Abstract
Introduction
The detrimental effects of sleep deprivation (SD) on cognitive function and quality of life are well known, and sleep disturbances are a major physical and mental health issue worldwide. Working memory plays an important role in many complex cognitive processes. Therefore, it is necessary to identify strategies that can effectively counteract the negative effects of SD on working memory.Methods
In the present study, we utilized event-related potentials (ERPs) to investigate the restorative effects of 8 h of recovery sleep (RS) on working memory impairments induced by total sleep deprivation for 36 h. We analyzed ERP data from 42 healthy male participants who were randomly assigned to two groups. The nocturnal sleep (NS) group completed a 2-back working memory task before and after normal sleep for 8 h. The sleep deprivation (SD) group completed a 2-back working memory task before and after 36 h of total sleep deprivation (TSD) and after 8 h of RS. Electroencephalographic data were recorded during each task.Results
The N2 and P3 components-which are related to working memory-exhibited low-amplitude and slow-wave characteristics after 36 h of TSD. Additionally, we observed a significant decrease in N2 latency after 8 h of RS. RS also induced significant increases in the amplitude of the P3 component and in the behavioral indicators.Discussion
Overall, 8 h of RS attenuated the decrease in working memory performance caused by 36 h of TSD. However, the effects of RS appear to be limited.Free full text
Recovery sleep attenuates impairments in working memory following total sleep deprivation
Abstract
Introduction
The detrimental effects of sleep deprivation (SD) on cognitive function and quality of life are well known, and sleep disturbances are a major physical and mental health issue worldwide. Working memory plays an important role in many complex cognitive processes. Therefore, it is necessary to identify strategies that can effectively counteract the negative effects of SD on working memory.
Methods
In the present study, we utilized event-related potentials (ERPs) to investigate the restorative effects of 8h of recovery sleep (RS) on working memory impairments induced by total sleep deprivation for 36h. We analyzed ERP data from 42 healthy male participants who were randomly assigned to two groups. The nocturnal sleep (NS) group completed a 2-back working memory task before and after normal sleep for 8h. The sleep deprivation (SD) group completed a 2-back working memory task before and after 36h of total sleep deprivation (TSD) and after 8h of RS. Electroencephalographic data were recorded during each task.
Results
The N2 and P3 components—which are related to working memory—exhibited low-amplitude and slow-wave characteristics after 36h of TSD. Additionally, we observed a significant decrease in N2 latency after 8h of RS. RS also induced significant increases in the amplitude of the P3 component and in the behavioral indicators.
Discussion
Overall, 8 h of RS attenuated the decrease in working memory performance caused by 36 h of TSD. However, the effects of RS appear to be limited.
1. Introduction
Proper sleep has been shown to exert beneficial effects on memory, cognitive function, work performance, and immune-related parameters (Walker and Stickgold, 2004; Stickgold, 2005; Anderson and Horne, 2006; Djonlagic et al., 2009). However, the incidence of sleep-related problems continues to increase. While the most direct and obvious behavioral manifestation of sleep deprivation (SD) is drowsiness, it also significantly impairs cognitive function (Drummond et al., 2006; Killgore et al., 2006; Alhola and Polo-Kantola, 2007; Anderson and Platten, 2011). Honn et al. (2020) demonstrated that SD reduces processing speed in visual search, spatial memory, paired associative learning, motor response, and other cognitive tasks. Additional evidence suggests that SD significantly increases response times in working memory tasks. Such changes are also accompanied by decreased activation of the frontoparietal cortex (FPC), which plays an important role in cognitive control. Specifically, the FPC can bypass top-down cognitive control, thus enabling individuals to focus on goal-related information while suppressing irrelevant information (Smallwood et al., 2012; Wen et al., 2013).
Working memory is a limited-capacity system involved in the temporary storage and maintenance of information related to a specific task (Baddeley, 2010). As such, it acts as a bridge between short- and long-term memory (Baddeley, 2000) and is involved in operating, processing, and executing various control processes. Previous studies have consistently demonstrated that SD significantly impairs working memory (Lo et al., 2012; Gerhardsson et al., 2019). In addition to decreasing the quality of information stored in the working memory, SD reduces processing speeds and alters event-related potentials (ERPs) during task performance by prolonging latency and reducing the amplitude of the N2 and P3 components (Zhang et al., 2019). These changes are also associated with a decreased ability to discriminate between target stimuli (Koslowsky and Babkoff, 1992), reduced availability of disposable attentional resources, and alterations in selective attention toward emotional stimuli/signal processing (Schacht et al., 2008).
Numerous research groups have aimed to identify interventions that can effectively counteract the aforementioned negative consequences of SD. Although caffeine and other drugs can effectively maintain work performance and alertness (Rosenthal et al., 1991; Rogers et al., 2005; Biggs et al., 2007), they have limited effects on high-level cognitive functions. In addition, the use of caffeine may be associated with recovery costs in individuals with long-term sleep deficiency (Doty et al., 2017). Recovery sleep (RS) refers to a short period of adequate sleep following SD, and it represents a potential non-pharmacological strategy for combating the effects of SD on cognitive function. Some studies have reported that RS can attenuate SD-induced hyperalgesia (Roehrs et al., 2012; Stroemel-Scheder et al., 2020) and cognitive impairment (Ruggiero and Redeker, 2014). RS also alleviates fatigue and improves attention/alertness, and longer periods of RS are associated with the restoration of cognitive function to a greater degree (Studte et al., 2015). One study reported that 8h of RS can attenuate impairments in response inhibition caused by 36h of total sleep deprivation (TSD) (Jin et al., 2015). One night of TSD causes obvious changes in the topological characteristics of the small-world network in the brain. Although two nights of RS can completely restore the global properties of the brain network, it has not been found to induce changes in the local function (Jiang et al., 2018). Following 58h of wakefulness, sleep stage distribution resembles that of a baseline night when the sleep duration of the recovery night is extended to 14h (Hennecke et al., 2019). Moreover, a 90-min nap during a day of sleep deprivation can restore hippocampus-dependent learning, and the structural composition of the hippocampus has been shown to predict the success of learning recovery (Saletin et al., 2016). Nevertheless, one or two nights of RS after complete or chronic sleep loss cannot sufficiently attenuate the associated neurobehavioral deficits or restore self-monitoring abilities and brain metabolism (Banks et al., 2010; Lo et al., 2016; Boardman et al., 2018). Therefore, the amount of sleep required to restore cognitive function following extended wakefulness remains unclear.
