Abstract
Free full text
Acceptance and Commitment Therapy and Contextual Behavioral Science: Examining the Progress of a Distinctive Model of Behavioral and Cognitive Therapy
Abstract
A number of recent authors have compared acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) and traditional cognitive behavior therapy (CBT). The present article describes ACT as a distinct and unified model of behavior change, linked to a specific strategy of scientific development, which we term “contextual behavioral science.” We outline the empirical progress of ACT and describe its distinctive development strategy. A contextual behavioral science approach is an inductive attempt to build more adequate psychological systems based on philosophical clarity; the development of basic principles and theories; the development of applied theories linked to basic ones; techniques and components linked to these processes and principles; measurement of theoretically key processes; an emphasis on mediation and moderation in the analysis of applied impact; an interest in effectiveness, dissemination, and training; empirical testing of the research program across a broad range of areas and levels of analysis; and the creation of a more effective scientific and clinical community. We argue that this is a reasonable approach, focused on long-term progress, and that in broad terms it seems to be working. ACT is not hostile to traditional CBT, and is not directly buoyed by whatever weaknesses traditional CBT may have. ACT should be measured at least in part against its own goals as specified by its own developmental strategy.
All scientific theories are ultimately shown to be incorrect. Thus, the point of the scientific journey is not to generate correct ideas but to develop more useful half-truths whose limitations can be more quickly and certainly known. A progressive scientific field builds on useful ideas, continuously weeding out those that are not. It is impossible to know whether a more progressive field has been accomplished by focusing only on the present. Progressivity unfolds over time, sometimes slowly.
There is a tension between an urge for immediate progress and the willingness to take the careful steps that might create progress in the long run. That tension is felt especially strongly in applied areas because human suffering is present now but the generation of scientific knowledge often takes an unpredictable amount of time. In comparison to their most art-focused colleagues, empirical clinicians are used to arguing for the ultimately greater progressivity of an empirical approach, but in the cognitive behavior therapy (CBT) tradition relatively little has been written about how to produce greater progress within an empirical approach. There, too, what seems to be fastest now could be much slower later and what seems slower now might ultimately go farther.
The CBT Tradition and the Origins of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy
Acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT, said as one word, not initials; Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999) is sometimes placed outside of or opposed to CBT (e.g., Hofmann & Asmundson, 2008), but ACT is part of the larger family of behavioral and cognitive therapies (Forman & Herbert, 2009) and has always been said to be so (e.g., Hayes, Strosahl, et al., 1999, p. 79). ACT is an overarching model of key intervention and change processes, linked to a research program on the nature of language and cognition, to a pragmatic philosophy of science, and to a model of how to speed scientific development that we call here a contextual behavioral science (CBS) approach. Describing that approach is a primary purpose of the present paper.
The similarities and differences between ACT and the CBT mainstream needs to be seen in the context of respective views about how to create scientific progress. ACT researchers are skeptical of the idea that CBT needs to apply “the cognitive model of a particular disorder with the use of a variety of techniques designed to modify the dysfunctional beliefs and faulty information processing characteristic of each disorder” (Beck, 1993, p. 194) or that its core is to “identify distorted cognitions” and then to subject these distortions “to logical analysis and empirical hypothesis-testing which leads individuals to realign their thinking with reality” (Clark, 1995, p. 155), but that skepticism is a reflection of its process-focused development program. In the 1980s we conducted more than a dozen studies on the theories behind common CBT procedures, and found little or no support for these models (see Rosenfarb & Hayes, 1984, on cognitive reappraisal/self-statements for an example of these). We made early theoretical attempts to analyze cognitive therapy using behavioral principles (e.g., Zettle & Hayes, 1980, 1982) but our long-term interest was in extending a process-based behavioral approach and its underlying development strategy (see Zettle, 2005, for a history of ACT).
ACT: A CBS Approach
CBS is a principle-focused, inductive strategy of psychological system building that emphasizes developing interventions based on theoretical models tightly linked to basic principles that are themselves constantly upgraded and evaluated. The strategy has been abstracted and extended from traditional behavior analysis. Only an outline can be presented here because the issues it raises (e.g., induction vs. deduction, pragmatic vs. correspondence theories of truth, the nature of theory) are complex and controversial. It involves the integration and simultaneous development of multiple levels of a research program including philosophical assumptions, basic science, basic and applied theory, intervention development, treatment testing, and dissemination, all done dynamically and “horizontally.” While a more detailed breakdown is possible, we describe the approach in terms of nine characteristics (see Table 1), considering each in an abstract way and briefly describing the results so far inside ACT and relational frame theory (RFT).
Table 1
|
EXPLICATE PHILOSOPHICAL ASSUMPTIONS
Philosophy of science is the process of clarifying and taking responsibility for the assumptions necessary to do complex intellectual work. ACT is grounded in functional contextualism, a type of psychological pragmatism that extends Skinner’s radical behaviorism (Hayes, Hayes, Reese, & Sarbin, 1993) by adopting a functional approach to truth and meaning linked to the prediction and influence, with precision, scope, and depth, of whole organisms interacting in and with a context considered historically and situationally (Hayes, 1993). The core unit of analysis adopted is the act in context: the ongoing situated purposive action (Hayes, 1993; Pepper, 1942). All actions are considered to be whole events, having meaning only with reference to their context.
The truth criterion of contextualism is “successful working” toward one’s analytic goals (Hayes, 1993). Functional contextualists are disinterested in ontological claims (truth with a capital “T”) because that claim is always also an “act in context” (Skinner, 1974, p. 234), thus “ontology” always ultimately dissolves into pragmatic psychological epistemology. While functional contextualists assume the one (“real”) world, there may be many ways of successfully differentiating the world, depending on one’s goals. “Causality” is not taken to be in the world but is a way of speaking about how to behave effectively, in given contexts for given purposes.
Scientifically, this explains the environmentalism of contextualistic behavior analysts who seek the prediction and influence of behavior. “Influence” requires the specification of manipulable events, as only contextual variables can be manipulated directly (Hayes & Brownstein, 1986). Thus models that specify the relation of one psychological action to another (including thought-behavior or emotion-behavior relations) are viewed as inherently incomplete until they identify the contextual variables that would allow in principle for the goal of “influence” to be met (Biglan & Hayes, 1996). Thoughts may be related to particular emotional and overt behavioral events, but only in historical and situational contexts that give rise both to these thoughts and to their relation to subsequent emotions and actions. It is the italicized portion of that sentence that is most often missed in traditional cognitive models and it is a key clinical focus of ACT.
DEVELOP A BASIC ACCOUNT OF COMPLEX HUMAN BEHAVIOR WITH MANIPULABLE, CONTEXTUAL PRINCIPLES ORGANIZED NTO THEORIES
Manipulable contextual factors are specified by behavioral principles that apply in a specific way to a given event (precision), are broadly applicable (scope), and that maintain coherence across levels of analysis such as psychology and neurobiology (depth). A CBS approach goes beyond traditional behavior analysis by asking clinicians to help develop the basic work needed to support application, and by organizing principles into models and theories of domains relevant to application. This addresses two key flaws in the original model of behavior therapy development: what to do when basic principles are not adequate and how to scale them into functional analytic theories.
The change from behavior therapy to CBT reflected the right problem but not the best solution. An urge to move ahead quickly on the problem of cognition caused the search for more adequate basic behavioral principles in this area to be abandoned in favor of clinical models of cognition. We thought this was ultimately likely to slow progress. Conversely, our early theoretical focus was on basic behavioral research on rule governance (e.g., Hayes, Brownstein, Haas, & Greenway, 1986; see Hayes, 1989, for a book-length treatment), but the lack of a clear understanding of verbal rules soon led to an even more basic focus on the nature of human language and cognition itself. RFT (Hayes, Barnes-Holmes, & Roche, 2001) was the eventual result.
RFT researchers have discovered that the core of human language and cognition is learning to relate events mutually and in combination not simply on the basis of their formal properties (e.g., size, shape) but also on the basis of arbitrary cues. For example, whereas a young child will prefer a nickel over a dime because it is bigger, he or she later will prefer a dime over a nickel “because it is bigger.” Evidence suggests these relational skills are operant (e.g., Berens & Hayes, 2007), and that they impact all other behavioral processes, both operant and classical. For example, humans who are aroused by a stimulus that has been paired with shock will be even more aroused by a neutral stimulus related to the original stimulus in the presence of a “larger than” cue (Dougher, Hamilton, Fink, & Harrington, 2007).
Space precludes detailed citation (see Hayes et al., 2001; Rehfeldt & Barnes-Holmes, 2009, for book-length treatments) but RFT researchers have found that these “relational frames” begin in infancy, without them children do not develop normal language, weakness in relational framing is associated with significant cognitive deficits, some clinical disorders have specific framing deficits, and training in relational framing can increase language acquisition and higher-order skills such as perspective taking and empathy. Many complex cognitive phenomena, such as metaphorical reasoning, sense of self, lexical recognition, and implicit cognition can now be modeled and researched, both behaviorally and neurobiologically, in the RFT laboratory. RFT in turn is leading to applied programs in many areas. Althought ACT is the focus on this article, ACT is only one of the areas.
