne S, Fenlon D. What is the evidence for the use of mindfulness-based interventions in cancer care? A review. Psycho-Oncology. 2011;20(7):681–697. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
3. Bohlmeijer E, Prenger R, Taal E, Cuijpers P. The effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction therapy on mental health of adults with a chronic medical disease: A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2010;68(6):539–544. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
4. Hofmann SG, Sawyer AT, Witt AA, Oh D. The effect of mindfulness-based therapy on anxiety and depression: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2010;78(2):169–183. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
5. Gross CR, Kreitzer MJ, Thomas W, Reilly-Spong M, Cramer-Bornemann M, Nyman JA, et al. Mindfulness-based stress reduction for solid organ transplant recipients: A randomized controlled trial. Alternative Therapies in Health and Medicine. 2010;16(5):30–38. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
6. Gross CR, Kreitzer MJ, Reilly-Spong M, Wall M, Winbush NY, Patterson R, et al. Mindfulness-based stress reduction versus pharmacotherapy for chronic primary insomnia: A randomized controlled clinical trial. Explore: The Journal of Science & Healing. 2011;7(2):76–87. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
7. Chiesa A, Serretti A. Mindfulness based cognitive therapy for psychiatric disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research. 2011;187(3):441–453. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
8. Fjorback LO, Arendt M, Ornbol E, Fink P, Walach H. Mindfulness-based stress reduction and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy: A systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2011;124(2):102–119. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
9. Chiesa A, Serretti A. Mindfulness-based stress reduction for stress management in healthy people: A review and meta-analysis. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine. 2009;15(5):593–600. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
10. Krasner MS, Epstein RM, Beckman H, Suchman AL, Chapman B, Mooney CJ, et al. Association of an educational program in mindful communication with burnout, empathy, and attitudes among primary care physicians. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2009;302(12):1284–1293. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
11. Rosenzweig S, Reibel DK, Greeson JM, Edman JS, Jasser SA, McMearty KD, et al. Mindfulness-based stress reduction is associated with improved glycemic control in type 2 diabetes mellitus: A pilot study. Alternative Therapies in Health and Medicine. 2007;13(5):36–38. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
12. Gregg JA, Callaghan GM, Hayes SC, Glenn-Lawson JL. Improving diabetes self-management through acceptance, mindfulness, and values: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2007;75(2):336–343. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
13. Davidson RJ, Kabat-Zinn J, Schumacher J, Rosenkranz M, Muller D, Santorelli SF, et al. Alterations in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness meditation. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2003;65(4):564–570. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
14. Kabat-Zinn J, Wheeler E, Light T, Skillings A, Scharf MJ, Cropley TG, et al. Influence of a mindfulness meditation-based stress reduction intervention on rates of skin clearing in patients with moderate to severe psoriasis undergoing phototherapy (UVB) and photochemotherapy (PUVA) Psychosomatic Medicine. 1998;60(5):625–632. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
15. Brown KW, Ryan RM. The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003;84(4):822–848. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
16. Mikulas WL. Mindfulness: Significant common confusions. Mindfulness. 2011;2(1):1–7. [Google Scholar]
17. Bauer-Wu S. Leaves falling gently: living fully with serious and life-limiting illness through mindfulness, compassion, and connectedness. California: New Harbinger; 2011. [Google Scholar]
18. Bishop SR, Lau M, Shapiro S, Carlson L, Anderson ND, Carmody J, et al. Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 2004;11(3):230–241. [Google Scholar]
19. Coffey KA, Hartman M, Fredrickson BL. Deconstructing mindfulness and constructing mental health: understanding mindfulness and its mechanisms of action. Mindfulness. 2010;1(4):235–253. [Google Scholar]
20. Goldin PR, Gross JJ. Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) on emotion regulation in social anxiety disorder. Emotion. 2010;10(1):83–91. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
