Europe PMC
Nothing Special   »   [go: up one dir, main page]

Europe PMC requires Javascript to function effectively.

Either your web browser doesn't support Javascript or it is currently turned off. In the latter case, please turn on Javascript support in your web browser and reload this page.

This website requires cookies, and the limited processing of your personal data in order to function. By using the site you are agreeing to this as outlined in our privacy notice and cookie policy.

Abstract 


Objective

This study investigates geographic variations in ADRD mortality in the US. By considering both state of residence and state of birth, we aim to discern the relative importance of these geospatial factors.

Methods

We conducted a secondary data analysis of the National Longitudinal Mortality Study (NLMS), that has 3.5 million records from 1973 to 2011 and over 0.5 million deaths. We focused on individuals born in or before 1930, tracked in NLMS cohorts from 1979 to 2000. Employing multi-level logistic regression, with individuals nested within states of residence and/or states of birth, we assessed the role of geographical factors in ADRD mortality variation.

Results

We found that both state of birth and state of residence account for a modest portion of ADRD mortality variation. Specifically, state of residence explains 1.19% of the total variation in ADRD mortality, whereas state of birth explains only 0.6%. When combined, both state of residence and state of birth account for only 1.05% of the variation, suggesting state of residence could matter more in ADRD mortality outcomes.

Conclusion

Findings of this study suggest that state of residence explains more variation in ADRD mortality than state of birth. These results indicate that factors in later life may present more impactful intervention points for curbing ADRD mortality. While early-life environmental exposures remain relevant, their role as primary determinants of ADRD in later life appears to be less pronounced in this study.

Free full text 


Logo of ssmphSSM - Population Health
SSM Popul Health. 2024 Sep; 27: 101708.
Published online 2024 Aug 20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssmph.2024.101708
PMCID: PMC11387211
PMID: 39262769

Geographic disparities in Alzheimer's disease and related dementia mortality in the US: Comparing impacts of place of birth and place of residence

Associated Data

Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement

Abstract

Objective

This study investigates geographic variations in ADRD mortality in the US. By considering both state of residence and state of birth, we aim to discern the relative importance of these geospatial factors.

Methods

We conducted a secondary data analysis of the National Longitudinal Mortality Study (NLMS), that has 3.5 million records from 1973 to 2011 and over 0.5 million deaths. We focused on individuals born in or before 1930, tracked in NLMS cohorts from 1979 to 2000. Employing multi-level logistic regression, with individuals nested within states of residence and/or states of birth, we assessed the role of geographical factors in ADRD mortality variation.

Results

We found that both state of birth and state of residence account for a modest portion of ADRD mortality variation. Specifically, state of residence explains 1.19% of the total variation in ADRD mortality, whereas state of birth explains only 0.6%. When combined, both state of residence and state of birth account for only 1.05% of the variation, suggesting state of residence could matter more in ADRD mortality outcomes.

Conclusion

Findings of this study suggest that state of residence explains more variation in ADRD mortality than state of birth. These results indicate that factors in later life may present more impactful intervention points for curbing ADRD mortality. While early-life environmental exposures remain relevant, their role as primary determinants of ADRD in later life appears to be less pronounced in this study.

Keywords: Geographic variation, ADRD mortality, Place of birth, Place of residence, Life course

1. Introduction

Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementia (ADRD) significantly impacts numerous American families. Approximately 5 million Americans over age 65 have Alzheimer's Disease – a subset of those with ADRD – and these numbers are expected to swell to nearly 14 million by 2050 (Hebert et al., 2013). Roughly one in five American women or one in ten American men are anticipated to face dementia in their lifetimes (Chêne et al., 2015). Fifteen percent of Americans over the age of 70 have dementia, with an estimated financial burden of just under $30,000 annually per person, excluding the expenses related to informal caregiving (Hurd et al., 2013). Individuals shouldering an average of over $6000 in out-of-pocket expenses (Hurd et al., 2013). The substantial economic and health burdens underscore the urgency to understand the fundamental causes of ADRD, one of the primary causes of mortality in the US.

Numerous studies have identified specific individual risk factors that contribute to the risk of ADRD mortality. Individual characteristics including lower educational attainment (Korhonen et al., 2020), poorer health (e.g., high blood pressure, physical inactivity, obesity, diabetes, etc.) (Chen et al., 2023; Omura et al., 2022), rural residence (Cross & Warraich, 2021), genetic predispositions (Bellenguez et al., 2022), and social and marital relationships (Kim & Hwang, 2024a, Kim & Hwang, 2024b, Kim & Kwon, 2023, Kim & Park, 2023). are associated with elevated risks of ADRD mortality. However, some of these individual characteristics could systematically vary by place, suggesting local environments could play a role in ADRD mortality. For example, although individuals choose their education attainment, compulsory schooling laws in the late 19th century and early 20th century in the US varied by state and meaningfully altered individual attainment (Clay et al., 2012, 2021; Lleras-Muney, 2002; Stephens & Yang, 2014). Moreover, health behaviors and healthcare systems are subject to significant geographic disparities (Tsui et al., 2020), which indicates that regional variations in healthcare access and health behaviors may contribute to an increased risk of ADRD mortality.