Impairments in cognitive function due to SD inevitably affect the task performance of individuals. Previous studies have used both neurophysiological and behavioral indicators to assess an individual’s physiological and psychological states. Among these, neurophysiological indicators are more sensitive for assessing the effects of SD. Chai et al. (2020) reported that two nights of RS after one night of TSD restored hippocampal connectivity to normal levels but did not fully restore behavioral performance or its associations with hippocampal connectivity (Chai et al., 2020). A study investigating joint rhythm (Gumenyuk et al., 2014) reported that significant differences in the reorienting negativity amplitude (related to behavioral responses) could be explained by the increased sensitivity of neurophysiological indicators. Electroencephalographic (EEG) data recorded from the human scalp reflect potential changes that indicate the activity of the brain, and ERPs are special evoked potentials related to endogenous neural activity and cognitive function. After RS, information processing efficiency was also restored which related to the promotion and recovery of memory by sleep. The N2 component of the ERP reflects performance monitoring and cognitive control (top-down and bottom-up), and its neural source has most consistently been identified as the anterior midcingulate cortex (aMCC) (Folstein and Van Petten, 2008; Huster et al., 2010). Some researchers have suggested that the P300 component plays a crucial role in brain processing at the intersection between perception and decision making (Verleger et al., 2005). However, others have suggested that it is more commonly involved in synthesizing and carrying information related to conscious access, attentional moderation, and post-response adaptations (Polich, 2007; Dehaene and Changeux, 2011; Raud and Huster, 2017). Previous studies have also demonstrated that the P3 wave is related to the renewal of working memory content and it decreases with an increase in working memory load (Donchin and Fabiani, 1991; Peng et al., 2020). Therefore, N2-P3 components that relate to different information processes will also change after RS (Zhang et al., 2019). Sleep cycles include four stages. Healthy adults need to sleep for 4–5cycles every night, which typically requires approximately 8h (Malik et al., 2018). Sleep “reshapes” hippocampal synapses, making room for learning the next day (Spano et al., 2019).
At present, it remains to be determined whether 8h of RS can attenuate SD-induced impairments in working memory. In the present study, we investigated the potential restorative effects of RS on working memory and ERP latency/amplitude after 36h of TSD. We tested the following hypotheses: a) 8h of RS partially attenuates the deleterious effects of 36h of TSD on working memory in our participants and b) following 36h of TSD, 8h of RS reduces the latencies of the working memory-related N2-P3 components and increases their amplitudes. A lack of significant differences between 36h TSD and subsequent 8h RS would suggest that the restorative effects of 8h of RS are limited.
2. Materials and methods
2.1. Participants
The present study included 42 healthy male college students (mean age, 23years; age range, 21–28years). Participants were randomly assigned to two groups: the nocturnal sleep (NS) group (20 participants) and the SD group (22 participants). All participants were right-handed and maintained good sleep habits (Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index <5 points) (Buysse et al., 1989). None of the patients reported a history of mental or physical illnesses, and none had previously undergone psychophysiological testing. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity, and their IQ scores were greater than the population average (Raven test scores >110 points). The research staff provided a full explanation of the study procedures prior to the experiment. All participants were asked to refrain from smoking, drinking alcohol/coffee, and taking any drugs for at least 48h prior to the experiment, and were instructed to maintain a normal sleep pattern for 1week. All participants slept for 7–9h per day between 10:00pm and 9:00am, and their sleep routines throughout the study period were confirmed using sleep diaries. All participants provided written informed consent and received monetary compensation upon the completion of the experiment. This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Fourth Military Medical University.
2.2. Experimental design
In the present study, we used three tasks: a 2-back pronunciation working memory (PWM) task (Figure 1A), a 2-back spatial working memory (SWM) task (Figure 1B), and a 2-back object working memory (OWM) task (Figure 1C). The stimuli for these tasks included 15 case-insensitive English letters (excluding similar letters), small black squares, and 12 geometric figures. Each task lasted approximately 5min and included 122 trials. Target stimuli were presented for 400ms, with an inter-stimulus interval of 1,600ms. Participants were asked to match the current stimulus with the stimulus presented two trials earlier, and were instructed to press the left mouse button for matching stimuli and the right mouse button for mismatching stimuli. The matching and mismatching stimuli were presented in a pseudo-random order in a 1:1 ratio.
According to the kind of stimulus material, working memory can be divided into the phonetic, spatial, and object types. To perform a more thorough investigation than past publications on the effects of sleep deprivation and restorative sleep on working memory, this study examined three types of working memory tasks. The tasks were independent of each other, and the participants exercised to eliminate the connection effect before engaging in the formal experiment.
2.3. Experimental procedures
A mixed experimental design was used. Prior to testing, all participants practiced the experimental tasks until they reached an accuracy of >90% (to exclude the influence of practice). All the participants visited the laboratory once. The NS group arrived at the laboratory at 6:00 p.m. on the day of the experiment and performed the first task at 8:00p.m. (baseline state; NS-BS). Following this, the participants slept in the laboratory that night (ensuring a sleep time of at least 8h), woke up the next day at 7:00am, and performed the second task at 8:00am (0h sleep deprivation; NS-SD0) with EEG data being recorded simultaneously (Figure 2). The SD group slept in the laboratory the night prior to completing the experimental tasks, and all participants were instructed to sleep from 11:00pm to 7:00am to ensure a sleep time of at least 8h. The SD group underwent 36h of SD followed by 8h of RS (Figure 3). TSD was initiated at 8:00am the following morning. The 2-back working memory tasks were performed both before and after TSD and 8h after RS, with EEG data being recorded simultaneously.