A key RFT insight of clinical importance is that relational framing is regulated by two distinguishable features: the relational context and the functional context. The relational context determines how and when events are related; the functional context determines what functions will be transformed in terms of a relational network. Stated more clinically, the relational context determines what you think; the functional context determines the psychological impact of what you think. Because relational frames are learned and arbitrarily applicable it is impossible to control the relational context so thoroughly so as to entirely keep unhelpful relations from being derived. For example, myriad arbitrary cues can lead children to derive that they are not as attractive, lovable, intelligent, or worthwhile as they should be. As with all learning, once relating occurs, it can be inhibited but will never be fully unlearned. Once a child derives “I am unlovable,” that behavior will always be at some strength. This is part of why it is hard to restructure cognitive networks and schemas fully, efficiently, and permanently.
It is the functional context that determines the impact of relational responding—an observation that is put to good use in ACT. In imagination, one can taste an orange, or notice that the word contains “range.” The impact of these two will be hugely different. Relational context interventions can also be functional context interventions and the two can easily conflict. For example, challenging the rationality of a thought can easily make a thought more central, noticed, or important. RFT provides guidance about how to balance these processes. For example, teaching a person to add “I am having the thought that …” to a self evaluation such as “I’m bad” is indeed designed to amplify the person’s cognitive network (it is a relational context intervention) but in a way that diminishes automatic and unhelpful cognitive control (it is also designed to be a helpful functional context intervention).
Clinical readers exposed to RFT will initially find little such guidance. Eyes glaze over, understanding lags, or what is understood seems obvious. That is not unlike the experience of clinicians reading unfamiliar basic behavior research of any kind. Solving that problem is dependent on the next element of a CBS approach.
DEVELOP A MODEL OF PATHOLOGY, INTERVENTION, AND HEALTH TIED TO BEHAVIORAL PRINCIPLES
Behavioral principles are difficult to scale directly into clinical work, and early bold attempts to do so (Kanfer & Saslow, 1969) were long ago put aside. An inadequate analysis of cognition was one source of the difficulty but the other was complexity. To all but a few, basic behavioral principles are too technical and abstract to give ready clinical guidance in many situations—it would be like asking people wanting to use a computer operating system to first understand the programming language used to build it.
The CBS solution to this conundrum is to develop clinically useful models of pathology and treatment based on middle-level terms that are not behavioral principles but are based on them. The ACT model is meant to be a kind of user-friendly interface—an operating system if you will—that stands atop a far more extensive enterprise. Terms like “defusion” are based on RFT concepts but do not demand that the clinician immediately understand basic principles in order to make applied use of them. Six key middle-level processes have been identified and organized into the model of pathology, intervention, and health shown in Figures 1 and and2.2. We will briefly consider each process.
Cognitive Fusion
Cognitive fusion refers to verbal dominance over behavioral regulation to the exclusion of other sources of stimulus control (“verbal” is meant technically here, i.e., “via relational frames”). Cognitive fusion is argued in RFT to be due to the pervasiveness of literal, reason-giving, problem-solving, and evaluative contexts sustained by natural language communities. Although fusion is not necessarily harmful, it becomes so when over-extended. People begin to take their thoughts literally, without noticing the process of thinking itself. Common examples of cognitive fusion include the excessive reliance on rules about what is possible for one’s life that are targeted by most forms of CBT.
Because learning is additive, not subtractive, RFT suggests that it is often safer to create more flexible responding by diminishing the excessive impact of cognitive events than trying to correct their content. Psychoeducation can be helpful when there is an absence of information, but in many clinical situations entanglement with thoughts is the more central issue. Deliberate attempts to alter negative cognitive content can paradoxically increase its functional importance. Given a change agenda, people tend toward suppressive tactics that expand difficult content (John & Gross, 2004; Wegner, 1994) even if that was decidedly not the goal of the clinician.
Defusion involves the creation of nonliteral, nonevaluative contexts that diminish the unnecessary regulatory functions of cognitive events, and increase contact with the ongoing process of relating as opposed merely to its products. Said in another way, the focus is on mindfully noticing thinking as it occurs. Some examples of defusion techniques include thanking one’s mind for a thought, watching thoughts go by as if they were written on leaves floating down a stream, repeating words out loud until only the sound remains, or giving thoughts a shape, size, and texture. Clients can practice labeling the process of thinking (e.g., “I am having the thought that I will never be successful”), and practice behaving in ways that directly contradict a thought (e.g., saying “I cannot walk” as one walks across the room). The goal is greater behavioral flexibility, not an immediate change in their frequency or form.
Experiential Avoidance
Experiential avoidance is the attempt to alter the form, frequency, or intensity of private experiences such as thoughts, feelings, bodily sensations, or memories, even when doing so is costly, ineffective, or unnecessary (Hayes, Wilson, Gifford, Follette, & Strosahl, 1996). According to RFT, humans target aversive private events for change in the same ways that external events are targeted due to an overextension of the problem-solving and evaluative functions of cognition. Some emotions or other private events are evaluated negatively and rules are constructed to keep them at bay. These avoidance or escape rules often contain stimuli that relate to the experiences to be altered, or to events that evoke them. For example, avoiding anxiety in order to avoid a shameful incapacity to function tends to elicit anxiety in response to the verbal construction of an inability to function. Distracting or soothing events become related to the avoided event, thus creating more opportunities for the aversive event to appear. Checking to see whether avoidance is effective necessitates cognitive contact with what is being avoided, thus evoking it. The avoidance behavior itself, through negative reinforcement, also strengthens the behavior regulatory effect of the avoided private event even if that prevents adaptive, values-based responses.
All complex organisms show avoidance learning, but in nonverbal organisms what is avoided are aversive stimuli, not reactions to them. Without bidirectional relational responding there is neither an evolutionary reason nor a robust means to avoid what follows the presentation of aversive stimuli, and emotional or other responses are in that category. Conversely, the relational nature of human language and cognition makes it possible to categorize, evaluate, and seek to avoid emotional responses themselves. Furthermore, arbitrary applicability means that virtually any situation can evoke aversive emotions, such as when a beautiful sunset makes a person sad because a person who passed away is not there to see it. The verbal nature of experiential avoidance greatly expands avoidance learning per se.
While there are some differences, experiential avoidance overlaps with several other concepts in the literature such as lack of distress tolerance (Brown, Lejuez, Kahler, & Strong, 2002), intolerance of uncertainty (Dugas, Freeston, & Ladouceur, 1997), and cognitive and emotional suppression (e.g., Wenzlaff & Wegner, 2000). As RFT explains, experiential avoidance often works in the short term to reduce some discomfort, but can have long-term negative effects. This idea is supported in the larger literature (Baumeister, Zell, & Tice, 2007; John & Gross, 2004; Pennebaker & Chung, 2007) and by numerous studies on experiential avoidance per se (see Chawla & Ostafin, 2007, for a recent review).
The alternative to experiential avoidance is acceptance: adoption of an intentionally open, receptive, and flexible posture with respect to moment-to-moment experience. Acceptance is not passive tolerance or resignation but an intentional behavior that alters the function of inner experiences from events to be avoided to a focus of interest, curiosity, and observation as part of living a valued life. Ironically, acceptance is one of the biggest functional changes possible, and often will ultimately change the form of emotional events themselves.
Loss of Flexible Contact With the Present
The contexts that promote fusion and experiential avoidance often also take an individual out of flexible contact with the present; what becomes central is being somewhere else, where difficult events are not occurring. ACT promotes attending to what is present in a focused, voluntary, and flexible fashion, linked to one’s values and purposes. This is accomplished by using language more as a tool to note and describe internal events, than to predict and judge them. A sense of self called “self as process” is actively encouraged; the defused, nonjudgmental, flexible, and ongoing noting of thoughts, feelings, and other private events when doing so is useful. The present-moment focus of ACT is seen in therapy itself, in which attention is brought to present-moment experience, bodily postures, tone of voice, and so on in a focused and flexible manner. The ability to move voluntarily from one domain of the present to another, or to persist in a focus when that is needed, is useful in bringing attention itself under purposive control and greatly expands what the present environment affords. Contemplative and mindfulness homework is often used to practice a different mode of mind that is less judgmental and problem solving and more curious, appreciative, flexible, and open.
Attachment to a Conceptualized Self
ACT seeks to undermine an attachment to a conceptualized self (our fused, evaluative stories about who we are) and to promote contact with a sense of self based on the “I/here/nowness” of conscious experience. It is usually termed “self as context” although other terms (“noticing self,” “observer perspective,” “transcendent sense of self”) have been used. When answering a series of questions about what one did (“I did this,” “I felt that,” “I saw this,” etc.) “I” does not refer to the content of the answers but to the perspective from which observations are made and in that way normal language development leads to a sense of self as a perspective. RFT researchers have found that a sense of locus derives from learning “deictic relational frames,” such as “I–you,” “here–there,” and “now–then,” and that these are central to the ability to take the perspective of others, feel empathy, or to communicate (Rehfeldt & Barnes-Holmes, 2009). This insight has led to new assessment and training methods for children who fail to develop perspective-taking skills (Rehfeldt, Dillen, Zionek, & Kowalchuk, 2007).
The limits of perspective taking cannot be consciously known (e.g., you cannot consciously note the limits of consciousness) and thus provide a transcendent, spiritual aspect to human experience. This idea was one of the seeds from which both ACT and RFT grew (Hayes, 1984). It is important in ACT in part because self-as-context interventions help clients see inner experiences as distinct from consciousness as such, and thus not necessarily a threat. This in turn undermines excessive rule control and increases psychological flexibility. Self as context is fostered in ACT by mindfulness exercises, metaphors, and experiential processes. For example, clients may be asked to imagine that they are older and to write a letter of advice back to the person struggling now, or they may engage in eyes-closed mindfulness activities where they are asked to look at difficult experiences and then to notice who is noticing.