In fact, the local environment itself likely also matters for dementia risk, especially Alzheimer's disease and mortality. Case and Paxson (2009) found that lower local infant mortality rates around age two in the US, indicative of the local health environment, were linked with improved cognition at older ages. Similarly, Topping et al. (2023) showed that early life state-level infant mortality rates were associated with ADRD mortality, especially among those under 65 years of age. On the policy front, Medicaid, primarily a state-administered health insurance program, has been linked to several health outcomes. Miller et al. (2019) found recent ACA-related expansions reduced mortality. Earlier expansions also appear to improve population health (Boudreaux et al., 2016; Miller & Wherry, 2019). Access to Food Stamps or the Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program also improved health years later and reduced mortality rates (Heflin et al., 2019; Hoynes et al., 2016; Jones, 2020). While improved health could decrease dementia prevalence, lower mortality could increase prevalence by eliminating the competing risk. In any case, policies at the state or local levels can theoretically influence both the risk and prevalence of dementia.

The relative importance of early-, mid-, or late-life environments for ADRD mortality risk remains uncertain. Supporting the case for the importance of early-life environments, lower educational attainment is one of the most widely recognized risk factors for dementia (Cook & Fletcher, 2015; Harrison et al., 2015; Sattler et al., 2012; Wilson et al., 2009), as well as ADRD mortality (Garcia et al., 2021; Korhonen et al., 2020). It is hypothesized that higher education is linked with lower dementia prevalence and its related mortality via increases in cognitive reserve (Stern, 2009). Importantly, primary and secondary education is largely determined in early-life, and historic state policies in the late 19th and early 20th altered attainment in cohorts currently at risk for dementia (Clay et al., 2012, 2021; Lleras-Muney, 2002; Stephens & Yang, 2014). More directly, a few studies found that while both residing in and being born in the US “stroke belt” states is a strong predictor of dementia, birth location may dominate (Glymour et al., 2011; Topping et al., 2021a).

A growing body of research also underscores the role of early-life environments in shaping health outcomes (Topping et al., 2024). Cognition is related to the local disease environment when an individual was two years old (Case & Paxson, 2009). Exposure to food stamps in early childhood (i.e., ages 0–5) decreased metabolic syndrome (Hoynes et al., 2016), while early-life exposure to Medicaid around these same ages improved adult health (Boudreaux et al., 2016) and decreased mortality (Goodman-Bacon, 2021). In the US, however, most major social safety-net expansions started in the 1960s. This suggests that many individuals impacted by these policies during childhood might be too young to observe dementia onset and mortality. One exception is the Mother's Pension Program—a cash transfer program for mothers between 1911 and 1935 that varied state-by-state—that meaningfully altered longevity, educational attainment, and income for beneficiaries' sons (Aizer et al., 2016). In the early 20th century, poverty was widespread in the US, exacerbated by the Great Depression. However, the impact of the Great Depression varied across the US, with the South Atlantic states enduring a milder downturn and the Mountain states suffering more, influenced by industrial composition and long-term regional trends in employment (Margo, 1993; Rosenbloom & Sundstrom, 1999).

In addition to early exposure, individuals are likely to be exposed to different environments during middle and old age. Approximately one-third of people in the US reside in a different state than where they were born (Kahn & Pearlin, 2006). Therefore, to better understand the contribution of early-life environments to ADRD mortality risk, it is required to consider the role of later-life environments simultaneously (Fletcher et al., 2022). For example, Miller et al. (2019) found that ACA Medicaid expansions reduced mortality among individuals aged 55–64. Expansions of the Earned Income Tax Credit (EITC) in the 1990s increased employment and mental health among mothers (Evans & Garthwaite, 2014; Hoynes & Patel, 2018) and improved the overall health status (Lenhart, 2019). The fact that (i) many US safety-net programs started after 1960, (ii) exposure to the social safety-net in later-life offers material and health advantages, and (iii) data limitations on early-life exposure to dementia risks, all curb researchers’ ability to detect early-life exposure effects. This would lend support to the notion that later-life environments may matter more for determining ADRD mortality, particularly among individuals currently at risk of dementia. Moreover, regional variation in competing causes of death (deaths from other causes such as infection or cardiovascular disease) during the 1970s–1990s, potentially attributable to varying levels of regional public investment (Mensah et al., 2017), may reflect in regional variation in ADRD mortality later in life.

Although there are a few studies on geographic variation in mortality, evidence on the relative importance of state of residence versus state of birth is mixed (Finkelstein et al., 2021; W. Xu et al., 2020). Xu et al. (2021) found that the state of birth accounts for almost twice the variation in cardiovascular disease mortality compared to the state of residence. Conversely, another study revealed that state of birth and state of residence contributed the similar amount of variation in all-cause mortality (Xu et al., 2020). With respect to Alzheimer's disease mortality specifically, one study documented the association between local-level infant mortality rates in early life and death from ADRD in the US (Topping et al., 2023). However, Topping et al. (2021b) reported that an individual's state of birth explains relatively little variation in Alzheimer's disease mortality—providing evidence against early-life factors affecting Alzheimer's disease mortality. The findings suggested that almost all the variation in Alzheimer's disease mortality related to the state of birth (about 4%) is accounted for by the state of residence in later life.

Our study investigates the relative importance of early-versus late-life environment contexts in explaining ADRD mortality risk. This research holds critical implications for policymakers endeavoring to discern the optimal life course stages for policy interventions and for researchers or practitioners seeking insights into how ADRD mortality risk progresses over the lifespan. Specifically, our study considers whether birthplace and/or place of residence are most important when modeling ADRD mortality. Our study offers several key improvements. First, we broaden the definition of dementia to include all dementia types, not just Alzheimer's disease. Second, we use a more representative sample from across the United States. Finally, we have approximately four times as many ADRD deaths recorded. With these improvements, our results offer continued support for the notion that later-life contexts matter more than early-life contexts in ADRD risk for current cohorts at risk of dementia.

2. Data and methods

2.1. Study design

Multi-level logistic regression models were conducted.