Sleep inertia can be observed after waking up from complete and habitual night sleep and seems to be a common step in the sleep–wake transition process. Sleep inertia tends to be exacerbated by prior sleep loss or extended wakefulness prior to a sleep episode (Miccoli et al., 2008; Hilditch et al., 2017). To avoid sleep inertia, RS was implemented from 11:00 p.m. to 7:00 a.m., and a third set of EEG data was acquired at 8:00 a.m. on the third day (Dinges, 1990). Previous research has demonstrated that individual performance is relatively impaired within 5min of awakening and gradually returns to normal over a period of 15–30min (Dinges, 1986). Before TSD and 8h after RS, the three types of working memory tasks were performed from 8:00a.m. to 8:30a.m. After TSD, the three types of working memory tasks were performed from 20:00 to 20:30. Two participants completed the experiment simultaneously. Two medical workers and one researcher were present throughout the TSD period to prevent the participants from sleeping or napping. During the experiment, participants were allowed to eat, drink, and perform light physical activities, but were not allowed to engage in strenuous exercise or ingest caffeine, alcohol, or tea. The illumination in the laboratory was set to 100lx with a normal fluorescent lamp.
2.4. EEG recordings
EEG data were acquired in a dark, sound-proof, and electronically shielded EEG laboratory using 64-electrode caps. Stimuli were generated and presented using the Stim-2 software (NeuroScan Inc., United States). The electrodes were arranged in accordance with the international 10–20 system. Horizontal and vertical electrooculograms were recorded during EEG acquisition, and the bilateral mastoid process was used as the reference electrode. The recordings were performed at 1,000Hz, and the channel impedance was maintained at below 5 kΩ.
2.5. Data analysis
We analyzed various behavioral parameters, including mean reaction time, accuracy, and the number of correct responses per unit time (number of correct responses per unit time=correct ratio×1,000/correct time).
In this study, the EEG amplitude represented brain potential intensities. The amplitude size is closely related to the number of neurons involved in synchronous firing, as well as the arrangement direction of the neurons. Latency provided a measure the time interval from stimulus presentation to the peak amplitude value for each condition (Kiesel et al., 2008). ERP data could not be recorded in three cases due to technical issues. Data from these three participants were excluded during post-processing (NS group: 19; SD group: 20).
EEG data were pre-processed using the SCAN 4.3 software (Neuroscan, Inc., United States), following which ocular artifacts were removed via regression analysis. The data were bandpass filtered at 0.05–30Hz (frequency slope: 24dB/oct) and were divided into 900-ms epochs (−100ms to 800ms). A period of 100ms prior to stimulation was included for baseline correction. Trials in which the voltage exceeded ±100μV were excluded. Non-physiological artifacts mainly included those generated from the contact between the electrode and the scalp to the device or to the environment (i.e., the environment around the device or the device in the participant). Generally, non-physiological artifacts display various waveforms, which may preclude data interpretation in severe cases (Maddirala and Shaik, 2016). The mean number of accepted trials was 92.4±17.46 (NS: 95.6±7.85 vs. SD: 89.2±27.07, p > 0.05). As guided by previous studies, P3 (250–450ms) and N2 components (150–350ms) were analyzed for the following channels: F3, Fz, F4, C3, Cz, C4, P3, Pz, and P4 (Zhang et al., 2019; Peng et al., 2020).
Repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to analyze both the behavioral data and ERP findings using SPSS (version 22.0, IBM Corp., United States).
2.6. Statistical analysis of data from the NS and SD groups
For ERP analyses, we assessed the main and interaction effects of the groups (NS and SD), sleep states (NS-BS and NS-SD0; SD-SD0 and SD-SD36), tasks (PWM, SWM, and OWM), regions (frontal, central, and parietal), and sites (left, middle, and right). Behavioral data were compared between the two groups (NS and RS), two sleep states (no-sleep deprived: NS-BS and NS-SD0; sleep deprived: SD-SD0 and SD-SD36), and three tasks (PWM, SWM, and OWM). Greenhouse–Geisser corrections for non-sphericity and post hoc tests with Bonferroni correction were performed. The results are presented as the mean and standard deviation.
2.7. Statistical analysis of data from the SD groups
To analyze ERP data, we assessed the main and interaction effects of sleep states (SD-SD0, SD-SD36, and RS-8h), tasks (PWM, SWM, and OWM), regions (frontal, central, and parietal), and sites (left, middle, and right). We analyzed the same behavioral parameters as those mentioned above, and compared behavioral data between the three sleep states (RS-SD0, RS-SD36, and RS-8h) and three tasks (PWM, SWM, and OWM). Greenhouse–Geisser corrections for non-sphericity and post hoc tests with Bonferroni correction were performed. The results are presented as the mean and standard deviation.
3. Results
3.1. Results of NS vs. SD comparisons
3.1.1. Behavioral performance
The mean reaction time, accuracy, and number of correct responses per unit time are presented in Tables 1, ,2.2. For accuracy (F(1, 37)=16.420, p<0.001, η2 p =0.307) and number of correct responses per unit time (F(1, 37)=4.869, p=0.034, η2 p =0.116), there were significant interaction effects between group and sleep state. These results suggested that in the SD group, accuracy (p<0.001) and the number of correct responses per unit of time (p=0.009) decreased significantly after 36h of TSD. However, there was no statistically significant differences between the two states in the NS group (Figure 4). No other main or interaction effects were statistically significant.
Table 1
SD-0h | SD-36h | RS-8h | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
PWM | SWM | OWM | PWM | SWM | OWM | PWM | SWM | OWM | |
Mean reaction time (ms) | 539.94 (101.20) | 520.16 (91.58) | 522.19 (88.44) | 561.88 (104.23) | 527.46 (94.15) | 543.85 (107.88) | 528.09 (66.34) | 498.80 (84.98) | 526.07 (80.19) |
Correct rate (%) | 0.89 (0.10) | 0.95 (0.04) | 0.88 (0.09) | 0.82 (0.13) | 0.85 (0.12) | 0.79 (0.13) | 0.88 (0.08) | 0.93 (0.04) | 0.87 (0.07) |
Correct number/s | 1.73 (0.45) | 1.88 (0.35) | 1.76 (0.44) | 1.53 (0.40) | 1.68 (0.41) | 1.53 (0.45) | 1.70 (0.30) | 1.93 (0.36) | 1.69 (0.34) |
PWM, pronunciation working memory; SWM, spatial working memory; OWM, object working memory.