Values Problems
Just as successful working in contextualism requires a stated goal in order to be applied as a truth criterion, when a pragmatic perspective is brought into clinical work it leads naturally to the clinical centrality of what clients most deeply want. The overarching goal of ACT is increasing the ability to persist or change in behavior in the service of one’s chosen values. Acceptance, defusion, being present, and so on are not ends in themselves; rather, they are meant to help clear the path for a more vital, values-consistent life. Values dignify these other processes and make them meaningful.
In ACT, values are defined as chosen, verbally constructed consequences of dynamic, evolving patterns of activity for which the predominant reinforcer becomes intrinsic to the behavioral pattern itself (Wilson & Dufrene, 2009). Appetitive rather than avoidant, values of this kind are never finished; they are more like a direction than a destination. Being a loving parent, for example, is not an outcome that can be obtained like a degree or a new boat. It is an ongoing, continuous process of supporting and encouraging children as they grow, rebel, take risks, and make mistakes. The moment-to-moment reinforcing consequences of parenting can even be painful and yet be appetitive. For example, being there psychologically as one’s child undergoes a serious medical operation can feel horrifyingly vulnerable, and yet be a sweet, loving, important, and meaningful thing to do.
The key problem areas in values work are a lack of values clarity (the failure to contact and specify appetitive consequences of importance), values based not on personal choice but on pliance (domination of rules followed to avoid social criticism or achieve social approval), and avoidant tracking (domination of rules followed to avoid or escape difficult feelings such as shame or guilt). Choices based on avoidance, social compliance, or fusion (e.g., “I should value X” or “A good person would value Y” or “My mother wants me to value Z”) are not helpful behaviorally (Sheldon & Elliot, 1999) because they do not lead to the flexibility characteristic of choices linked to positive consequences.
A variety of methods of values assessment and clarification have evolved in ACT (e.g., Wilson & Dufrene, 2009). These involve clinical methods of looking for vitality, meaning, and purpose, and sorting them out from pliance and avoidance. ACT uses exercises and journaling to help a client note and choose life directions in various domains (e.g., family, career, spirituality) and to track the consequences for engaging in value-directed behavior.
Inaction, Impulsivity, or Avoidant Persistence
Finally, ACT encourages the development of larger and larger patterns of effective action linked to chosen values, undermining inaction, impulsivity, or avoidant persistence. ACT is a modern form of behavior therapy—despite its focus on cognition it never left that theoretical wing—and committed action is where that link is most obvious. Unlike values, which are constantly instantiated but never achieved as an object, concrete goals that are values consistent can be achieved. ACT protocols almost always involve therapy work and homework linked to short-, medium-, and long-term behavior change goals.
Exposure, skills acquisition, shaping, goal setting, contingency management, and other behavior change procedures are all part of the ACT model and indeed of many ACT protocols. What is different is the functional set. Any behavioral technique, principle, or functional analysis can be applied—harnessed to the goals of the model. More than 20 years ago, for example, it was made clear that in ACT “Exposure work … is not designed to reduce anxiety. Instead, exposure gives people an opportunity to practice experiencing anxiety without also struggling with anxiety” (Hayes, 1987, p. 365; cf., Arch & Craske, 2008). Over time, the larger ACT model has impacted how we view behavioral procedures at a process level, allowing their more precise definition. For example, in ACT, exposure is viewed as organized contact with previously repertoire-narrowing stimuli in a context designed to produce greater psychological flexibility—thus providing a technical definition of a term long lacking one.
It is sometimes said that ACT is unconcerned with symptoms, but this is correct only in a specific sense. In line with its tradition, ACT eschewed the a priori scientific importance of syndromal analysis, seeking instead a bottom-up, inductive account of the way that verbal/cognitive processes interacted with traditional behavioral processes so as to produce psychological inflexibility. ACT addresses cognitive, emotional, and behavioral elements of psychological problems but only in that context. When presenting problems are discussed, it is often clear that client “symptoms” are functioning to keep clients from engaging in one or more valued domains such as spending time with family or going to work regularly. And as an empirical fact ACT reduces “symptoms” quite well (Hayes, Luoma, Bond, Masuda, & Lillis, 2006). But that impact is secondary to the primary focus on valued living.
Psychological Inflexibility
Psychological inflexibility and flexibility refers to patterns of behavior that are regulated by the six repertoire-narrowing or six repertoire-expanding processes specified in the ACT model. The goal of ACT is psychological flexibility: being able to contact the moment as a conscious human being more fully as it is, not as what it says it is, and based on what the situation affords, persisting or changing in behavior in the service of chosen values. That definition includes all six processes. While distinguishable, each of the six processes are understood more fully in the context of the others. The relation between each process in each direction is theoretically and practically meaningful. Acceptance seems to depend upon defusion, for example, because it is hard to embrace private events fully if they are as we evaluate them to be. So it is through all 30 relations among the six processes.
ACT Defined
The six processes—acceptance, defusion, the now, self, values, and committed action—can be further organized. The first four are acceptance and mindfulness processes; the last four are commitment and behavior change processes. Thus, an easy definition of ACT is a behavioral and cognitive intervention that uses acceptance and mindfulness processes, and commitment and behavior change processes, to produce psychological flexibility. Treatment attempts to build the acceptance and mindfulness processes that undermine excessive literality and create a more conscious, present, flexible approach to psychological experiences; it also attempts to strengthen the commitment and behavior change processes that enhance values-based action.
Because of its bottom-up, inductive nature, the ACT model is not a model of any specific type of disorder, nor of a set of techniques. One could say it is a model of how to do CBT or of therapy in general, but in an even more general sense it is meant as a model of how relational learning can interact with direct contingencies in human psychology.
ACT in Practice
Because of the nature of language itself, trying to undermine literality using literal language is difficult and can easily lead to more entanglement in the name of “understanding.” Therefore, ACT focuses heavily on experiential exercises in which clients are encouraged to come into contact with psychological processes more directly. Metaphors and stories allow ACT therapists to help clients learn to relate to their experiences in more flexible ways without creating a new set of rigid rules. Behavioral techniques dominate but are linked to fostering the processes in the ACT model.
In essence, the ACT model provides a functional dimensional diagnostic system (cf. Hayes et al., 1996; Wilson & Dufrene, 2009) inside a unified model of behavior. Each of the six processes, as supplemented by traditional functional analysis, and applied to the specific cognitive, behavioral, emotional, and social content, can be linked directly to intervention methods and clinical targets. At the level of the model, ACT is not a technology; it is a perspective into which a wide variety of technologies, some identified with ACT and some not, can be deployed in a coherent fashion linked to basic principles. Movement in the processes in the model is the functional goal and any techniques that move these processes can be part of an ACT intervention.
The therapeutic relationship in ACT is simply a social scaling of the model. It involves the therapists targeting these processes, from positive ACT processes (psychologically, for the therapist) and with positive ACT processes (Pierson & Hayes, 2007; Wilson & Dufrene, 2009). For example, in ACT the therapist targets acceptance from a posture of self-acceptance, and does so in an accepting way.
Summary
Although the number of concepts is few, they are new to some readers so it seems worth summarizing what has been said. From an ACT/RFT approach, psychopathology is caused in large part by the tendency to become entangled in cognition, taking thoughts literally and remaining in a problem-solving mode even when that is not helpful. The domination of verbal/cognitive processes over other sources of stimulus control is termed “cognitive fusion.” In part as a result, there is a tendency to avoid and escape from aversive private events, such as emotions, thoughts, memories, and bodily sensations, even when this creates behavioral harm. This is termed “experiential avoidance,” which is thought to enormously restrict behavioral flexibility and effectiveness. People lose contact with present-moment contingencies due to entanglement with a conceptualized past and future and resulting attentional inflexibility. They fail to stay in contact with a more transcendent sense of self, allowing behavioral patterns to be dominated by a conceptualized sense of self instead. All of these contribute to narrow and rigid behavioral patterns characterized by inaction, impulsivity, and avoidant persistence in specific domains, dominated by excessive social compliance and avoidance rather than chosen values. The coming together of all of these processes is termed “psychological inflexibility,” which is argued to be at the core of most human suffering. Cognitive, emotional, and behavioral inflexibility narrows the opportunities that are apparently present to move in a valued direction. Theoretically, it is argued that all six processes are related to each other in all directions, but specific life histories, organismic patterns, and current situations enhance or diminish these processes and their interrelationships. ACT views syndromes as loose collections of unknown utility and prefers to focus case conceptualization on these empirically supported processes within the model. The goal of ACT is not necessarily the regulation of emotional and cognitive content but flexibility: contacting the moment more fully as it is and persisting or changing in behavior in the service of chosen values.
BUILD AND TEST TECHNIQUES AND COMPONENTS LINKED TO PROCESSES AND PRINCIPLES
The theoretical model provides the conceptual scaffolding for creating and deploying treatment technologies and components. In a CBS approach treatment technologies and components are often examined in smaller, sometimes lab-based, studies to determine their efficacy in impacting relevant behavior and processes of change. This approach avoids the problem with large-scale dismantling studies, which are expensive and often delayed for many years, limiting their impact because they occur too late in the dissemination cycle. There are now scores of studies on ACT component methods, including those not done by ACT researchers but clearly applicable to these components. We will review only a small subset of this work—not to summarize it but to show how it relates to the development model.