2.2. Data

We use data from the restricted version of the National Longitudinal Mortality Study (NLMS). The NLMS is a data product that leverages a set of regularly collected US government surveys to study mortality by creating linkages between the surveys and mortality records. These surveys included the Annual Social and Economic Supplements, which cover the period from March 1973 to March 2011, Current Population Surveys for February 1978, April 1980, August 1980, December 1980, and September 1985, and one 1980 Census cohort, totaling 39 cohorts in all. Each survey is then linked to death certificates in order to identify mortality status through the National Center for Health Statistics in the US. Together this data product is referred to as NLMS cohorts (NLMS, 2014). Across all NLMS cohorts, approximately 5% of available respondents have a “fail-edit” with mortality data, meaning it is not feasible to verify their vital status, and they are not part of the NLMS (Table 2, NLMS, 2014). The key variables we use from the NLMS to complete our analysis are the individual's age and state-of-residence when completing the survey (i.e., CPS or Census), place of birth, race/ethnicity, sex, educational attainment, birth cohort, and cause of death (if applicable).

Table 2

Sample descriptive statistics.

SurvivorsNon-ADRD mortalityADRD mortalityStatistical difference between non-ADRD and ADRD mortality
Age at interview64.5271.09c74.03cc
(0.0338)(0.0276)(0.159)
Age at end of follow-up78.479.31c85.14cc
(0.02457)(0.02648)(0.136)
% Female0.64290.5124c0.657c
(0.0022)(0.0015)(0.009)
% non-Hispanic White0.88370.8807a0.92cc
(0.0015)(0.001)(0.006)
% non-Hispanic Black0.08080.0934c0.063cc
(0.0013)(0.001)(0.005)
% Hispanic0.02360.0175c0.011cc
(0.0006)(0.0003)(0.002)
% Other race/ethnicity0.01190.0084c0.006ca
(0.0005)(0.0003)(0.001)
% < High school0.28440.4986c0.443cc
(0.002)(0.0015)(0.009)
% High school0.39960.2999c0.332cc
(0.0022)(0.0014)(0.009)
% Some college0.15360.1054c0.116c
(0.0017)(0.0009)(0.006)
% College0.16240.096c0.11cb
(0.0017)(0.0009)(0.006)
N73,500175,0004300

Notes: ADRD death classifications are in Table 1. Standard errors in parenthesis. ADRD = Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementias.

ap < 0.1.
bp < 0.05.
cp < 0.001 relative to the reference category (survivors).
Source: Authors' calculations using NLMS data at a Federal Statistical Research Data Center under FSRDC Project Number 2338 (CBDRB-FY23-P2338-R10359).

Our sample includes individuals born in or before 1930 and observed in NLMS cohorts from 1979 to 2000. The 1973 NLMS cohort is excluded because it requires 6 years of follow-up prior to the start of the National Death Index in 1979 (NLMS, 2014). All individuals are followed until death or the end of 2002, whichever is earlier. The final cohort from the 2000 CPS began surveys in March 2000. Thus, we have a minimum of approximately 2.5 years follow up time in the sample by additionally omitting the 2001–2002 cohorts. These restrictions are necessary because place of birth at the state level is not included in NLMS cohorts from 2003 onward (NLMS, 2014) and individuals dying after 2002 rarely have place of birth recorded at the state level. Individuals included in the sample are also observed between ages 52–93 at the time of survey (inclusive) to capture ages most at risk of ADRD mortality.1 Finally, included individuals are born in, and reside in, a state with at least ten ADRD sample deaths.

The analytic sample consists of approximately 253,000 individuals. Due to the sampling structure (i.e., age at interview and using waves before 2001), over 70% of the sample dies during the follow-up period. Individual follow-up is determined from mortality information in the NLMS, which originates from death certificates through the National Center for Health Statistics (NLMS, 2014). Although dementia is a leading cause of death in the United States (J. Xu et al., 2022), this single cause of death represents just under 2% of the analytic sample (or around 2.4% of observed deaths).

2.3. Measures

The dependent variable is a binary indicator for an ADRD death. Among individuals identified as deceased in NLMS, cause of death is identified by ICD-9 and ICD-10 codes. Table 1 highlights the specific codes we use to classify a death as ADRD mortality. The two key explanatory variables in our analysis are state of birth and state of residence. The NLMS “Place of Birth--Final” variable characterizes individual's state of birth. It consists of survey responses for some NLMS cohorts supplemented with administrative record linkages such as the Social Security Administration's NUMIDENT file. State of residence is the recorded Federal Information Processing Standards (FIPS) state code in the NLMS data. If early-life factors dominate, then birth location should capture more variation than current residency (and vice versa). Race/ethnicity is a categorical control variable consisting of non-Hispanic White (reference), non-Hispanic Black, Hispanic, and non-Hispanic other race or ethnicity. It is constructed using NLMS data on race and Hispanic ethnicity. Sex is a binary indicator for females (with reference group males). Educational attainment is grouped as less than high school, high school, some college, and college graduates using the education recode in NLMS (reference group is a college degree, or 4+ years of college). Demographic covariates are reported by individuals or heads of households in the respective underlying survey (i.e., CPS or Census).

Table 1

ICD-9 and ICD-10 codes used to identify ADRD.

ICD-9ICD-10
Alzheimer's Disease331.0F00 & G30a
Vascular Dementias290.4F01a
Disease Dementias/Other conditions294.1
Unspecified Dementias331.1–331.2F03
Senile Dementia290.0–290.3
Other cerebral degeneration331.8
Frontotemporal dementiaG31.0
SenileR54
aF01 includes F01.1, F01.3, F01.8, and F01.9; G30 includes G30.0, G30.1, G30.8, and G30.9. ICD = International Classification of Diseases; ADRD = Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementias.