Table 2
BS | SD-0h | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
PWM | SWM | OWM | PWM | SWM | OWM | |
Mean reaction time (ms) | 562.51 (163.77) | 518.58 (137.09) | 519.83 (117.88) | 557.28 (165.27) | 495.90 (136.18) | 538.57 (134.62) |
Correct rate (%) | 0.90 (0.10) | 0.93 (0.05) | 0.90 (0.07) | 0.90 (0.08) | 0.94 (0.04) | 0.90 (0.07) |
Correct number/s | 1.74 (0.53) | 1.92 (0.50) | 1.83 (0.48) | 1.76 (0.54) | 2.04 (0.55) | 1.79 (0.48) |
PWM, pronunciation working memory; SWM, spatial working memory; OWM, object working memory.
3.1.2. The N2 component
The descriptive statistics for the N2 component of the NS group are presented in Tables 3, ,4.4. N2 latency (F(1, 37)=4.426, p=0.042, η2 p =0.107) was affected by significant interaction effects between group and sleep state. N2 latency was significantly prolonged after a 36h TSD in the SD group (p=0.002). However, there was no difference in N2 latency between the two sleep states in the NS group (p=0.085). For N2 amplitude, there were no significant interaction effects between the groups and sleep states (F(1, 37)=0.004, p=0.947, η2 p <0.001). No other main or interaction effects were statistically significant.
Table 3
Baseline | SD-0h | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
N2 | P3 | N2 | P3 | ||
F3 | M (SD) | 238.94 (29.63) | 363.22 (33.47) | 241.55 (32.17) | 363.44 (30.42) |
Fz | M (SD) | 237.69 (29.24) | 359.19 (33.29) | 242.07 (31.22) | 358.87 (33.56) |
F4 | M (SD) | 232.58 (27.75) | 360.64 (31.81) | 230.14 (28.15) | 359.71 (32.25) |
C3 | M (SD) | 228.34 (26.27) | 353.49 (31.16) | 228.347 (30.14) | 358.41 (32.03) |
Cz | M (SD) | 222.83 (23.89) | 351.83 (30.23) | 227.66 (27.92) | 350.44 (32.31) |
C4 | M (SD) | 220.66 (24.04) | 359.01 (28.57) | 218.66 (26.21) | 353.82 (29.44) |
P3 | M (SD) | 206.81 (31.96) | 346.72 (28.67) | 207.25 (29.44) | 347.09 (38.32) |
Pz | M (SD) | 207.52 (23.71) | 345.30 (31.13) | 214.69 (29.20) | 347.03 (36.55) |
P4 | M (SD) | 212.61 (32.96) | 322.81 (31.84) | 205.50 (33.84) | 332.04 (32.02) |
Table 4
Baseline | SD-0h | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
N2 | P3 | N2 | P3 | ||
F3 | M (SD) | −2.90 (3.94) | 6.97 (2.37) | −2.11 (3.80) | 5.76 (2.89) |
Fz | M (SD) | −3.22 (4.40) | 7.79 (2.36) | −2.22 (3.97) | 6.63 (3.48) |
F4 | M (SD) | −2.45 (4.05) | 8.13 (2.60) | −1.54 (3.75) | 7.40 (3.24) |
C3 | M (SD) | −2.04 (3.61) | 7.73 (2.06) | −1.94 (2.86) | 6.91 (2.97) |
Cz | M (SD) | −1.66 (4.24) | 9.38 (2.26) | −1.95 (3.52) | 8.02 (3.41) |
C4 | M (SD) | −1.02 (3.50) | 9.30 (2.34) | −1.45 (3.13) | 8.17 (3.26) |
P3 | M (SD) | −2.89 (5.50) | 7.76 (2.49) | −2.20 (4.84) | 7.80 (3.01) |
Pz | M (SD) | −1.08 (3.59) | 8.51 (2.68) | −1.76 (3.63) | 8.21 (2.73) |
P4 | M (SD) | −1.17 (4.00) | 7.02 (2.43) | −2.34 (5.35) | 7.74 (3.07) |
3.1.3. The P3 component
The descriptive statistics for the P3 component of the NS group are presented in Tables 3, ,4.4. For P3 latency (F(1, 37) =0.002, p =0.962, η2 p <0.001) and P3 amplitude (F(1, 37) =0.902, p =0.348, η2 p =0.024), there were no significant interaction effects between group and sleep state. However, a simple effect analysis revealed that before sleep deprivation, the P3 amplitude was significantly lower in the SD group (p =0.013). The average amplitudes and latencies of P3 as elicited by the nine electrode sites are presented in Figures 5, ,6.6. No other main or interaction effects were statistically significant.
3.2. Results of SD vs. RS comparisons
3.2.1. Behavioral performance
The results of the behavioral experiments are presented in Table 1. The mean reaction time tended to be longer after the 36h TSD and tended to be shorter after 8h of RS, although the differences were not significant (F(2, 38)=1.07, p=0.353, η2 p =0.053). For accuracy (F(2, 38)=17.023, p<0.001, η2 p =0.473) and the number of correct responses per unit time (F(2, 38)=5.893, p=0.006, η2 p =0.237), there was a significant main effect of sleep state. The accuracy and number of correct responses per unit time were significantly decreased after the 36h TSD (accuracy: p<0.001; number of correct responses per unit time: p=0.02) and were restored after the 8h RS (accuracy: p<0.001; number of correct responses per unit time: p=0.006) (Figure 7). No other main or interaction effects were statistically significant.
3.2.2. The N2 component
The descriptive statistics for the N2 component of the RS group are presented in Tables 5, ,6.6. For the N2 latency, there was a significant main effect of sleep state (F(2, 38)=4.511, p=0.017, η2 p =0.192). After the 36h TSD, the N2 latency (p=0.01) increased significantly and showed a decreasing trend after the 8h RS (p=0.11). There was no main effect of sleep state on the amplitude of N2 (F(2, 38)=0.465, p=0.631, η2 p =0.024). No other main or interaction effects were statistically significant.