21. Hölzel BK, Lazar SW, Gard T, Schuman-Olivier Z, Vago DR, Ott U. How does mindfulness meditation work? Proposing mechanisms of action from a conceptual and neural perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2011;6(6):537–559. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
22. Chiesa A, Malinowski P. Mindfulness-based approaches: Are they all the same? Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2011;67(4):404–424. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
23. Sauer S, Lynch S, Walach H, Kohls N. Dialectics of mindfulness: Implications for western medicine. Philosophy, Ethics, and Humanities in Medicine. 2011;6(10):1–7. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
24. Grossman P. On measuring mindfulness in psychosomatic and psychological research. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2008;64(4):405–408. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
25. Grossman P. Defining mindfulness by how poorly I think I pay attention during everyday awareness and other intractable problems for psychology’s (re)invention of mindfulness: comment on Brown et al. (2011) Psychological Assessment. 2011;23(4):1034–1040. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
26. Mokkink LB, Terwee CB, Patrick DL, Alonso J, Stratford PW, Knol DL, et al. The COSMIN checklist for assessing the methodological quality of studies on measurement properties of health status measurement instruments: An international Delphi study. Quality of Life Research. 2010;19(4):539–549. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
27. Framson C, Kristal AR, Schenk JM, Littman AJ, Zeliadt S, Benitez D. Development and validation of the Mindful Eating Questionnaire. Journal of the American Dietetic Association. 2009;109(8):1439–1444. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
28. Tharaldsen KB, Bru E. Validation of the Mindful Coping Scale. Emotional and Behavioural Difficulties. 2011;16(1):87–103. [Google Scholar]
29. Reavley N, Pallant JF. Development of a scale to assess the meditation experience. Personality and Individual Differences. 2009;47(6):547–552. [Google Scholar]
30. Solloway SG, Fisher WP. Mindfulness practice: A Rasch variable construct innovation. Journal of Applied Measurement. 2007;8(4):359–372. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
31. Neff KD. The development and validation of a scale to measure self-compassion. Self & Identity. 2003;2223(3) [Google Scholar]
32. Chawla N, Collin S, Bowen S, Hsu S, Grow J, Douglass A, et al. The Mindfulness-based Relapse Prevention Adherence and Competence Scale: Development, interrater reliability, and validity. Psychotherapy Research : Journal of the Society for Psychotherapy Research. 2010;20(4):388–397. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
33. De Vet HCW, Terwee CB, Mokkink LB, Knol DL. Measurement in Medicine. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
34. Mokkink LB, Terwee CB, Patrick DL, Alonso J, Stratford PW, Knol DL, et al. The COSMIN study reached international consensus on taxonomy, terminology, and definitions of measurement properties for health-related patient-reported outcomes. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 2010;63(7):737–745. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
35. Terwee CB, Roorda LD, Knol DL, de Boer MR, de Vet HC. Linking measurement error to minimal important change of patient-reported outcomes. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 2009;62(10):1062–1067. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
36. Terwee CB, Mokkink LB, Knol DL, Ostelo RW, Bouter LM, de Vet HC. Rating the methodological quality in systematic reviews of studies on measurement properties: A scoring system for the COSMIN checklist. Quality of Life Research. 2012;21(4):651–657. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
37. Terwee CB, Bot SD, de Boer MR, van der Windt DA, Knol DL, Dekker J, et al. Quality criteria were proposed for measurement properties of health status questionnaires. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 2007;60(1):34–42. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
38. van Tulder M, Furlan A, Bombardier C, Bouter L Editorial Board of the Cochrane Collaboration Back Review Group. Updated method guidelines for systematic reviews in the Cochrane Collaboration Back Review Group. Spine. 2003;28(12):1290–1299. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
39. Carlson LE, Brown KW. Validation of the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale in a cancer population. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2005;58(1):29–33. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