Defusion
ACT researchers have found that word repetition (Hayes, Strosahl, et al., 1999, pp. 154–156) reduces both believability and subjective distress experienced from negative self-relevant phrases (Masuda, Hayes, Sackett, & Twohig, 2004). The “soldiers on parade” mindfulness exercise (Hayes, Strosahl, et al., 1999, pp. 158–160) reduces subjective distress from an intrusive thought-provoking task (Marcks & Woods, 2005) and willingness to engage in the task again (Marcks & Woods, 2007).
Acceptance
Research has shown that acceptance interventions alone and in combination with other ACT components increase persistence and willingness to engage in distressing tasks (e.g., Hayes, Bissett, et al., 1999; Levitt, Brown, Orsillo, & Barlow, 2004; McMullen et al., 2008; Takahashi, Muto, Tada, & Sugiyama, 2002) and produce lower reported distress (e.g., Gutiérrez, Luciano, Rodríguez, & Fink, 2004; Levitt et al., 2004) compared to inactive and emotion control/distraction conditions.
Self as Context
War veterans experiencing PTSD exposed to a full ACT protocol decreased significantly more in PTSD symptoms than when exposed to one without the self-as-context component (Williams, 2006).
The Present
Studies on training attentional flexibility (e.g., Langer & Moldoveanu, 2000) suggest that a flexible present-moment focus can be helpful. Asking participants to focus on sensations during painful stimulation increases task persistence (e.g., Cioffi & Holloway, 1993).
Values
In pain tolerance tasks, brief values interventions have been found to increase task persistence, without decreasing self-reports of pain, especially when high degrees of pain are encountered (Páez-Blarrina et al., 2008). Studies have also shown that values interventions increase task persistence when combined with other ACT components (e.g., Gutiérrez et al., 2004). Brief values writing exercises have been shown to increase school performance in stigmatized minority students (Cohen, Garcia, Apfel, & Master, 2006), reduce physiological stress in distressing tasks (Creswell et al., 2005), and improve reactions to health messages (Harris & Napper, 2005).
Committed Action
The literature on consistent application of behavioral methods is vast and hardly needs to be cited. For scientific reasons, ACT researchers have sometimes excluded behavioral methods so that the psychological impact of the other elements of an ACT model can be examined. For example, Twohig, Hayes, and Masuda (2006) and Twohig et al. (2010) found that the elements of an ACT model minus behavioral exposure were effective for obsessive-compulsive disorder. In essence this becomes a kind of dismantling study, testing a deliberately hobbled ACT intervention. While a few studies of this kind seem needed, it can plant a dangerous seed in which ACT is thought to be all of the elements in an ACT model except validated behavior change methods. That is incorrect, although some have criticized ACT researchers on that basis (e.g., Öst, 2008). To yield to this kind of criticism would peel ACT away from its own model. A better alternative is to test behavioral methods without any ACT elements compared to those with ACT elements. Studies of this kind have found ACT elements to be helpful (e.g., Eifert & Heffner, 2003; Levitt et al., 2004). Another is to examine the mediators of ACT when behavioral technology is used to see whether the packages work through ACT-sensible means. For instance, Lundgren, Dahl, and Hayes (2008) found that an ACT epilepsy protocol that included behavioral elements worked through changes in acceptance and values, a functional path that is very unlikely to be due to the behavioral element alone.
Psychological Flexibility
A great deal of research exists to support the core importance of flexible behavior, cognition, and emotion. For example, it is known that depressed individuals are less sensitive to emotional contexts and show less reactivity to both positive and negative emotional stimuli (Rottenberg, Gross, & Gotlib, 2005), and show behavioral rigidity in tasks that require adaptive skills (Hopkinson & Neuringer, 2003). Depressive symptoms and response to treatment are better predicted by flexibility of attributions than by the content of cognitive schemas (Moore & Fresco, 2007). Indeed, cognitive flexibility moderates the relationship between negative life events and depression, even after controlling for the influence of explanatory style and the interaction between explanatory style and negative life events (Moore & Fresco, 2007). Similarly, inflexible application of emotion regulation strategies is more determinative of effectiveness than are the strategies themselves (Bonanno, Papa, Lalande, Westphal, & Coifman, 2004). It is known that flexibility is related to other aspects of the ACT model. For example, emotionally avoidant and controlling coping strategies are more likely to be applied rigidly, independent of the current context (Folkman, Lazarus, Gruen, & DeLongis, 1986).
Another example of psychological flexibility is the desynchrony effect that frequently occurs in ACT interventions in which the link between private events (i.e., emotions and cognitions) and overt behavior is reduced. For example, acceptance and values interventions sometimes increase persistence in a distressing task even without reducing distress (Hayes, Bissett, et al., 1999; McMullen et al., 2008), suggesting these interventions may reduce the dominant control of distress over subsequent behavior and allow for more flexible responding in aversive contexts. Similarly, ACT for persons with psychosis reduces self-report of positive symptoms less than treatment as usual, but patients are rehospitalizated less—particularly those who admit to positive symptoms (Bach & Hayes, 2002)—suggesting less behavioral impact and more psychological flexibility in the presence of hallucinations and delusions.
MEASURE THEORETICAL PROCESSES AND THEIR RELATIONSHIPS TO PATHOLOGY AND HEALTH
The goal of functional contextualism is prediction and influence with precision, scope, and depth. Scope in particular requires good theory, not just good technology. Exploring processes of change allows one to test the theoretical model, providing another link between principles, theory, and treatment components/packages. Thus, a CBS approach focuses on developing adequate measures of the key processes thought to be involved in psychological difficulty and in psychological change and examines their relations to psychopathology and behavior. There needs to be tight links between theoretical constructs and the auxiliaries and conditions of measurement, so that empirical problems can be attributed to the theory rather than to characteristics of the measure (Hayes, 2004).
A number of process measures have been developed. The work is too broad to describe in detail here but a few highlights can be mentioned. The Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (AAQ; Hayes et al., 2004; see Bond et al., in press, for a review of the AAQ II) assesses experiential avoidance and psychological flexibility fairly broadly and does a very good job of predicting many forms of psychopathology, as is predicted by the model (Hayes et al., 2006). To detect process changes in targeted protocols problem-specific versions of the AAQ have been developed in such areas as smoking (Gifford et al., 2004), weight (Lillis & Hayes, 2008), psychosis (Shawyer et al., 2007), chronic pain (McCracken, Vowles, & Eccleston, 2004), epilepsy (Lundgren et al., 2008), and diabetes (Gregg, Callaghan, Hayes, & Glenn-Lawson, 2007). Believability ratings are commonly used to assess defusion (e.g., Varra, Hayes, Roget, & Fisher, 2008) and specific defusion questionnaires have emerged (Wicksell, Renofalt, Olsson, Bond, & Melin, 2008). Values measures are also beginning to appear (e.g., Lundgren et al., 2008; Wilson, Sandoz, & Kitchens, 2010). Mindfulness measures also effectively tap these processes (Baer, Smith, & Allen, 2004; Baer, Smith, Hopkins, Krietemeyer, & Toney, 2006).
EMPHASIZING MEDIATION AND MODERATION IN THE ANALYSIS OF APPLIED IMPACT
Exploring the relationship between processes of change and outcome in treatment studies through meditational analysis allows a further test of the treatment model beyond whether the treatment package can produce a positive impact per se. Mediation provides important information regarding the ability of a treatment package to target functionally important processes of change. If mediation fails it is important to determine where this occurred (Follette, 1995) because it matters whether the failure was due to failures of technology (i.e., the intervention did not impact the processes of change) or limitations in the model (i.e., the intervention did impact processes of change, but this did not account for changes in outcome).
Nearly two dozen formal mediational analyses of ACT now exist, including those that are analyzed and being written up but are not yet published or in press. Successful ACT mediators include general or specific measures of acceptance and psychological flexibility (e.g., Gifford et al., 2004; Gregg et al., 2007; Lappalainen et al., 2007; Lillis, Hayes, Bunting, & Masuda, 2009; Lundgren et al., 2008), defusion (e.g., Hayes et al., 2004; Lundgren et al., 2008; Varra et al., 2008; Zettle & Hayes, 1986), and values (e.g., Lundgren et al., 2008), among others. In virtually every case, when alternative mediators drawn from other perspectives were applied to ACT interventions they did not work or did not work as well as those drawn from ACT theory.
The quality of the evidence on mediation in ACT varies. For example, some studies show mediation using processes assessed before outcome differences are seen (e.g., Gifford et al., 2004; Lundgren et al., 2008; Zettle & Hayes, 1986, as reanalyzed in Hayes et al., 2006); in other cases the mediators were assessed concurrently with outcome. What is most noteworthy, however, is the consistency across the entire dataset: the small number of concepts specified by an ACT model work very consistently as mediators across the very wide range of problems targeted by ACT.