Table 5
SD-0h | SD-36h | RS-8h | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N2 | P3 | N2 | P3 | N2 | P3 | ||
F3 | M (SD) | 232.57 (36.77) | 372.41 (33.91) | 250.74 (49.32) | 374.85 (33.49) | 243.72 (39.16) | 372.36 (36.55) |
Fz | M (SD) | 235.89 (38.35) | 370.90 (34.97) | 247.39 (44.22) | 378.39 (32.22) | 242.68 (36.11) | 369.26 (35.48) |
F4 | M (SD) | 229.21 (36.35) | 371.33 (31.72) | 248.93 (48.36) | 377.63 (33.64) | 238.15 (39.51) | 370.58 (33.27) |
C3 | M (SD) | 217.58 (33.88) | 367.87 (33.40) | 231.75 (46.67) | 369.35 (29.87) | 225.80 (39.55) | 363.95 (38.51) |
Cz | M (SD) | 224.36 (36.51) | 361.43 (34.43) | 229.79 (45.19) | 370.04 (34.69) | 230.90 (40.53) | 357.77 (35.88) |
C4 | M (SD) | 215.79 (35.35) | 368.32 (27.19) | 230.50 (44.68) | 368.90 (34.81) | 229.42 (39.82) | 360.26 (37.63) |
P3 | M (SD) | 207.55 (45.76) | 359.62 (34.36) | 209.64 (34.93) | 362.05 (40.21) | 211.55 (39.28) | 352.95 (36.65) |
Pz | M (SD) | 206.46 (31.04) | 353.29 (36.24) | 223.56 (43.61) | 352.26 (37.77) | 211.94 (34.55) | 348.34 (41.02) |
P4 | M (SD) | 209.79 (46.66) | 334.74 (39.05) | 222.84 (45.27) | 337.95 (45.44) | 212.25 (45.42) | 338.58 (45.31) |
Table 6
SD-0h | SD-36h | RS-8h | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N2 | P3 | N2 | P3 | N2 | P3 | ||
F3 | M (SD) | −2.50 (4.17) | 7.80 (3.35) | −3.27 (4.93) | 6.33 (3.21) | −3.35 (2.92) | 7.74 (4.33) |
Fz | M (SD) | −2.72 (3.95) | 8.31 (3.44) | −3.48 (5.87) | 7.85 (3.87) | −4.03 (3.44) | 8.47 (5.12) |
F4 | M (SD) | −1.88 (3.66) | 8.93 (3.38) | −2.69 (4.58) | 7.11 (3.85) | −3.06 (3.42) | 9.09 (4.67) |
C3 | M (SD) | −2.04 (4.03) | 8.49 (3.39) | −2.09 (4.75) | 6.88 (3.56) | −2.30 (2.62) | 8.92 (4.52) |
Cz | M (SD) | −1.93 (4.51) | 9.64 (3.65) | −1.68 (5.36) | 8.14 (3.28) | −2.20 (3.69) | 10.36 (5.20) |
C4 | M (SD) | −1.36 (3.63) | 9.05 (3.03) | −0.93 (4.29) | 7.58 (3.38) | −1.08 (3.03) | 10.35 (4.46) |
P3 | M (SD) | −2.55 (5.08) | 8.51 (3.82) | −2.53 (4.93) | 7.27 (3.55) | −2.81 (4.87) | 8.71 (4.19) |
Pz | M (SD) | −0.68 (4.17) | 9.20 (4.07) | −0.91 (4.21) | 8.36 (3.68) | −1.05 (4.13) | 9.96 (4.22) |
P4 | M (SD) | −1.65 (4.03) | 8.00 (3.41) | −0.93 (4.91) | 7.79 (4.41) | −1.60 (3.99) | 8.30 (3.77) |
3.2.3. The P3 component
The descriptive statistics for the P3 component of the RS group are presented in Tables 5, ,6.6. For P3 amplitude, there was a significant main effect of sleep state (F(1.52, 28.95)=4.948, p=0.012, η2P=0.207). After the 36h TSD, the P3 amplitude (p=0.003) decreased significantly and was restored after the 8h RS (p=0.015). We also observed a significant interaction effect of sleep state and region on the P3 amplitude (F(1.87, 35.55) =2.489, p =0.050, η2P =0.116). During the three sleep states, the fluctuations in P3 were mainly focused in frontal and central regions (Figure 8). For latency of P3, the main effect of and sleep state was not significant (F(2,38)=11.921, p=0.160, η2 p =0.092). No other main or interaction effect was statistically significant.
4. Discussion
In the present study, we analyzed ERPs to investigate the effects of 8h of RS on working memory impairment induced by 36h of TSD. The results of our previous study had revealed that TSD significantly impairs the accuracy of 2-back working memory tasks (Peng et al., 2020). The current behavioral findings demonstrate that 8h of RS can improve both the accuracy of responses and the number of correct responses per unit time in such tasks. Changes in behavioral indicators effectively reflect the improvement in working memory ability, which may also be related to the recovery of vigilance and disposable attentional resources (Doty et al., 2017). Although a constant cognitive load (2-back) was utilized in the present study, our results indicated that working memory ability improved after 8h of RS when compared with the performance observed after 36h of TSD without RS. Moreover, despite there being no marked improvements in reaction time after 8h of RS, the number of correct responses per unit time increased significantly. Changes in accuracy rates can also influence response times; as such, there may be situations where participants sacrifice accuracy to reduce reaction time (de Bruijn et al., 2020). The number of correct responses per unit of time combines reaction time and accuracy and more accurately reflects an individual’s working memory ability and level of cognitive control.
Notably, 8h of RS after TSD also induced a significant decrease in N2 latency and a significant increase in P3 amplitude. The N2 component is considered to reflect an individual’s mental state and level of attention (Schacht et al., 2008), whereas the P3 component is thought to be involved in the decision-making process during cognitive matching tasks (Gosselin et al., 2005). Increases in P3 latency and decreases in P3 amplitude are associated with prolonged wakefulness (Panjwani et al., 2010).In addition, several studies have reported a decrease in reaction time and sustained attention following SD (Rupp et al., 2009; Chua et al., 2014). Thus, our findings are in accordance with previous results and support the notion that 8h of RS can improve performance and alertness (Zhang et al., 2014). The restoration of attention and alertness following RS may have enabled participants to allocate more attentional resources to working memory tasks, thereby attenuating TSD-induced impairments (Donchin and Fabiani, 1991). Previous studies have also indicated that compared with drowsiness, SD is associated with more pronounced decreases in the activation of the frontoparietal network (which in involved in working memory) (Almklov et al., 2015). Furthermore, SD can reduce metabolic activity in regions associated with information processing and executive control (Choo et al., 2005), whereas RS can restore the overall network organization following TSD (Jiang et al., 2018).