40. MacKillop J, Anderson EJ. Further psychometric validation of the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS) Journal of Psychopathology & Behavioral Assessment. 2007;29(4):289–293. [Google Scholar]
41. Christopher MS, Charoensuk S, Gilbert BD, Neary TJ, Pearce KL. Mindfulness in Thailand and the United States: A case of apples versus oranges? Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2009;65(6):590–612. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
42. Van Dam NT, Earleywine M, Borders A. Measuring mindfulness? An item response theory analysis of the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale. Personality & Individual Differences. 2010;49(7):805–810. [Google Scholar]
43. Cordon SL, Finney SJ. Measurement invariance of the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale across adult attachment style. Measurement & Evaluation in Counseling & Development. 2008;40(4):228–245. [Google Scholar]
44. Baer RA, Smith GT, Hopkins J, Krietemeyer J, Toney L. Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment. 2006;13(1):27–45. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
45. Christopher MS, Gilbert BD. Incremental validity of components of mindfulness in the prediction of satisfaction with life and depression. Current Psychology. 2010;29(1):10–23. [Google Scholar]
46. Ghorbani N, Watson P, Weathington B. Mindfulness in Iran and the United States: Cross-cultural structural complexity and parallel relationships with psychological adjustment. Current Psychology. 2009;28(4):211–224. [Google Scholar]
47. Frewen P, Evans E, Maraj N, Dozois D, Partridge K. Letting go: Mindfulness and negative automatic thinking. Cognitive Therapy & Research. 2008;32(6):758–774. [Google Scholar]
48. Waters AJ, Reitzel LR, Cinciripini P, Li Y, Marcus MT, Vidrine JI, et al. Associations between mindfulness and implicit cognition and self-reported affect. Substance Abuse. 2009;30(4):328–337. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
49. Schmertz SK, Anderson PL, Robins DL. The relation between self-report mindfulness and performance on tasks of sustained attention. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2009;31(1):60–66. [Google Scholar]
50. Baer RA, Smith GT, Allen KB. Assessment of mindfulness by self-report: The Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills. Assessment. 2004;11(3):191–206. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
51. Vujanovic AA, Youngwirth NE, Johnson KA, Zvolensky MJ. Mindfulness-based acceptance and posttraumatic stress symptoms among trauma-exposed adults without axis I psychopathology. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2009;23(2):297–303. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
52. Vujanovic AA, Bonn-Miller MO, Bernstein A, McKee LG, Zvolensky MJ. Incremental validity of mindfulness skills in relation to emotional dysregulation among a young adult community sample. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy. 2010;39(3):203–213. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
53. Buchheld N, Grossman P, Walach H. Measuring mindfulness in insight meditation (Vipassana) and meditation-based psychotherapy: The development of the Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory (FMI) Journal of Meditation and Meditation Research. 2001;1(1):11–34. [Google Scholar]
54. Walach H, Buchheld N, Buttenmüller V, Kleinknecht N, Schmidt S. Measuring mindfulness—the Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory (FMI) Personality & Individual Differences. 2006;40(8):1543–1555. [Google Scholar]
55. Leigh J, Bowen S, Marlatt GA. Spirituality, mindfulness and substance abuse. Addictive Behaviors. 2005;30(7):1335–1341. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
56. Kabat-Zinn J. Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present, and future. Clinical Psychology: Science & Practice. 2003;10(2):144–156. [Google Scholar]
57. Feldman G, Hayes A, Kumar S, Greeson J, Laurenceau J. Mindfulness and emotion regulation: The development and initial validation of the Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale-Revised (CAMS-R) Journal of Psychopathology & Behavioral Assessment. 2007;29(3):177–190. [Google Scholar]
58. Chadwick P, Hember M, Symes J, Peters E, Kuipers E, Dagnan D. Responding mindfully to unpleasant thoughts and images: Reliability and validity of the Southampton Mindfulness questionnaire (SMQ) The British Journal of Clinical Psychology / the British Psychological Society. 2008;47(Pt 4):451–455. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
59. Baer RA, Smith GT, Lykins E, Button D, Krietemeyer J, Sauer S, et al. Construct validity of the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire in meditating and nonmeditating samples. Assessment. 2008;15(3):329–342. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