Comparing treatments at the level of processes of change is a useful approach in determining whether interventions are distinct from each other (O’Donohue & Yeater, 2003). The first two published ACT outcome studies involved a direct comparison of ACT to cognitive therapy (CT; Zettle & Hayes, 1986; Zettle & Raines, 1989). Believability of depressive thoughts mediated outcomes in ACT but not CT (Hayes et al., 2006; Zettle & Hayes, 1986); reductions in dysfunctional attitudes correlated with treatment effects for CT but not ACT (Zettle & Raines, 1989). Two recent effectiveness studies comparing traditional CBT and CT to ACT (Forman et al., 2007; Lappalainen et al., 2007) also both demonstrated different processes impacted by these approaches. Forman, Hoffman, et al. found that changes in the acceptance and acting with awareness subscales of the Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills (KIMS; Baer et al., 2004), as well as the AAQ, related to outcomes with ACT but not as well with CT, which instead related more to changes in the observing subscale of the KIMS. Lappalainen et al. found that CBT increased self-confidence more than acceptance at post, while the reverse was found for ACT. Both related to outcomes on the SCL-90 but if partial correlations with outcome were calculated for both, self-confidence no longer predicted outcome. Interestingly, at follow-up ACT participants now had significantly higher self-confidence than CBT participants.1
Several studies have also explored moderation in relation to the theoretical model. A study by Masuda et al. (2007) provides an example. It found ACT was equally effective in targeting stigma toward mental illness regardless of participants’ level of psychological flexibility, but that psychoeducation was less effective when individuals reported higher levels of psychological inflexibility. Differences in moderation have also been shown comparing ACT to traditional CBT. Forman, Hoffman, et al. (2007) compared the impact of a brief ACT intervention for food cravings to a traditional CBT model (drawn from Friedman & Brownell, 1996), and no treatment. Outcomes differed depending on an individual’s level of sensitivity to food in the environment. Individuals who reported few difficulties with food did worse with ACT, but individuals who reported high levels of difficulty did significantly better than either CBT or no treatment when exposed to ACT.
Mediation and moderation data are high tests of a CBS approach because they simultaneously examine the utility and coherence of the relationship between theory, technology, and outcomes. It is not important to CBS that ACT technology always be more successful than other approaches—indeed it has not been (e.g., Forman, Hoffman, et al., 2007). The failures so far seem theoretically sensible (e.g., it makes sense that acceptance will not help with food cravings if the person is not dominated by food to begin with) but a lot remains to be learned. The consistency of the evidence on the model, however, provides a target for the creativity of researchers and clinicians, who can then focus more on empirically supported processes than labels, packages, and manuals (Rosen & Davison, 2003).
EARLY AND CONTINUOUS TESTS OF EFFECTIVENESS, DISSEMINATION, AND TRAINING STRATEGIES
A CBS approach differs from stage-based treatment development models in which effectiveness and dissemination research is only conducted after multiple studies have tested the treatment. Contextual behavioral scientists are not trying to find out what is “true” in an ontological sense and then see whether that knowledge is useful. From a pragmatic perspective, the truth of a given analysis is found in its utility where it is applied. This suggests that effectiveness, training, and dissemination research should begin early and be emphasized throughout (Hayes, 2002). Knowledge development in such an approach is embedded and horizontal. Diverse populations and settings need to be a significant part of the process instead of passive recipients of knowledge developed elsewhere. Factors relevant to dissemination such as feasibility, cost-effectiveness, and acceptability of the treatment by both clinicians and their clients (Hayes, 1998) should be considered early, and a broad and diverse development community needs to be created.
ACT researchers have focused on effectiveness and training testing early in the development process. The first ACT study of the modern era was an effectiveness study (Strosahl, Hayes, Bergan, & Romano, 1998) that showed that training clinicians in ACT produced better overall outcomes in an outpatient setting. Since then two additional effectiveness studies have been conducted with ACT targeting heterogeneous clinical populations (Forman et al., 2007; Lappalainen et al., 2007) in addition to studies on training (e.g., Luoma et al., 2007).
TEST THE RESEARCH PROGRAM ACROSS A BROAD RANGE OF AREAS AND LEVELS OF ANALYSIS
The only way to test and further develop a putatively unified model is to apply it to topographically distinct populations understood in terms of the functional dimensional processes specified by the model. That needs to be done while varying everything that is not central conceptually (e.g., individual and group interventions, long and short interventions, in-person interventions vs. telephone, books, or the Internet). Such an approach to treatment testing may allow researchers to more readily identify the boundary conditions of the model in accounting for pathology and specifying treatment course and outcome, suggesting areas for further model and treatment development, especially if there is attention to mediation, moderation, and components, not just outcomes.
The breadth of ACT research is undeniable, and RFT is even broader. That breadth exists because of this strategic vision. A recent review (Hayes et al., 2006) identified controlled ACT studies on work stress, pain, smoking, anxiety, depression, diabetes management, substance use, stigma toward substance users in recovery, adjustment to cancer, epilepsy, coping with psychosis, borderline personality disorder, and trichotillomania, and comparisons that included cognitive therapy, behavior therapy, psychoeducation, attention placebo, pharmacotherapy, general treatment as usual, and wait-list. Overall the between-group effect size was d=.66 at post and d=.65 at follow-up. Three independent meta-analyses have arrived at broadly similar values (Öst, 2008; Powers, Vörding, & Emmelkamp, 2009; Pull, 2009). Since 2006, successful controlled studies have appeared in additional areas, including obsessive-compulsive disorder, marijuana dependence, skin picking, racial prejudice, prejudice toward people with mental health problems, whiplash-associated disorders, generalized anxiety disorder, chronic pediatric pain, weight-maintenance and self-stigma, clinicians’ adoption of evidence-based pharmacotherapy, and training clinicians in psychotherapy methods other than ACT. This process of expansion is bound to continue into prevention, organizations, schools, communities, religious bias, environmental issues, sexism, compassion, and the like. Interventions with even more challenging populations (e.g., those with IQs below 70, brain-injured clients, young children, prisoners) are currently being tested.
Whether the model succeeds or fails in these areas is an empirical matter—it is the strategy we are pointing to here. In some ways the whole point is to try to find the failures, so that further model and technological development can occur. The best way to do that is to push the model as far as it can go and to be prepared to change when deficiencies are contacted. Already, studies show that ACT may not work quite as well for minor problems and less entangled and avoidant clients (Forman, Herbert, et al., 2007; Zettle, 2003) and instead are leading to model-consistent methods to attempt to overcome this problem by, for example, emphasizing values and compassion more than struggle with personal pain.
Those outside of the behavior analytic tradition are commonly worried about the expansive goals of the ACT/RFT community, but it is fully consistent with Skinner’s (1948) original vision. It is not arrogance that led an animal learner to write a utopian novel like Walden Two (Skinner, 1948); rather, it is an explication of the vision and values of an inductive tradition that started with the study of rats and pigeons in order to understand human complexity. Walden Two is a goal, not a claim to knowledge. In the same way, the CBS tradition is committed, as its Web site notes, to the “creation of a psychology more adequate to the challenge of the human condition.” That is an aspiration, not a claim to knowledge.
CREATE AN OPEN, DIVERSE, AND NONHIERARCHICAL DEVELOPMENT COMMUNITY
ACT and RFT are being developed by an open, diverse, nonhierarchical worldwide community of clinicians, basic scientists, applied scientists, scholars, and students. The creation of a broad and diverse development community is a necessary feature of the CBS approach for three reasons. First, the inductive and yet broadly focused development strategy of a CBS approach can be quite slow. Only many hands can mount such an agenda. Secondly, in a CBS approach all ideas and methods are assumed to be contextually limited. Diversity and openness ensures that the range of ideas, settings, backgrounds, professions, and cultures brought to this development strategy is large and that blind spots can be more readily contacted, especially those held by developers and early explorers. Finally, community control over development avoids the problem faced by labeled and certified empirically supported treatments linked to scientific development strategies based on adherence to validated manuals, namely, that only developers or the anointed have the power to add or subtract elements. The hierarchy, self-focus, and ossification that may result could well slow long-term scientific progress. Given the distant and high aspirations of a CBS approach, the flexibility and temporal extension of communitarian control is necessary.
The link between ACT/RFT and a CBS development strategy is reflected by the consensus the development community reached several years ago about the name of its society. “ACT” or “RFT” is nowhere in it—instead, it is named the Association for Contextual Behavioral Science (ACBS: www.contextualpsychology.org). As of late 2011, ACBS had nearly 4,000 members, more than half outside of the United States.
It is one thing to create a community—it is another to keep it open and flexible. It is a value of the ACT/RFT development community to consciously limit hierarchy, isolation, and self-interest. For example, ACBS has eschewed certification of therapists. ACT trainers are “recognized” by a free process of peer review, and must sign a values statement in which protocols are made available for free or at low cost, and entanglement of ACT training with proprietary claims are prohibited. Whether protocols that comport with an ACT model are branded “ACT” is considered inside the CBS community to be a strategic decision, not a matter of fundamental importance, and various considerations have led well-known and respected CBS researchers to call their protocols by other names at times (e.g., McCracken, 2005). There is no attempt to restrict those decisions in presentations accepted at ACBS conventions.
The Scientific Development Strategy of Empirical Clinical Psychology
Empirical clinical psychology needs to attend more to its development strategy. In the usual approach, philosophical assumptions are not explicated, basic processes are all too often an afterthought, and if they exist at all theories are generally narrowly focused, vague, expressed in commonsense terms, and at times wholy untested. The idea seems to be that the Food and Drug Administration (FDA) style of medication validation can be used to build a progressive field based on manualized treatments focused on well-specified syndromes that are empirically validated in well-controlled studies. That seems unlikely and at the very least it needs to be defended. To our knowledge, no progressive science has ever been built in such a fashion.
The success of this plan requires that psychiatric syndromes lead to functional understood entities with known etiologies, that is, to diseases. Measured against that criterion it has been an abject failure. The American Psychiatric Association planning committee for the next edition of the DSM (Kupfer, First, & Regier, 2002) concluded “Reification of DSM-IV entities, to the point that they are considered to be equivalent to diseases, is more likely to obscure than to elucidate research findings…. All these limitations in the current diagnostic paradigm suggest that research exclusively focused on refining the DSM-defined syndromes may never be successful in uncovering their underlying etiologies. For that to happen, an as yet unknown paradigm shift may need to occur.” (p. xix). Without the functional simplification that is provided by the discovery of diseases, psychiatric syndromes can lead to an endless random walk through a wilderness of topographical correlations.