Sustained attention and alertness are essential for the performance of daily activities. Based on the observed changes in the N2 and P3 components (i.e., increased amplitude and decreased latency) after 8h of RS, we speculated that RS can effectively attenuate impairments in attention and alertness, thus influencing the information integration process. The deterioration of sustained attention seems to be a long-lasting negative effect of SD (de Bruin et al., 2017; Lowe et al., 2017), and is likely caused by decreased arousal of the central nervous system (CNS) (Schneider and Fisk, 1984; Cote et al., 2009). In contrast, more automatic or bottom-up processes appear to be less affected by changes in CNS arousal (Schneider and Fisk, 1984). Therefore, improvements in sustained attention are likely to occur earlier given the greater sensitivity of sustained attention to SD. The completion of a cognitive task usually requires the joint participation of several psychological processes, including early sensory perception, alertness, basic attentional mechanisms, working memory, and decision making. The P3 component appears relatively late, suggesting that it is more reflective of conscious participation and likely involves top-down cognitive control (Kusztor et al., 2019). The observed increases in amplitude and decreases in latency also suggest that 8h of RS improves the ability to integrate dynamic information during working memory tasks. Communication between the hippocampus and prefrontal areas is vital for the optimal redistribution of temporal memory traces to more resident cortical storage. Therefore, interrupting this communication may impair an individual’s ability to form a new memory. In this regard, Chai et al. recently reported that RS re-normalizes hippocampal connections (Chai et al., 2020).
Normal sleep is divided into two phases: rapid eye movement (REM) and slow-wave sleep (SWS). Deep sleep during the N3 stage of SWS is particularly important for restoring mental and physical energy. Following SD, we observed compensatory responses during the restorative sleep stages. Interestingly, the intensity (rather than the duration) of sleep influences the recovery of function following SD. Sleep intensity during SWS is regarded as an indicator of homeostatic sleep pressure (Borbély, 1982; Hennecke et al., 2019). After one night of SD, less than 10h of RS can sufficiently reduce the level of sleep stress to that observed at the end of a typical 8h period of normal sleep (Daan et al., 1984; Achermann and Borbély, 1994). Nevertheless, further increases in the duration of SWS have been observed on the second night of recovery (Carskadon and Dement, 1985). RS exhibits characteristics distinct from those of normal sleep, including a decrease in sleep-onset latency. Key changes have also been observed during the N2 and N3 stages. In addition, longer periods of RS result in a sleep stage distribution similar to that of normal sleep (Hennecke et al., 2019). Therefore, individuals in the RS group experienced increases in the proportion of SWS during RS relative to the amount observed during normal sleep. It is possible that SD-induced impairments in working memory function are specifically attenuated during SWS.
The behavioral and EEG data obtained in this study support our hypothesis that 8h of RS can attenuate impairments in working memory caused by 36h of TSD. However, we did not observe significant changes in all the indicators identified in our previous study (Peng et al., 2020). Insufficient sleep may lead individuals to provide conservative estimates of their performance, which may increase the likelihood of compensatory behaviors and protect against the negative consequences of SD (Boardman et al., 2018). Therefore, the results of this study should be interpreted with caution. Previous research has demonstrated that simple cognitive responses are less affected by SD and can be easily recovered following RS, whereas impairments in higher-level cognitive functions are less easily reversed (Nilsson et al., 2005; Skurvydas et al., 2020). Improvements in cognitive function following RS are mainly reflected by changes in alertness and sustained attention, which allow participants to allocate more attentional resources to the current task (Jin et al., 2015). Nonetheless, further studies are required to elucidate the mechanisms by which RS restores cognitive function after TSD. The main goal of this study was to further explore the recovery effect of 8h of restorative sleep on impaired cognitive ability based on previous findings (i.e., how sleep deprivation impairs working memory or other cognitive functions). In addition, we also included a blank control group, unlike most prior studies.
The present study has some limitations. We did not assess working memory performance using tasks of varying difficulty, which limits our ability to infer how changes in workload impact the restorative effect of RS. In addition, our study included only male volunteers, and caution should be exercised when attempting to extend our findings to female individuals. All participants in this study had good sleep quality; however, the impact of sleep deprivation can differ between the normal population and people with insomnia or rhythm disorders. Rhythm disorders can cause changes in an individual’s melatonin secretion cycle, leading to specific deficits in the neurophysiological activity in the attention domain (Gumenyuk et al., 2014). Therefore, our inferences may be limited to an optimally sleeping population. Considering the number of participants in our study and some non-significant findings related to EEG indicators, further studies are required to determine whether 8h of RS can restore cognitive function to baseline levels. In future studies, we plan to use analytical methods based on the power spectrum. Multimodal studies involving brain network analyses of EEG and imaging data can also help further explain our results. We did not use sleep monitoring technology to investigate whether RS induces specific alterations in sleep structure, necessitating further studies in this regard. Finally, circadian biorhythms are known to affect behavioral performance (Montplaisir, 1981), and their effects should also be considered in future studies.
Our results align with those of previous studies, suggesting that 8h of RS can partially attenuate the deleterious effects of TSD on working memory. This study provides experimental evidence of the recovery of cognitive function after acute sleep loss. RS has potential value as an applied non-pharmacological strategy to alleviate the effects of sleep deprivation. For example, failure to maintain a high level of alertness may lead to serious consequences during military missions. The widespread use of high-tech equipment demands high levels of cognitive ability and brain function, and sleep deprivation may become more prominent under high-tech warfare conditions in future. Therefore, strengthening research on sleep deprivation and providing effective medical support under continuous combat conditions is of great significance to the success of military endeavors.