2.4. Analytic strategy

We compare the relative importance of state-of-birth versus state-of-residence using a series of random effects models (xtlogit, re in Stata). Standard covariates including sex, race/ethnicity, and educational attainment (Xi) are regressed on a binary indicator for ADRD mortality (ADRDi). An ADRD death is coded as 1 and survivors or non-ADRD deaths are coded as zero. Age is included as a fixed effect (γi). Previous literature identifies these characteristics as influential in understanding the risk profile for ADRD (Chêne et al., 2015; Cook & Fletcher, 2015; Harrison et al., 2015; Mehta & Yeo, 2017; Rajan et al., 2013; Sattler et al., 2012; Weuve et al., 2018; Wilson et al., 2009). NLMS cohort (φi) fixed effects are also included as in equation (1).

ADRDi=αi+βXi+γi+φi+ε
(1)

αi is the random effect. Although coefficients for each state are not directly measured in the modeling strategy, we compare the amount of variation explained by state of birth, state of residence, or a combination of both state of birth and residence for each model using ln(σu2) – a variance component for the random effects – and ρ – the proportion of total variance attributable to variance in the random effects. We additionally use the AIC and BIC to assess model quality.

3. Results

Our sample descriptive statistics (Table 2) demonstrate that ADRD mortality is primarily among the oldest Americans. Women and individuals from advantaged backgrounds appear more likely to survive to an age when ADRD can be the main underlying cause of death. Survivors are the youngest sample members—on average being interviewed at age 65 and last observed at age 78—while the deceased are significantly older. Individuals dying from non-ADRD causes on average are interviewed at age 71 and die 8 years later. However, people dying of ADRD are significantly older. On average they are interviewed at age 74 and die at age 85.

Nearly two-thirds of survivors are women, while closer to half of the deceased from non-ADRD causes are women. ADRD deaths though are predominantly among women. ADRD deaths are also primarily among non-Hispanic White individuals. While 88% of survivors are non-Hispanic White, 92% of ADRD deaths are among non-Hispanic Whites. Non-Hispanic Black individuals make up a slightly larger share of the deceased from non-ADRD causes relative to their share of survivors, but they make up a significantly smaller share of ADRD deaths relative to their share of survivors. The distribution of educational attainment among the deceased skews toward lower education levels. For example, half of the deceased from non-ADRD causes have less than a high school degree while just 28% of survivors have less than a high school degree. People dying of ADRD however, appear slightly more educated than those deceased from other causes. Just 44% of ADRD deaths are among people with less than a high school education.

We begin with observing spatial variation in ADRD mortality across the United States in Fig. 1. Similar to Akushevich et al. (2021), our figure highlights significant spatial variation in dementia mortality. The northwest, and to a lesser degree southeast, appears to have higher rates of ADRD mortality while the midsection tends to have lighter shades of blue representing lower ADRD mortality rates. Despite these broad similarities, the side-by-side maps of spatial ADRD mortality rates by state of birth and state of residence are clearly not the same. ADRD mortality is higher among individuals born in Idaho or New York relative to individuals who live in in those states. Conversely, ADRD mortality is lower among individuals born in Washington or Louisiana relative to those who live in those states.

Fig. 1

Spatial variation in ADRD mortality by state of residence and state of birth.

Notes: Arizona and Nevada excluded for state of birth illustration in accordance with Census Bureau rounding rules. ADRD = Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementias.

Source: Authors' calculations using NLMS data at a Federal Statistical Research Data Center under FSRDC Project Number 2338 (CBDRB-FY23-P2338-R10359).

Regression results (Table 3) suggest that state of residence explains relatively more variation in ADRD mortality than either state of birth alone or a combination of state of birth and state of residence. Column 1 contains baseline estimation excluding random effects. Column 2 includes state of residence random effects while column 3 includes state of birth random effects. Column 4 includes a combination of state of residence and state of birth random effects. Both the AIC and BIC are minimized when including only state of residence random effects (column 2) while σu2 and ρ are maximized in this specification. Table 3 shows that state of residence explains 1.19% of the total variation in ADRD mortality (column 2), whereas state of birth explains only 0.6% (column 3). Column 4 suggests that both state of residence and state of birth explain only 1.05% of the total variation in ADRD mortality.

Table 3

Random effects models predicting ADRD mortality.

(1) ADRD mortality(2) ADRD mortality(3) ADRD mortality(4) ADRD mortality
Random effectsNoneSORSOBSOR & SOB
Female (reference: male)1.509c1.514c1.508c1.511c
(0.04923)(0.04945)(0.04923)(0.04933)
nH Black (reference: nH White)0.6897c0.6812c0.6545c0.6619c
(0.04813)(0.04888)(0.04751)(0.04729)
Hispanic (reference: nH White)0.7314a0.6964b0.6958b0.6843b
(0.09129)(0.08993)(0.09111)(0.08954)
Other race/ethnicity (reference: nH White)0.79760.7510.74010.748
(0.1279)(0.1313)(0.1271)(0.131)
Less than high school (reference: college)0.7921c0.7995c0.7858c0.7974c
(0.04127)(0.04197)(0.04111)(0.04187)
High school (reference: college)0.94940.95320.94850.9551
(0.05088)(0.05122)(0.05088)(0.05134)
Some college (reference: college)0.91740.90630.90630.9105
(0.05925)(0.05861)(0.0586)(0.05888)
Constant0.01994c0.02022c0.02078c0.02016c
(0.00272)(0.002826)(0.002876)(0.002767)
N253,000253,000253,000253,000
ln(σu2)0.03968c0.01982c0.03485c
(0.01063)(0.006801)(0.009743)
σu0.19920.14080.1867
ρ0.01190.0060.0105
AIC42200421004216042150
BIC42930428404290042890