The same problem of incoherence occurs if all that is of concern is the validation of technology. Technological knowledge alone gives us no basis for dealing with a new problem or situation, no systematic means to develop new techniques, and no way to organize the field except under broad tribal labels such as “CBT” that are gradually becoming meaningless. It becomes “difficult to assimilate the mountain of seemingly disconnected bits of information that science-as-technology presents. The field becomes an incoherent mass, impossible to master and impossible to teach [and] the shallow level of analysis means that other areas of science cannot be related to clinical techniques” (Hayes, Strosahl, et al., 1999, p. 15).
The field needs a development strategy linked to pragmatically useful processes and theory, but a domination of empirically validated technology over progressive theory is deeply embedded in contemporary empirical clinical thinking. Junior researchers often are applauded (or at least funded) for mixing and matching techniques and syndromes like cooks in a kitchen, relabeling every combination as a model, approach, or treatment, provided only that in the end the technological stew is tested in the form of a randomized trial. The mountain of manuals and models that result is impossible to simplify or even to characterize. Acceptance and mindfulness techniques are now entering into new manuals at light speed, sometimes sitting side by side with seemingly contradictory methods, and without a well-crafted strategy in place for developing a real science out of this technological tangle.
Even serious scholars do not seem to understand the very different strategy being followed by the CBS community. For example, Öst (2008) criticized the lack of attention to syndromal diagnosis in the ACT literature, failing to note why this choice was made or to note the use of functional dimensional process information instead. He compared the ACT literature to a matched set of traditional CBT studies using a set of methodological standards drawn from a traditional model of development applied and argued that the ACT literature was weaker, but failed to note or explain the evidence that instead ACT studies were following a different model of scientific development. The evidence for that claim is in the very list of matched CBT studies he created. The CBT list of studies was 100% composed of studies on depression and anxiety, was funded well (77% with grants of more than $150,000), contained no mediational analyses, and provided almost no process information of any kind. Öst was not being baised: that is, indeed, where the traditional vision of scientific development inside CBT had led us, with large and expensive studies excessively focused on a relatively narrow set of problems but with very weak process knowledge and limited evidence of theoretical coherence and progress. In contrast, the ACT literature he reviewed was enormously broad (in seven different areas and only 38% in depression or anxiety), was underfunded (only 8% were based on grants of $150,000 or more), and yet almost every study contained good and supportive processes data, and the majority had successful mediational analyses published, in press, or in preparation focused on a handful of key processes of change that could nevertheless accommodate the enormous range of problems being addressed. As measured against its own criteria, which readers of the present article would now better understand, the early ACT literature is in many ways stronger than the traditional CBT literature Öst himself held up as a proper comparison, despite its minority status and relative lack of funding.
As another example of the lack of understanding regarding an ACT approach, in their meta-analysis of ACT Powers et al. (2009) entered disorder-specific distress as a primary outcome, even at times categorizing targeted behavioral outcomes as secondary. Although scientific politics and the dominance of mainstream measures may require ACT researchers to work both sides of that street, disorder-specific distress is generally a secondary concern compared to valued actions and life functioning in ACT. For example, the ACT model when applied to chronic pain is designed to move patients away from a focus on pain intensity toward a focus on its unnecessary interference with living. Nevertheless, Powers et al. entered pain intensity as a primary outcome of ACT studies on chronic pain in their meta-analysis and large effect sizes suddenly approached zero in some cases. The meta-anlysis was still reasonably supportive, but the failure to characterize evidence in an ACT-consistent way contributed to the incorrect conclusion that ACT was only as effective as any alternative active treatment (see Levin & Hayes, in press, for a re-analysis of their data set).
Due in part to the dominance of the FDA model, ACT is most often approached as a collection of techniques evaluated only in terms of outcomes. The comparisons that result are of limited interest. Although many of the techniques in ACT are relatively distinct, many are shared with other approaches, especially within CBT writ large but also with experiential, humanistic, and analytic traditions. The level of technique is a moving target and unless processes are understood, it is impossible to know when different techniques are truly different, and outcomes alone are not an adequate basis upon which to build a progressive science.
So far the vast majority of ACT critics have not explored ACT extensively, whether that is meant experientially or in the intellectual sense of a deep understanding of functional contextualism, RFT, and the CBS strategy of development. Various ACT methods are argued to be the same or different, but the grounds for the argument seem to be appearances, not well-crafted theory, data, or deep and broad exposure. Everyday language and commonsense understanding based on mainstream assumptions is too flexible for that exercise to work well. On such a basis, a sophisticated CBT scholar can argue that ACT is the same as cognitive therapy (Hofmann & Asmundson, 2008) and in the next breath argue that ACT is the same as an obscure Japanese treatment from the early part of the last century (Hofmann, 2008). Logically, that evidentiary basis would make cognitive therapy a form of Morita therapy, but the problem is not the failure to see that conclusion, it is trying to understand deep theoretical, philosophical, and strategic differences on the basis of loose verbal associations grounded in the hegemony of mainstream assumptions. A more interesting way to compare approaches, if comparisons are to be made, is to compare their assumptions, theories, evidence regarding processes of change, component evidence, and developmental strategy. We have cast the present article as we have because in the long run what seems most important are not empirically supported packages, techniques, or labels, and the monuments to immortality developers tend to build around them but empirically supported processes and theories (Rosen & Davison, 2003) that can be linked to a strategy for the creation of scientific and practical progress, and that can be given away to the field for the good of those we serve.
Conclusion
The scientific progress of empirical clinical psychology seems mixed—no one claims that either paradise or failure has been achieved. Some see the glass as half full and say that the dominant strategy is working; others see it half empty and lobby for strategic change. Frankly, that argument will not be decided by established researchers—it will be decided sociologically by junior researchers, theorists, and students choosing where to invest their life energy.
It seems clear that the field is changing with the ascendance of mindfulness and acceptance-based approaches and their core assumptions that differ so from traditional CBT assumptions. This is not an issue of label or school. The statement “unlike CBT, there is little emphasis in MBCT [mindfulness-based cognitive therapy] on changing the content of thoughts; rather, the emphasis is on changing awareness of and relationship to thoughts” (Segal, Teasdale, & Williams, 2004, p. 54) could have been stated by an ACT theorist without altering a single word other than the name of the therapy.
Generational changes within a field provide opportunities for advancement and renewal. These may be wasted, however, unless we think seriously about the scientific development strategies that are most likely to produce long-term progress. It will make little long-term difference to the field if we take our existing protocols, add a dash of mindfulness here, and a dollop of values there, test them, and gather them into a loose pile all under the tribal label of CBT. Both advocates of traditional CBT and of newer forms alike need to be much more clear about their own scientific development strategy and how it can best be evaluated. As we have tried to describe, ACT and RFT researchers have done so. The contextual behavioral science approach seems coherent, reasonable, and distinctive, and it has now yielded a body of work that is substantial enough for it to deserve to be considered on its own terms.
It would be unusual to evaluate an empirical clinical approach by examining the clarity of its philosophical assumptions; by the adequacy, progressivity, and coherence of its basic behavioral principles; by the integrity of its processes of change; by the coherence and general utility of its theory; and by the consistency of the link between all of these and successful outcomes. Nevertheless, from our perspective these issues seem far more important than the technological, informal, tribal, or brute-force empirical questions that seem to dominate in the dialogue so far.
In this paper we have pointed to signs that the ACT/RFT development strategy is succeeding in areas where success has not been common in applied psychology. But whether it succeeds or fails depends not one bit on the success or failure of traditional CBT or any other area of empirical clinical science and practice. ACT and traditional CBT are distinct models but they are part of the same family and they share the same opponent: the human suffering that exists because of scientific ignorance. Long-term scientific progress is the key to defeating such a difficult opponent, but that will take more than effort. It requires a strategy that works.
Footnotes
1This calculation was left out of the text of the published manuscript but it can be computed from the included table.