5. Conclusion
In summary, our results suggest that RS may exert its effects by improving alertness and sustained attention in sleep-deprived individuals. Because SWS dominates the sleep period during RS, these restorative effects are likely to occur during SWS. However, RS had limited effects in the present study, and further studies are required to determine whether 8h of RS can restore cognitive function to baseline levels.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Fourth Military Medical University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
YS, ZP, and YH designed the study. ZP produced the results and wrote the manuscript. LX, HW, SW, and TS contributed data collection and analysis. YY and YS were the guarantors of this study. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
This research was supported by the National Science Foundation of China: 211CXGCM113040301.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
- Achermann P., Borbély A. A. (1994). Simulation of daytime vigilance by the additive interaction of a homeostatic and a circadian process. Biol. Cybern. 71, 115–121. 10.1007/BF00197314, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Alhola P., Polo-Kantola P. (2007). Sleep deprivation: impact on cognitive performance. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 3, 553–567. PMID: [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Almklov E. L., Drummond S. P., Orff H., Alhassoon O. M. (2015). The effects of sleep deprivation on brain functioning in older adults. Behav. Sleep Med. 13, 324–345. 10.1080/15402002.2014.905474 [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson C., Horne J. A. (2006). Sleepiness enhances distraction during a monotonous task. Sleep 29, 573–576. 10.1093/sleep/29.4.573, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson C., Platten C. R. (2011). Sleep deprivation lowers inhibition and enhances impulsivity to negative stimuli. Behav. Brain Res. 217, 463–466. 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.09.020, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley A. (2000). The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 417–423. 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01538-2, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley A. (2010). Working memory. Curr. Biol. 20, R136–R140. 10.1016/j.cub.2009.12.014 [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Banks S., Van Dongen H. P., Maislin G., Dinges D. F. (2010). Neurobehavioral dynamics following chronic sleep restriction: dose-response effects of one night for recovery. Sleep 33, 1013–1026. 10.1093/sleep/33.8.1013, PMID: [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Biggs S. N., Smith A., Dorrian J., Reid K., Dawson D., van den Heuvel C., et al. . (2007). Perception of simulated driving performance after sleep restriction and caffeine. J. Psychosom. Res. 63, 573–577. 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2007.06.017, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Boardman J. M., Bei B., Mellor A., Anderson C., Sletten T. L., Drummond S. P. A. (2018). The ability to self-monitor cognitive performance during 60 h total sleep deprivation and following 2 nights recovery sleep. J. Sleep Res. 27:e12633. 10.1111/jsr.12633, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Borbély A. A. (1982). A two process model of sleep regulation. Hum. Neurobiol. 1, 195–204. PMID: [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Buysse D. J., Reynolds C. F., 3rd, Monk T. H., Berman S. R., Kupfer D. J. (1989). The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 28, 193–213. 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Carskadon M. A., Dement W. C. (1985). Sleep loss in elderly volunteers. Sleep 8, 207–221. 10.1093/sleep/8.3.207 [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Chai Y., Fang Z., Yang F. N., Xu S., Deng Y., Raine A., et al. . (2020). Two nights of recovery sleep restores hippocampal connectivity but not episodic memory after total sleep deprivation. Sci. Rep. 10:8774. 10.1038/s41598-020-65086-x, PMID: [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Choo W. C., Lee W. W., Venkatraman V., Sheu F. S., Chee M. W. (2005). Dissociation of cortical regions modulated by both working memory load and sleep deprivation and by sleep deprivation alone. NeuroImage 25, 579–587. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.11.029, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Chua E. C., Yeo S. C., Lee I. T., Tan L. C., Lau P., Cai S., et al. . (2014). Sustained attention performance during sleep deprivation associates with instability in behavior and physiologic measures at baseline. Sleep 37, 27–39. 10.5665/sleep.3302, PMID: [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Cote K. A., Milner C. E., Smith B. A., Aubin A. J., Greason T. A., Cuthbert B. P., et al. . (2009). CNS arousal and neurobehavioral performance in a short-term sleep restriction paradigm. J. Sleep Res. 18, 291–303. 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00733.x, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Daan S., Beersma D. G., Borbély A. A. (1984). Timing of human sleep: recovery process gated by a circadian pacemaker. Am. J. Phys. 246, R161–R183. 10.1152/ajpregu.1984.246.2.R161, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- de Bruijn E. R. A., Jansen M., Overgaauw S. (2020). Enhanced error-related brain activations for mistakes that harm others: ERP evidence from a novel social performance-monitoring paradigm. NeuroImage 204:116238. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116238, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- de Bruin E. J., van Run C., Staaks J., Meijer A. M. (2017). Effects of sleep manipulation on cognitive functioning of adolescents: a systematic review. Sleep Med. Rev. 32, 45–57. 10.1016/j.smrv.2016.02.006, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Dehaene S., Changeux J. P. (2011). Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron 70, 200–227. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.018 [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Dinges D. F. (1986). Differential effects of prior wakefulness and circadian phase on nap sleep. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 64, 224–227. 10.1016/0013-4694(86)90170-7, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Dinges D. F. (1990). “Are you awake? Cognitive performance and reverie during the hypnopompic state” in Sleep and Cognition. eds. Bootzin R. R., Kihlstrom J. F., Schacter D. L., editors. (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; ), 159–175. [Google Scholar]
- Djonlagic I., Rosenfeld A., Shohamy D., Myers C., Gluck M., Stickgold R. (2009). Sleep enhances category learning. Learn. Mem. 16, 751–755. 10.1101/lm.1634509, PMID: [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Donchin E., Fabiani M. (1991). The Use of Event-Related Brain Potentials in the Study of Memory: Is P300 a Measure of event distinctiveness?. C Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Center for International Studies. Cambridge, MA. 44, 58–77. [Google Scholar]