Notes: Exponentiated coefficients with standard errors in parenthesis. Age at interview and NLMS cohort fixed effects are included in all regressions (coefficients not reported). In Column 4, the random effect accounts for the unique combinations of state of residence and state of birth. ln(σu2) is a variance component for the random effects and ρ is the proportion of total variance attributable to variance in the random effects. nH = non-Hispanic; RE = Random Effects; SOR = State of Residence; SOB = State of Birth; AIC = Akaike Information Criterion; BIC = Bayesian Information Criterion; ADRD = Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementias.

ap < 0.05.
bp < 0.01.
cp < 0.001.
Source: Authors' calculations using NLMS data at a Federal Statistical Research Data Center under FSRDC Project Number 2338 (CBDRB-FY23-P2338-R10359).

To assess the robustness of our findings, we conducted a series of sensitivity analyses. First, we examined whether our results are sensitive to the inclusion of age as a quadratic continuous variable. In Table S1 of the online supplementary file, substituting a quadratic continuous age covariate confirms the robustness of our main conclusions. Second, to address the potential information misclassification bias caused by including non-deceased participants—where participants who may develop dementia before death are classified as non-cases—we estimated models that exclude survivors from the sample. The results, presented in Table S2 of the online supplementary file, remain substantially similar to the original findings. We have also used alternative specifications that use Stata's “melogit” command that nest state of residence and state of birth random effects instead. In this case, the variance explained by state of residence is still the dominating feature.

4. Discussion

ADRD is a pressing public health concern, the impacts of which reverberate through individual lives, families, and the broader socio-economic fabric of the US. This study investigates the relative importance of early-versus late-life environment contexts in explaining ADRD mortality risk. The findings of this study support the notion that later-life environments could matter more than early-life environments for the likelihood of dying from ADRD. Leveraging a large sample of ADRD deaths available in restricted-use NLMS data, we find that state of birth consistently explains less variation in ADRD deaths relative to observed state-of-residence at ages 52–93. These results corroborate an earlier study using a smaller sample and more narrow definition of dementia mortality (Topping et al., 2021b); however, we doubt this is to be a definitive or final verdict on the question. While our results are interesting, informative, and a necessary first step to understanding the relative importance of environment contexts during the life course, future research with younger birth cohorts aging into ADRD mortality and incorporating alternative frameworks to modeling competing risks of mortality could be better equipped to address optimal life course stages for policy intervention.

These findings echo and expand upon the current discourse in the field. While early-life factors such as educational attainment, influenced by state-level policies and local environments, undoubtedly play a role in cognitive outcomes in later life, our findings suggest that where an individual spends their later years might be just as, if not more, influential. Delving deeper into potential mechanisms, several plausible explanations emerge. One of the foremost considerations is the exposure to state-specific health policies and infrastructures (Osypuk et al., 2014). Every state has its own health mandates (e.g., Affordable Care Act/Medicaid), insurance structures, and established guidelines for treating older adults. These policies, whether they pertain to preventive care, early detection, or disease management, can significantly shape the health trajectories of its older residents. Furthermore, states that have robust healthcare infrastructures, with easy access to geriatric care specialists, neurologists, and mental health professionals, might offer residents a better chance at early detection and management of ADRD (Bradford et al., 2009).

That said, it is essential to approach these findings with caution. While we have made strides in our understanding, the magnitudes of explained variation by state of residence or birth are modest. The substantial remainder of unexplained variation suggests that there are other significant factors, beyond state of residence or birth, that influence ADRD outcomes. There are undoubtedly many risk factors that remain unknown and several known risk factors lack a causal link to dementia. These might include individual genetic predispositions, lifestyle factors, or other unmeasured environmental exposures. An asset of our study design is that we can remain agnostic on what specific early- and late-life factors contribute to our observed patterns. It implicitly incorporates both known and unknown environmental risk factors into determining whether early- or late-life matters more for ADRD mortality. In this way, the study can retain applicability even after new discoveries are made. Thus, our study serves as a foundational step, opening doors for more nuanced and multi-dimensional research into the geographical distribution of ADRD.

This study has several limitations. First, we are examining ADRD mortality, not the presence of cognitive impairment, decline, or even the presence of dementia leading up to death. To be classified as an individual with ADRD mortality, this condition must have been listed as the underlying cause of death on the death certificate. It is unlikely that all individuals with underlying dementia indications or pathology at the end of life will be listed with dementia as an underlying cause of death (Todd et al., 2013), which could bias our results. Thus, although the populations of individuals with dementia and those dying from dementia overlap, they are distinct. NLMS is not equipped to comment on the presence of cognitive impairment or pathology, only the underlying cause of death as recorded. It is likely that some individuals with respiratory or circulatory diseases listed as the underlying cause of death may have had ADRD (Brunnström & Englund, 2009). When considering the link between pollution and dementia, a review found higher exposure is almost always linked with dementia, but the link with cognitive decline is more mixed (Peters et al., 2019). In their analysis of geographic disparities of Alzheimer's Disease mortality, Akushevich et al. (2021) conclude that differences in filling out death certificates are an unlikely cause of geographic disparities. Their conclusion suggests this limitation may not meaningfully contribute to our results.