References
- Arch JJ, Craske MG. Acceptance and commitment therapy and cognitive behavioral therapy for anxiety disorders: Different treatments, similar mechanisms? Clinical Psychology, Science and Practice. 2008;15:263–279. [Google Scholar]
- Bach P, Hayes SC. The use of acceptance and commitment therapy to prevent the rehospitalization of psychotic patients: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2002;70:1129–1139. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Baer RA, Smith GT, Allen KB. Assessment of mindfulness by self-report: The Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills. Assessment. 2004;11:191–206. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Baer RA, Smith GT, Hopkins J, Krietemeyer J, Toney L. Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment. 2006;13:27–45. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Baumeister RF, Zell AL, Tice DM. How emotions facilitate and impair self-regulation. In: Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of emotion regulation. New York: Guilford Press; 2007. pp. 408–426. [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT. Cognitive therapy: Past, present, and future. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1993;61:194–198. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Berens NM, Hayes SC. Arbitrarily applicable comparative relations: Experimental evidence for a relational operant. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 2007;40:45–71. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Biglan A, Hayes SC. Should the behavioral sciences become more pragmatic? The case for functional contextualism in research on human behavior. Applied and Preventive Psychology: Current Scientific Perspectives. 1996;5:47–57. [Google Scholar]
- Bonanno GA, Papa A, Lalande K, Westphal M, Coifman K. The importance of being flexible: The ability to both enhance and suppress emotional expression predicts long-term adjustment. Psychological Science. 2004;15:482–487. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Bond FW, Hayes SC, Baer RA, Carpenter K, Orcutt HK, Waltz T, Zettle RD. Preliminary psychometric properties of the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II: A revised measure of psychological flexibility and acceptance. Behavior Therapy (in press) [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Brown RA, Lejuez CW, Kahler CW, Strong DR. Distress tolerance and duration of past smoking cessation attempts. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2002;111:180–185. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Chawla N, Ostafin B. Experiential avoidance as a functional dimensional approach to psychopathology: An empirical review. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2007;63:871–890. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Cioffi D, Holloway J. Delayed costs of suppressed pain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1993;64:274–282. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Clark DA. Perceived limitations of standard cognitive therapy: A reconsideration of efforts to revise Beck’s theory and therapy. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy. 1995;9:153–172. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen GL, Garcia J, Apfel N, Master A. Reducing the racial achievement gap: A social-psychological intervention. Science. 2006;313:1307–1310. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Creswell JD, Welch WT, Taylor SE, Sherman DK, Gruenewald TL, Mann T. Affirmation of personal values buffers neuroendocrine and psychological stress responses. Psychological Science. 2005;16:846–851. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Dougher MJ, Hamilton DA, Fink BC, Harrington J. Transformation of the discriminative and eliciting functions of generalized relational stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 2007;88:179–197. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Dugas MJ, Freeston MH, Ladouceur R. Intolerance of uncertainty and problem orientation in worry. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 1997;21:593–606. [Google Scholar]
- Eifert GH, Heffner M. The effects of acceptance versus control contexts on avoidance of panic-related symptoms. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 2003;34:293–312. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Folkman S, Lazarus RS, Gruen RJ, DeLongis A. Appraisal, coping, health status, and psychological symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986;50:571–579. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Follette WC. Correcting methodological weaknesses in the knowledge base used to derive practice standards. In: Hayes SC, Follette VM, Dawes RM, Grady KE, editors. Scientific standards of psychological practice: Issues and recommendations. Reno, NV: Context Press; 1995. pp. 229–247. [Google Scholar]
- Forman EM, Herbert JD. New directions in cognitive behavior therapy: Acceptance-based therapies. In: O’Donohue W, Fisher JE, editors. General principles and empirically supported techniques of cognitive behavior therapy. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2009. pp. 102–114. [Google Scholar]
- Forman EM, Herbert JD, Moitra E, Yeomans PD, Geller PA. A randomized controlled effectiveness trial of acceptance and commitment therapy and cognitive therapy for anxiety and depression. Behavior Modification. 2007;31:1–28. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Forman EM, Hoffman KL, McGrath KB, Herbert JD, Brandsma LL, Lowe MR. A comparison of acceptance- and control-based strategies for coping with food cravings: An analog study. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2007;45:2372–2386. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Friedman MA, Brownell KD. A comprehensive treatment manual for the management of obesity. In: Van Hasselt VB, Hersen M, editors. Sourcebook of psychological treatment manuals for adult disorders. New York: Plenum Press; 1996. pp. 375–422. [Google Scholar]
- Gifford EV, Kohlenberg BS, Hayes SC, Antonuccio DO, Piasecki MM, Rasmussen-Hall ML, Palm KM. Acceptance theory-based treatment for smoking cessation: An initial trial of acceptance and commitment therapy. Behavior Therapy. 2004;35:689–705. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Gregg JA, Callaghan GM, Hayes SC, Glenn-Lawson JL. Improving diabetes self-management through acceptance, mindfulness, and values: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2007;75:336–343. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Gutiérrez O, Luciano C, Rodríguez M, Fink BC. Comparison between an acceptance-based and a cognitive-control-based protocol for coping with pain. Behavior Therapy. 2004;35:767–784. [Google Scholar]
- Harris PR, Napper L. Self-affirmation and the biased processing of threatening health-risk information. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2005;31:1250–1263. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC. Making sense of spirituality. Behaviorism. 1984;12:99–110. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC. A contextual approach to therapeutic change. In: Jacobson N, editor. Psychotherapists in clinical practice: Cognitive and behavioral perspectives. New York: Guilford Press; 1987. pp. 327–387. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC. Rule-governed behavior: Cognition, contingencies, and instructional control. Reno, NV: Context Press; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC. Analytic goals and varieties of scientific contextualism. In: Hayes SC, Hayes LJ, Reese HW, Sarbin TR, editors. varieties of scientific contextualism. Reno, NV: Context Press; 1993. pp. 11–27. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC. Market-driven treatment development. The Behavior Therapist. 1998;21:32–33. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC. Getting to dissemination. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 2002;9:424–429. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC. Falsification and the protective belt surrounding entity postulating theories. Journal of Applied and Preventive Psychology. 2004;11:35–37. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC, Barnes-Holmes D, Roche B. Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum; 2001. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC, Bissett RT, Korn Z, Zettle RD, Rosenfarb IS, Cooper LD, et al. The impact of acceptance versus control rationales on pain tolerance. The Psychological Record. 1999;49:33–47. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC, Brownstein AJ. Mentalism, behavior-behavior relations and a behavior analytic view of the purposes of science. The Behavior Analyst. 1986;9:175–190. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC, Brownstein AJ, Haas JR, Greenway DE. Instructions, multiple schedules, and extinction: Distinguishing rule-governed from schedule controlled behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1986;46:137–147. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC, Hayes LP, Reese H, Sarbin TR. Varieties of scientific contextualism. Reno, NV: Context Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC, Luoma JB, Bond FW, Masuda A, Lillis J. Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: Model, processes and outcomes. Behavior Research and Therapy. 2006;44:1–25. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC, Strosahl K, Wilson KG. Acceptance and commitment therapy: An experiential approach to behavior change. New York: Guilford Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC, Strosahl K, Wilson KG, Bissett RT, Pistorello J, et al. Measuring experiential avoidance: A preliminary test of a working model. Psychological Record. 2004;54:553–578. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC, Wilson KG, Gifford EV, Follette VM, Strosahl K. Experiential avoidance and behavioral disorders: A functional dimensional approach to diagnosis and treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1996;64:1152–1168. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Hofmann SG. Acceptance and commitment therapy: New wave or Morita therapy? Clinical Psychology, Science and Practice. 2008;15:280–285. [Google Scholar]
- Hofmann SG, Asmundson GJG. Acceptance and mindfulness-based therapy: New wave or old hat? Clinical Psychology Review. 2008;28:1–16. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Hopkinson J, Neuringer A. Modifying behavioral variability in moderately depressed students. Behavior Modification. 2003;27:251–264. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- John OP, Gross JJ. Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: Personality processes, individual difference, and life span development. Journal of Personality. 2004;72:1301–1333. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Kanfer FH, Saslow G. Behavioral diagnosis. In: Franks CM, editor. Behavior therapy: Appraisal and status. New York: McGraw Hill; 1969. pp. 417–444. [Google Scholar]
- Kupfer DJ, First MB, Regier DA. A research agenda for DSM-V. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Langer EJ, Moldoveanu M. The construct of mindfulness. Journal of Social Issues. 2000;56:1–9. [Google Scholar]
- Lappalainen R, Lehtonen T, Skarp E, Taubert E, Ojanen M, Hayes SC. The impact of CBT and ACT models using psychology trainee therapists: A preliminary controlled effectiveness trial. Behavior Modification. 2007;31:488–511. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Levin M, Hayes SC. Is acceptance and commitment therapy superior to established treatment comparisons? Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics (in press) [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Levitt JT, Brown TA, Orsillo SM, Barlow DH. The effects of acceptance versus suppression of emotion on subjective and psychophysiological response to carbon dioxide challenge in patients with panic disorder. Behavior Therapy. 2004;35:747–766. [Google Scholar]
- Lillis J, Hayes SC. Measuring avoidance and inflexibility in weight related problems. International Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy. 2008;4:30–40. [Google Scholar]