60. Christopher MS, Neuser NJ, Michael PG, Baitmangalkar A. Exploring the psychometric properties of the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire. Mindfulness. 2012;3(2):124–131. [Google Scholar]
61. Fernandez AC, Wood MD, Stein LA, Rossi JS. Measuring mindfulness and examining its relationship with alcohol use and negative consequences. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2010;24(4):608–616. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
62. Hollis-Walker L, Colosimo K. Mindfulness, self-compassion, and happiness in non-meditators: A theoretical and empirical examination. Personality & Individual Differences. 2011;50(2):222–227. [Google Scholar]
63. Barnes SM, Lynn SJ. Mindfulness skills and depressive symptoms: A longitudinal study. Imagination, Cognition and Personality. 2010;30(1):77–91. [Google Scholar]
64. Cash M, Whittingham K. What facets of mindfulness contribute to psychological well-being and depressive, anxious, and stress-related symptomatology? Mindfulness. 2010;1(3):177–182. [Google Scholar]
65. Lavender JM, Gratz KL, Tull MT. Exploring the relationship between facets of mindfulness and eating pathology in women. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy. 2011;40(3):174–182. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
66. Fisak B, von Lehe AC. The relation between the Five Facets of Mindfulness and Worry in a Non-clinical Sample. Mindfulness. 2012;3(1):15–21. [Google Scholar]
67. Van Dam NT, Earleywine M, Danoff-Burg S. Differential item function across meditators and non-meditators on the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire. Personality & Individual Differences. 2009;47(5):516–521. [Google Scholar]
68. Baer RA, Samuel DB, Lykins EL. Differential item functioning on the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire is minimal in demographically matched meditators and nonmeditators. Assessment. 2011;18(1):3–10. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
69. Lau MA, Bishop SR, Segal ZV, Buis T, Anderson ND, Carlson L, et al. The Toronto Mindfulness Scale: Development and validation. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2006;62(12):1445–1467. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
70. Davis KM, Lau MA, Cairns DR. Development and preliminary validation of a trait version of the Toronto Mindfulness Scale. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy. 2009;23(3):185–197. [Google Scholar]
71. Fresco DM, Moore MT, van Dulmen MH, Segal ZV, Ma SH, Teasdale JD, et al. Initial psychometric properties of the experiences questionnaire: Validation of a self-report measure of decentering. Behavior Therapy. 2007;38(3):234–246. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
72. Haigh EA, Moore MT, Kashdan TB, Fresco DM. Examination of the factor structure and concurrent validity of the Langer Mindfulness/Mindlessness Scale. Assessment. 2011;18(1):11–26. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
73. Cardaciotto L, Herbert JD, Forman EM, Moitra E, Farrow V. The assessment of present-moment awareness and acceptance: The Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale. Assessment. 2008;15(2):204–223. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
74. Schwartz CE, Sprangers MA. Methodological approaches for assessing response shift in longitudinal health-related quality-of-life research. Social Science & Medicine (1982) 1999;48(11):1531–1548. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
75. Eton DT, Bauer BA, Sood A, Yost KJ, Sloan JA. Patient-reported outcomes in studies of complementary and alternative medicine: Problems, solutions, and future directions. Explore (NY) 2011;7(5):314–319. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
76. US Department of Health and Human Services Food and Drug Administration. US Department of Health and Human Services Food and Drug Administration; 2009. [11 November 2012]. Guidance for industry: Patient-reported outcome measure: Use in medical product development to support labeling claims. http://www.fda.gov/downloads/Drugs/Guidances/UCM193282.pdf. [Google Scholar]
77. Sauer S, Walach H, Schmidt S, Hinterberger T, Lynch S, Büssing A, et al. Assessment of mindfulness: Review on state of the art. Mindfulness. 2012:1–15. [Google Scholar]
78. Grossman P, Van Dam NT. Mindfulness, by any other name…: trials and tribulations of sati in western psychology and science. Contemporary Buddhism. 2011;12(1):219–239. [Google Scholar]
79. Schlomer GL, Bauman S, Card NA. Best practices for missing data management in counseling psychology. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2010;57(1):1–10. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