Second, mortality selection is an important factor that could bias results. Bishop et al. (2022) suggest that individuals who died would have been more likely to develop ADRD (as measured by Medicare claims data while alive) had they lived longer. We see evidence of this pattern in our own sample (Table 2). Individuals dying during follow-up without ADRD are typically younger. There's also a larger portion with lower educational attainment, and individuals who are non-Hispanic Black among those dying of non-ADRD causes relative to those dying of ADRD. Thus, ADRD mortality is concentrated among the oldest old: those who have survived long enough to acquire and die from ADRD. At the same time, one of the most consistent risk factors for cognitive impairment or developing ADRD is lower educational attainment (Cook & Fletcher, 2015; Harrison et al., 2015; Sattler et al., 2012; Wilson et al., 2009). However, lower socioeconomic status, and education in particular, is importantly also a significant risk factor for higher mortality rates (Chetty et al., 2016; Montez et al., 2019) and reiterates the fact that we are studying the presence of ADRD mortality, not cognitive impairment, decline, or ADRD indications near the end of life.

Third, we limit our sample to individuals born in 1930 or earlier. While this ensures ample time to observe mortality, no one in our sample was exposed to the social programs forming today's safety-net during their young childhood. Instead, individuals from these cohorts primarily encountered the safety-net in adulthood. Two potential exceptions to this generalization are the Mothers' Pension program and the Aid to Families with Dependent Children (AFDC) program, which was introduced in 1935. In the case of the AFDC, the youngest in our sample might have been exposed during childhood, but never before reaching five years of age. Later safety-net programs like Food Stamps, the predecessor of SNAP, began as a pilot in 1961 (Hoynes et al., 2016), by which time our youngest cohort was already in their 30s. Medicaid started shortly after, and the Earned Income Tax Credit did not begin until 1975. Many programs comprising the US social safety-net differ from state to state. To the degree that improved health and wellbeing from these programs is linked with ADRD mortality, in our sample it would likely only be detected by adulthood state of residence. As individuals exposed to the social safety-net as children age, future research may need to revisit this important question.

In conclusion, our study sheds light on the often-overlooked geographical nuances in the understanding of ADRD. While early-life factors and state of birth may be influential, our findings emphasize the importance of considering where one resides in their later years, at least for current cohorts at risk of ADRD. It is crucial to consider the broader implications of these findings. If the state of residence in later life is indeed important, then interventions targeted at improving the quality of life for older adults, enhancing healthcare access, or reducing environmental risk factors in states with higher ADRD prevalence could be strategic starting points. Additionally, understanding the influence of state-specific policies or practices could guide more targeted, state-level interventions, potentially leading to reduced ADRD incidence or improved care outcomes. As an effort to address ADRD progress, such insights can guide more effective, targeted strategies, bringing us one step closer to a world with a deeper understanding and, hopefully, better management of this debilitating condition.

Ethical statement

This research was approved by the Institutional Review Boards at the University of Wisconsin-Madison (2016-0856).

Disclaimer

Any views expressed are those of the authors and not those of the U.S. Census Bureau. The Census Bureau has reviewed this data product to ensure appropriate access, use, and disclosure avoidance protection of the confidential source data used to produce this product. This research was performed at a Federal Statistical Research Data Center under FSRDC Project Number 2338 (CBDRB-FY23-P2338-R10359 and CBDRB-FY24-P2338-R11203).

CRediT authorship contribution statement

Jason Fletcher: Writing – review & editing, Supervision, Methodology, Investigation, Funding acquisition, Conceptualization. Katie Jajtner: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Methodology, Investigation, Formal analysis, Conceptualization. Jinho Kim: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Supervision, Methodology, Investigation.

Declaration of competing interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Acknowledgements

The authors are listed in alphabetical order. All authors contributed equally to the research. Jason Fletcher and Katie Jajtner acknowledge research funding from the NIA (RF1AG062765). Jinho Kim acknolwedges research funding from the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) grant funded by the Korea government (MSIT) (RS-2023-00219289). Any views expressed are those of the authors and not those of the U.S. Census Bureau. The Census Bureau has reviewed this data product to ensure appropriate access, use, and disclosure avoidance protection of the confidential source data used to produce this product. This research was performed at a Federal Statistical Research Data Center under FSRDC Project Number 2338 (CBDRB-FY23-P2338-R10359 and CBDRB-FY24-P2338-R11203).

Appendix ASupplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssmph.2024.101708.

1We note that our sample restrictions place some constraints on the age and birth year combinations available in our analysis. For example, a 52-year-old observed in our sample, since they must be born prior to 1930, can only be found in survey years 1979–1982 because of the dependence among age, survey year, and birth year: Age (at survey) = Survey Year – Birth Year.

Appendix A. Supplementary data

The following is/are the supplementary data to this article.

Multimedia component 1:
Click here to view.(34K, docx)Multimedia component 1

Data availability

The authors do not have permission to share data.