- Lillis J, Hayes SC, Bunting K, Masuda A. Teaching acceptance and mindfulness to improve the lives of the obese: A preliminary test of a theoretical model. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2009;37:58–69. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Lundgren T, Dahl J, Hayes SC. Evaluation of mediators of change in the treatment of epilepsy with acceptance and commitment therapy. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2008;31:225–235. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Luoma JB, Hayes SC, Roget N, Fisher G, Padilla M, Bissett R, Twohig MP. Augmenting continuing education with psychologically-focused group consultation: Effects on adoption of group drug counseling. Psychotherapy Theory, Research, Practice, Training. 2007;44:463–469. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Marcks BA, Woods DW. A comparison of thought suppression to an acceptance-based technique in the management of personal intrusive thoughts: A controlled evaluation. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2005;43:433–445. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Marcks BA, Woods DW. Role of thought-related beliefs and coping strategies in the escalation of intrusive thoughts: An analog to obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2007;45:2640–2651. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Masuda A, Hayes SC, Fletcher LB, Seignourel PJ, Bunting K, Herbst SA, Lillis J. The impact of acceptance and commitment therapy versus education on stigma toward people with psychological disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2007;45:2764–2772. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Masuda A, Hayes SC, Sackett CF, Twohig MP. Cognitive defusion and self-relevant negative thoughts: Examining the impact of a ninety year old technique. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2004;42:477–485. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- McCracken LM. Contextual cognitive-behavioral therapy for chronic pain. Seattle, WA: International Association for the Study of Pain; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- McCracken LM, Vowles KE, Eccleston C. Acceptance of chronic pain: Component analysis and a revised assessment method. Pain. 2004;107:159–166. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- McMullen J, Barnes-Holmes D, Barnes-Holmes Y, Stewart I, Luciano C, Cochrane A. Acceptance versus distraction: Brief instructions, metaphors, and exercises in increasing tolerance for self-delivered electric shocks. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2008;46:122–129. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Moore MT, Fresco DM. The relationship of explanatory flexibility to explanatory style. Behavior Therapy. 2007;38:325–332. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- O’Donohue W, Yeater EA. Individuating psychotherapies. Behaviour Modification. 2003;27:313–321. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Öst LG. Efficacy of the third wave of behavioral therapies: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2008;46(3):296–321. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Páez-Blarrina M, Luciano C, Gutierrez-Martinez O, Valdivia S, Ortega J, Valverde M. The role of values with personal examples in altering the functions of pain: Comparison between acceptance-based and cognitive-control-based protocols. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2008;46:84–97. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Pennebaker JW, Chung CK. Expressive writing, emotional upheavals, and health. In: Friedman H, Silver R, editors. Handbook of health psychology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2007. pp. 263–284. [Google Scholar]
- Pepper SC. World hypotheses: A study in evidence. Berkeley: University of California Press; 1942. [Google Scholar]
- Pierson H, Hayes SC. Using acceptance and commitment therapy to empower the therapeutic relationship. In: Gilbert P, Leahy R, editors. The therapeutic relationship in cognitive behavior therapy. London: Routledge; 2007. pp. 205–228. [Google Scholar]
- Powers MB, Vörding MBZS, Emmelkamp PMG. Acceptance and commitment therapy: A meta-analytic review. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. 2009;78:73–80. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Pull CB. Current empirical status of acceptance and commitment therapy. Current Opinion in Psychiatry. 2009;22(1):55–60. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Rehfeldt RA, Barnes-Holmes Y, editors. Derived relational responding: Applications for learners with autism and other developmental disabilities. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Rehfeldt RA, Dillen JE, Zionek MM, Kowalchuk RK. Assessing relational learning deficits in perspective-taking in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Psychological Record. 2007;57:23–47. [Google Scholar]
- Rosen GM, Davison GC. Psychology should list empirically supported principles of change (ESPs) and not credential trademarked therapies or other treatment packages. Behavior Modification. 2003;27:300–312. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenfarb I, Hayes SC. Social standard setting: The Achilles’ heel of informational accounts of therapeutic change. Behavior Therapy. 1984;15:515–528. [Google Scholar]
- Rottenberg J, Gross JJ, Gotlib IH. Emotion context insensitivity in major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2005;114:627–629. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Segal ZV, Teasdale JD, Williams JMG. Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy: Theoretical rationale and empirical status. In: Hayes SC, Follette VM, Linehan MM, editors. Mindfulness and acceptance: Expanding the cognitive-behavioral tradition. New York: Guilford Press; 2004. pp. 45–65. [Google Scholar]
- Shawyer F, Ratcliff K, Mackinnon A, Farhall J, Hayes SC, Copolov D. The voices acceptance and action scale (VAAS): Pilot data. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2007;63:593–606. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Sheldon KM, Elliot AJ. Goal striving, need satisfaction, and longitudinal well-being: The self-concordance model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1999;76:482–497. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Skinner BF. Walden Two. Oxford, UK: Macmillan; 1948. [Google Scholar]
- Skinner BF. About behaviorism. Oxford, UK: Knopf; 1974. [Google Scholar]
- Strosahl KD, Hayes SC, Bergan J, Romano P. Assessing the field effectiveness of acceptance and commitment therapy: An example of the manipulated training research method. Behavior Therapy. 1998;29:35–64. [Google Scholar]
- Takahashi M, Muto T, Tada M, Sugiyama M. Acceptance rationale and increasing pain tolerance: Acceptance-based and FEAR-based practice. Japanese Journal of Behavior Therapy. 2002;28:35–46. [Google Scholar]
- Twohig MP, Hayes SC, Masuda A. Increasing willingness to experience obsessions: Acceptance and commitment therapy as a treatment for obsessive compulsive disorder. Behavior Therapy. 2006;37:3–13. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Twohig MP, Hayes SC, Plumb JC, Pruitt LD, Collins AB, Hazlett-Stevens H, Woidneck MR. A randomized clinical trial of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy vs. Progressive Relaxation Training for obsessive compulsive disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2010;78:705–716. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Varra AA, Hayes SC, Roget N, Fisher G. A randomized control trial examining the effect of acceptance and commitment training on clinician willingness to use evidence-based pharmacotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2008;76:449–458. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Wegner DM. Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review. 1994;101:34–52. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Wenzlaff RM, Wegner DM. Thought suppression. Annual Review of Psychology. 2000;51:59–91. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Wicksell RK, Renofalt J, Olsson GL, Bond FW, Melin L. Development and preliminary validation of the Psychological Inflexibility in Pain Scale (PIPS) European Journal of Pain. 2008;12:491–500. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Williams LM. Unpublished dissertation. Charles Stuart University; Bathurst, New South Wales: 2006. Acceptance and commitment therapy: An example of third-wave therapy as a treatment for Australian Vietnam War veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson KG, Dufrene T. Mindfulness for two: An acceptance and commitment therapy approach to mindfulness in psychotherapy. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson KG, Sandoz EK, Kitchens J. The valued living questionnaire: Defining and measuring valued action within a behavioral framework. Psychological Record. 2010;60:249–272. [Google Scholar]
- Zettle RD. Acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) versus systematic desensitization in treatment of mathematics anxiety. Psychological Record. 2003;53:197–215. [Google Scholar]
- Zettle RD. The evolution of a contextual approach to therapy: From comprehensive distancing to ACT. International Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy. 2005;1:77–89. [Google Scholar]
- Zettle RD, Hayes SC. The conceptual and empirical status of rational-emotive therapy. In: Hersen M, Miller PM, Eisler R, editors. Progress in behavior modification. New York: Academic; 1980. pp. 125–166. [Google Scholar]
- Zettle RD, Hayes SC. Rule-governed behavior: A potential theoretical framework for cognitive-behavior therapy. In: Kendall PC, editor. Advances in cognitive-behavioral research and therapy. New York: Academic; 1982. pp. 73–118. [Google Scholar]
- Zettle RD, Hayes SC. Dysfunctional control by client verbal behavior: The context of reason giving. Analysis of Verbal Behavior. 1986;4:30–38. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Zettle RD, Raines JC. Group cognitive and contextual therapies in treatment of depression. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1989;45:438–445. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Full text links
Read article at publisher's site: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2009.08.002
Read article for free, from open access legal sources, via Unpaywall: https://europepmc.org/articles/pmc3635495?pdf=render
Citations & impact
Impact metrics
Citations of article over time
Alternative metrics
Smart citations by scite.ai
Explore citation contexts and check if this article has been
supported or disputed.
https://scite.ai/reports/10.1016/j.beth.2009.08.002
Article citations
Reducing Alcohol Misuse and Promoting Treatment Initiation Among Veterans Through a Brief Internet-Based Intervention: Protocol for a Randomized Controlled Trial.
JMIR Res Protoc, 13:e59993, 22 Aug 2024
Cited by: 0 articles | PMID: 39173142 | PMCID: PMC11377908
Meditation modalities for ADHD in minority pediatric populations in the USA: a scoping review.
Health Promot Perspect, 14(2):91-96, 29 Jul 2024
Cited by: 0 articles | PMID: 39291050 | PMCID: PMC11403344
Review Free full text in Europe PMC
Compare the effectiveness of two treatments, behavioral activation and acceptance and commitment therapy, on depression and mental rumination in mothers of children with cerebral palsy in Ilam city.
J Educ Health Promot, 13:260, 29 Jul 2024
Cited by: 0 articles | PMID: 39310014 | PMCID: PMC11414860
Impact of heartfulness meditation practice compared to the gratitude practices on wellbeing and work engagement among healthcare professionals: Randomized trial.
PLoS One, 19(6):e0304093, 07 Jun 2024
Cited by: 0 articles | PMID: 38848338
Acceptance and commitment therapy for patients with chronic pain: A systematic review and meta-analysis on psychological outcomes and quality of life.
PLoS One, 19(6):e0301226, 14 Jun 2024
Cited by: 0 articles | PMID: 38875241 | PMCID: PMC11178235
Review Free full text in Europe PMC
Go to all (243) article citations
Similar Articles
To arrive at the top five similar articles we use a word-weighted algorithm to compare words from the Title and Abstract of each citation.
Evolving an idionomic approach to processes of change: Towards a unified personalized science of human improvement.
Behav Res Ther, 156:104155, 03 Jul 2022
Cited by: 24 articles | PMID: 35863243
Review
[Acceptance and mindfulness-based cognitive-behavioral therapies].
Sante Ment Que, 38(2):35-63, 01 Jan 2013
Cited by: 2 articles | PMID: 24719002
The Idionomic Future of Cognitive Behavioral Therapy: What Stands Out From Criticisms of ACT Development.
Behav Ther, 54(6):1036-1063, 27 Jul 2023
Cited by: 1 article | PMID: 37863584 | PMCID: PMC10589451
Using values in cognitive and behavioral therapy: A bridge back to philosophy.
J Eval Clin Pract, 29(7):1189-1195, 24 May 2023
Cited by: 2 articles | PMID: 37226577
Review