80. Bohlmeijer E, ten Klooster PM, Fledderus M, Veehof M, Baer R. Psychometric properties of the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire in depressed adults and development of a short form. Assessment. 2011;18(3):308–320. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
81. Skinner TC, Roberton T, Allison GT, Dunlop S, Bucks RS. Experiential avoidance, mindfulness and depression in spinal cord injuries: A preliminary study. Australian Journal of Rehabilitation Counselling. 2010;16(1):27–35. [Google Scholar]
82. Hofling V, Strohle G, Michalak J, Heidenreich T. A short version of the Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2011;67(6):639–645. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
83. Bentler PM. Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin. 1990;107(2):238–246. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
84. Brown MW, Cudeck R. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In: Bollen KA, Long JS, editors. Testing Structural Equation Models. California: Sage; 1993. pp. 136–162. [Google Scholar]
85. Kline RB. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York: The Guilford Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
86. Argus G, Thompson M. Perceived social problem solving, perfectionism, and mindful awareness in clinical depression: An exploratory study. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2008;32(6):745–757. [Google Scholar]
87. Baum C, Kuyken W, Bonus M, Heidenreich T, Michalak J, Steil R. The psychometric properties of the Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills in clinical populations. Assessment. 2010;17(2):220–229. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
88. Bernstein A, Tanay G, Vujanovic AA. Concurrent relations between mindful attention and awareness and psychopathology among trauma-exposed adults: Preliminary evidence of transdiagnostic resilience. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy. 2011;25(2):99–113. [Google Scholar]
89. Emanuel AS, Updegraff JA, Kalmbach DA, Ciesla JA. The role of mindfulness facets in affective forecasting. Personality & Individual Differences. 2010;49(7):815–818. [Google Scholar]
90. Herndon F. Testing mindfulness with perceptual and cognitive factors: External vs. internal encoding, and the cognitive failures questionnaire. Personality & Individual Differences. 2008;44(1):32–41. [Google Scholar]
91. McCracken LM, Gauntlett-Gilbert J, Vowles KE. The role of mindfulness in a contextual cognitive-behavioral analysis of chronic pain-related suffering and disability. Pain. 2007;131(1-2):63–69. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
92. McCracken LM, Thompson M. Components of mindfulness in patients with chronic pain. Journal of Psychopathology & Behavioral Assessment. 2009;31(2):75–82. [Google Scholar]
93. McKee L, Zvolensky MJ, Solomon SE, Bernstein A, Leen-Feldner E. Emotional-vulnerability and mindfulness: A preliminary test of associations among negative affectivity, anxiety sensitivity, and mindfulness skills. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy. 2007;36(2):91–101. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
94. Roemer L, Lee JK, Salters-Pedneault K, Erisman SM, Orsillo SM, Mennin DS. Mindfulness and emotion regulation difficulties in generalized anxiety disorder: Preliminary evidence for independent and overlapping contributions. Behavior Therapy. 2009;40(2):142–154. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
95. Vujanovic AA, Zvolensky MJ, Bernstein A, Feldner MT, McLeish AC. A test of the interactive effects of anxiety sensitivity and mindfulness in the prediction of anxious arousal, agoraphobic cognitions, and body vigilance. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2007;45(6):1393–1400. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
96. Zvolensky MJ, Solomon SE, McLeish AC, Cassidy D, Bernstein A, Bowman CJ, et al. Incremental validity of mindfulness-based attention in relation to the concurrent prediction of anxiety and depressive symptomatology and perceptions of health. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy. 2006;35(3):148–158. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]

Citations & impact 


Impact metrics

Jump to Citations

Citations of article over time

Alternative metrics

Altmetric item for https://www.altmetric.com/details/1364479
Altmetric
Discover the attention surrounding your research
https://www.altmetric.com/details/1364479

Article citations


Go to all (98) article citations

Similar Articles 


To arrive at the top five similar articles we use a word-weighted algorithm to compare words from the Title and Abstract of each citation.

Funding 


Funders who supported this work.

NIDDK NIH HHS (2)