References

  • Aizer A., Eli S., Ferrie J., Lleras-Muney A. The long-run impact of cash transfers to poor families. The American Economic Review. 2016;106(4):935–971. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Akushevich I., Yashkin A.P., Yashin A.I., Kravchenko J. Geographic disparities in mortality from Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 69(8), 2306-2315. 2021 [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Bellenguez C., Küçükali F., Jansen I.E., Kleineidam L., Moreno-Grau S., Amin N., Naj A.C., Campos-Martin R., Grenier-Boley B., Andrade V. New insights into the genetic etiology of Alzheimer's disease and related dementias. Nature Genetics. 2022;54(4):412–436. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Bishop K.C., Ketcham J.D., Kuminoff N.V. National Bureau of Economic Research; 2022. [Google Scholar]
  • Boudreaux M.H., Golberstein E., McAlpine D.D. The long-term impacts of Medicaid exposure in early childhood: Evidence from the program's origin. Journal of Health Economics. 2016;45:161–175. 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2015.11.001. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Bradford A., Kunik M.E., Schulz P., Williams S.P., Singh H. Missed and delayed diagnosis of dementia in primary care: Prevalence and contributing factors. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders. 2009;23(4):306. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Brunnström H.R., Englund E.M. Cause of death in patients with dementia disorders. European Journal of Neurology. 2009;16(4):488–492. 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2008.02503.x. [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Case A., Paxson C. Early life health and cognitive function in old age. The American Economic Review. 2009;99(2):104–109. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Chen R., Charpignon M.-L., Raquib R.V., Wang J., Meza E., Aschmann H.E., DeVost M.A., Mooney A., Bibbins-Domingo K., Riley A.R. Excess mortality with alzheimer disease and related dementias as an underlying or contributing cause during the COVID-19 pandemic in the US. JAMA Neurology. 2023;80(9):919–928. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Chêne G., Beiser A., Au R., Preis S.R., Wolf P.A., Dufouil C., Seshadri S. Gender and incidence of dementia in the Framingham Heart Study from mid-adult life. Alzheimer's and Dementia. 2015;11(3):310–320. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Chetty R., Stepner M., Abraham S., Lin S., Scuderi B., Turner N., Bergeron A., Cutler D. The association between income and life expectancy in the United States, 2001-2014. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2016;315(16):1750–1766. 10.1001/jama.2016.4226. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Clay K., Lingwall J., Stephens M. National Bureau of Economic Research; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • Clay K., Lingwall J., Stephens M., Jr. Laws, educational outcomes, and returns to schooling evidence from the first wave of US state compulsory attendance laws. Labour Economics. 2021;68 [Google Scholar]
  • Cook C.J., Fletcher J.M. Can education rescue genetic liability for cognitive decline? Social Science & Medicine. 2015;127:159–170. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Cross S.H., Warraich H.J. Rural‐urban disparities in mortality from Alzheimer's and related dementias in the United States, 1999–2018. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2021;69(4):1095–1096. 10.1111/jgs.16996. [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Evans W.N., Garthwaite C.L. Giving mom a break: The impact of higher EITC payments on maternal health. American Economic Journal: Economic Policy. 2014;6(2):258–290. [Google Scholar]
  • Finkelstein A., Gentzkow M., Williams H. Place-based drivers of mortality: Evidence from migration. The American Economic Review. 2021;111(8):2697–2735. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Fletcher J.M., Engelman M., Johnson N.J., Hakes J., Palloni A. Understanding geographic disparities in mortality. Demography. 2022;60(2):351–377. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Garcia M.A., Downer B., Chiu C.-T., Saenz J.L., Ortiz K., Wong R. Educational benefits and cognitive health life expectancies: Racial/ethnic, nativity, and gender disparities. The Gerontologist. 2021;61(3):330–340. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Goodman-Bacon A. The long-run effects of childhood insurance coverage: Medicaid implementation, adult health, and labor market outcomes. The American Economic Review. 2021;111(8):2550–2593. [Google Scholar]
  • Harrison S.L., Sajjad A., Bramer W.M., Ikram M.A., Tiemeier H., Stephan B.C. Exploring strategies to operationalize cognitive reserve: A systematic review of reviews. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2015;37(3):253–264. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Hebert L.E., Weuve J., Scherr P.A., Evans D.A. Alzheimer disease in the United States (2010–2050) estimated using the 2010 census. Neurology. 2013;80(19):1778–1783. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Heflin C.M., Ingram S.J., Ziliak J.P. The effect of the supplemental nutrition assistance program on mortality. Health Affairs. 2019;38(11):1807–1815. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Hoynes H., Patel A.J. Effective policy for reducing poverty and inequality? The earned income Tax Credit and the distribution of income. Journal of Human Resources. 2018;53(4):859–890. [Google Scholar]
  • Hoynes H., Schanzenbach D.W., Almond D. Long-run impacts of childhood access to the safety net. The American Economic Review. 2016;106(4):903–934. [Google Scholar]
  • Hurd M.D., Martorell P., Delavande A., Mullen K.J., Langa K.M. Monetary costs of dementia in the United States. New England Journal of Medicine. 2013;368(14):1326–1334. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Jones J.W. 2020. [Google Scholar]
  • Kahn J.R., Pearlin L.I. Financial strain over the life course and health among older adults. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2006;47(1):17–31. 10.1177/002214650604700102. [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kim J., Hwang S. Separating the effects of transitions into and out of social isolation and loneliness on cognitive function in later life. The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2024;79(7):1996–2018. 10.1093/geronb/gbae082. [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kim J., Hwang S. The asymmetric effects of improving and declining marital satisfaction on cognitive function. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2024;86(4):887–909. 10.1111/jomf.12991. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kim J., Kwon K.Y. Investigating heterogeneity in the relationship between marital satisfaction and cognitive health by gender and across the cognitive function distribution. Innovation in Aging. 2023;7(7):1–9. 10.1093/geroni/igad079. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kim J., Park G. Prolonged social isolation and cognitive function in older adults: lack of informal social contact versus formal social activity as the source of social isolation. Aging & Mental Health. 2023;27(12):2438–2445. 10.1080/13607863.2023.2202616. [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Korhonen K., Einiö E., Leinonen T., Tarkiainen L., Martikainen P. Midlife socioeconomic position and old-age dementia mortality: A large prospective register-based study from Finland. BMJ Open. 2020;10(1) [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Lenhart O. The effects of income on health: New evidence from the earned income Tax Credit. Review of Economics of the Household. 2019;17(2):377–410. [Google Scholar]
  • Lleras-Muney A. Were compulsory attendance and child labor laws effective? An analysis from 1915 to 1939. The Journal of Law and Economics. 2002;45(2):401–435. [Google Scholar]
  • Margo R.A. Employment and unemployment in the 1930s. The Journal of Economic Perspectives. 1993;7(2):41–59. [Google Scholar]
  • Mehta K.M., Yeo G.W. Systematic review of dementia prevalence and incidence in United States race/ethnic populations. Alzheimer's and Dementia. 2017;13(1):72–83. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Mensah G.A., Wei G.S., Sorlie P.D., Fine L.J., Rosenberg Y., Kaufmann P.G., Mussolino M.E., Hsu L.L., Addou E., Engelgau M.M., Gordon D. Decline in cardiovascular mortality: Possible causes and implications. Circulation Research. 2017;120(2):366–380. 10.1161/CIRCRESAHA.116.309115. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Miller S., Altekruse S., Johnson N., Wherry L.R. National Bureau of Economic Research; 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • Miller S., Wherry L.R. The long-term effects of early life Medicaid coverage. Journal of Human Resources. 2019;54(3):785–824. 10.3368/jhr.54.3.0816.8173R1. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Montez J.K., Zajacova A., Hayward M.D., Woolf S.H., Chapman D., Beckfield J. Educational disparities in adult mortality across US states: How do they differ, and have they changed since the mid-1980s? Demography. 2019:1–24. 10.1007/s13524-018-0750-z. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • NLMS . 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • Omura J.D., McGuire L.C., Patel R., Baumgart M., Lamb R., Jeffers E.M., Olivari B.S., Croft J.B., Thomas C.W., Hacker K. Modifiable risk factors for Alzheimer disease and related dementias among adults aged≥ 45 years—United States, 2019. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. 2022;71(20):680. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Osypuk T.L., Joshi P., Geronimo K., Acevedo-Garcia D. Do social and economic policies influence health? A review. Current Epidemiology Reports. 2014;1:149–164. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Peters R., Ee N., Peters J., Booth A., Mudway I., Anstey K.J. Air pollution and dementia: A systematic review. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease. 2019;70(s1):S145–S163. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Rajan K.B., Hebert L.E., Scherr P.A., Mendes de Leon C.F., Evans D.A. Disability in basic and instrumental activities of daily living is associated with faster rate of decline in cognitive function of older adults. Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biomedical Sciences and Medical Sciences. 2013;68(5):624–630. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Rosenbloom J.L., Sundstrom W.A. The sources of regional variation in the severity of the Great Depression: Evidence from US manufacturing, 1919–1937. The Journal of Economic History. 1999;59(3):714–747. [Google Scholar]
  • Sattler C., Toro P., Schönknecht P., Schröder J. Cognitive activity, education and socioeconomic status as preventive factors for mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Psychiatry Research. 2012;196(1):90–95. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Stephens M., Yang D.-Y. Compulsory education and the benefits of schooling. The American Economic Review. 2014;104(6):1777–1792. [Google Scholar]
  • Stern Y. Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(10):2015–2028. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Todd S., Barr S., Passmore A.P. Cause of death in Alzheimer's disease: A cohort study. QJM: An International Journal of Medicine. 2013;106(8):747–753. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Topping M., Fletcher J., Kim J. Variation in adult cognition across domains and life course place effects in the UK. Journal of Aging and Health. 2024 [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Topping M., Kim J., Fletcher J. Association and pathways of birth in the stroke belt on old age dementia and stroke Mortality. SSM - Population Health. 2021;15 [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Topping M., Kim J., Fletcher J. Geographic variation in Alzheimer's disease mortality. PLoS One. 2021;16(7) [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Topping M., Kim J., Fletcher J. Area-level infant mortality exposure in early life and Alzheimer's disease mortality: Examining variation based on age, sex, and place of birth. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, Preprint. 2023:1–10. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Tsui, J., Hirsch, J. A., Bayer, F. J., Quinn, J. W., Cahill, J., Siscovick, D., & Lovasi, G. S. (2020). Patterns in geographic access to health care facilities across neighborhoods in the United States based on data from the national establishment time-series between 2000 and 2014. JAMA Network Open, 3(5), e205105. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract]
  • Weuve J., Barnes L.L., de Leon C.F.M., Rajan K.B., Beck T., Aggarwal N.T., Hebert L.E., Bennett D.A., Wilson R.S., Evans D.A. Cognitive aging in black and white Americans: Cognition, cognitive decline, and incidence of Alzheimer disease dementia. Epidemiology. 2018;29(1):151. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Wilson R.S., Hebert L.E., Scherr P.A., Barnes L.L., De Leon C.M., Evans D.A. Educational attainment and cognitive decline in old age. Neurology. 2009;72(5):460–465. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Xu W., Engelman M., Palloni A., Fletcher J. Where and when: Sharpening the lens on geographic disparities in mortality. SSM-Population Health. 2020 [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Xu J., Murphy S.L., Kochanek K.D., Arias E. NCHS Data Brief; 2022. No. 456. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Xu W., Topping M., Fletcher J. State of birth and cardiovascular disease mortality: Multilevel analyses of the national longitudinal mortality study. SSM-Population Health. 2021;15 [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]

Articles from SSM - Population Health are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

Citations & impact 


This article has not been cited yet.

Impact metrics

Alternative metrics

Altmetric item for https://www.altmetric.com/details/168404515
Altmetric
Discover the attention surrounding your research
https://www.altmetric.com/details/168404515

Similar Articles 


To arrive at the top five similar articles we use a word-weighted algorithm to compare words from the Title and Abstract of each citation.


Funding 


Funders who supported this work.

Ministry of Science, ICT and Future Planning (1)

National Institute on Aging (1)

National Research Foundation of Korea