- Doty T. J., So C. J., Bergman E. M., Trach S. K., Ratcliffe R. H., Yarnell A. M., et al. . (2017). Limited efficacy of caffeine and recovery costs during and following 5 days of chronic sleep restriction. Sleep 40:zsx171. 10.1093/sleep/zsx171, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Drummond S. P., Paulus M. P., Tapert S. F. (2006). Effects of two nights sleep deprivation and two nights recovery sleep on response inhibition. J. Sleep Res. 15, 261–265. 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2006.00535.x, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Folstein J. R., Van Petten C. (2008). Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: a review. Psychophysiology 45, 152–170. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00602.x, PMID: [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Gerhardsson A., Åkerstedt T., Axelsson J., Fischer H., Lekander M., Schwarz J. (2019). Effect of sleep deprivation on emotional working memory. J. Sleep Res. 28:e12744. 10.1111/jsr.12744, PMID: [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Gosselin A., De Koninck J., Campbell K. B. (2005). Total sleep deprivation and novelty processing: implications for frontal lobe functioning. Clin. Neurophysiol. 116, 211–222. 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.07.033, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Gumenyuk V., Howard R., Roth T., Korzyukov O., Drake C. L. (2014). Sleep loss, circadian mismatch, and abnormalities in reorienting of attention in night workers with shift work disorder. Sleep 37, 545–556. 10.5665/sleep.3494, PMID: [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Hennecke E., Elmenhorst D., Mendolia F., Putzke M., Bauer A., Aeschbach D., et al. . (2019). Reestablishment of individual sleep structure during a single 14-h recovery sleep episode after 58 h of wakefulness. J. Sleep Res. 28:e12641. 10.1111/jsr.12641, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Hilditch C. J., Dorrian J., Banks S. (2017). A review of short naps and sleep inertia: do naps of 30 min or less really avoid sleep inertia and slow-wave sleep? Sleep Med. 32, 176–190. 10.1016/j.sleep.2016.12.016, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Honn K. A., Halverson T., Jackson M. L., Krusmark M., Chavali V. P., Gunzelmann G., et al. . (2020). New insights into the cognitive effects of sleep deprivation by decomposition of a cognitive throughput task. Sleep 43:zsz319. 10.1093/sleep/zsz319, PMID: [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Huster R. J., Westerhausen R., Pantev C., Konrad C. (2010). The role of the cingulate cortex as neural generator of the N200 and P300 in a tactile response inhibition task. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 1260–1271. 10.1002/hbm.20933, PMID: [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Jiang Y., Chai Y., Yang F., Xu S., Basner M., Detre J. A., et al. . (2018). 0218 Effects of sleep deprivation and recovery sleep on human brain network organization. Sleep 41, A85–A86. 10.1093/sleep/zsy061.217 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Jin X., Ye E., Qi J., Wang L., Lei Y., Chen P., et al. . (2015). Recovery sleep reverses impaired response inhibition due to sleep restriction: evidence from a visual event related potentials study. PLoS One 10:e0142361. 10.1371/journal.pone.0142361, PMID: [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Kiesel A., Miller J., Jolicoeur P., Brisson B. (2008). Measurement of ERP latency differences: a comparison of single-participant and jackknife-based scoring methods. Psychophysiology 45, 250–274. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00618.x, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Killgore W. D., Balkin T. J., Wesensten N. J. (2006). Impaired decision making following 49 h of sleep deprivation. J. Sleep Res. 15, 7–13. 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2006.00487.x, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Koslowsky M., Babkoff H. (1992). Meta-analysis of the relationship between total sleep deprivation and performance. Chronobiol. Int. 9, 132–136. 10.3109/07420529209064524, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Kusztor A., Raud L., Juel B. E., Nilsen A. S., Storm J. F., Huster R. J. (2019). Sleep deprivation differentially affects subcomponents of cognitive control. Sleep 42:zsz016. 10.1093/sleep/zsz016, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Lo J. C., Groeger J. A., Santhi N., Arbon E. L., Lazar A. S., Hasan S., et al. . (2012). Effects of partial and acute total sleep deprivation on performance across cognitive domains, individuals and circadian phase. PLoS One 7:e45987. 10.1371/journal.pone.0045987, PMID: [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Lo J. C., Ong J. L., Leong R. L., Gooley J. J., Chee M. W. (2016). Cognitive performance, sleepiness, and mood in partially sleep deprived adolescents: the need for sleep study. Sleep 39, 687–698. 10.5665/sleep.5552, PMID: [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Lowe C. J., Safati A., Hall P. A. (2017). The neurocognitive consequences of sleep restriction: a meta-analytic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 80, 586–604. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.010, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Maddirala A. K., Shaik R. A. (2016). Motion artifact removal from single channel electroencephalogram signals using singular spectrum analysis. Biomed. Signal Process. Control. 30, 79–85. 10.1016/j.bspc.2016.06.017 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Malik J., Lo Y. L., Wu H. T. (2018). Sleep-wake classification via quantifying heart rate variability by convolutional neural network. Physiol. Meas. 39:085004. 10.1088/1361-6579/aad5a9, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Miccoli L., Versace F., Koterle S., Cavallero C. (2008). Comparing sleep-loss sleepiness and sleep inertia: lapses make the difference. Chronobiol. Int. 25, 725–744. 10.1080/07420520802397228, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Montplaisir J. (1981). Depression and biological rhythm: therapeutic effects of sleep deprivation. Union Med. Can. 110, 272–276. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Nilsson J. P., Söderström M., Karlsson A. U., Lekander M., Akerstedt T., Lindroth N. E., et al. . (2005). Less effective executive functioning after one night’s sleep deprivation. J. Sleep Res. 14, 1–6. 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2005.00442.x, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Panjwani U., Ray K., Chatterjee A., Bhaumik S., Kumar S. (2010). Electrophysiological correlates of cognition improve with nap during sleep deprivation. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 108, 549–556. 10.1007/s00421-009-1222-3, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Peng Z., Dai C., Ba Y., Zhang L., Shao Y., Tian J. (2020). Effect of sleep deprivation on the working memory-related N2-P3 components of the event-related potential waveform. Front. Neurosci. 14:469. 10.3389/fnins.2020.00469, PMID: [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Polich J. (2007). Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 2128–2148. 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019, PMID: [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Raud L., Huster R. J. (2017). The temporal dynamics of response inhibition and their modulation by cognitive control. Brain Topogr. 30, 486–501. 10.1007/s10548-017-0566-y, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Roehrs T. A., Harris E., Randall S., Roth T. (2012). Pain sensitivity and recovery from mild chronic sleep loss. Sleep 35, 1667–1672. 10.5665/sleep.2240, PMID: [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Rogers P. J., Heatherley S. V., Hayward R. C., Seers H. E., Hill J., Kane M. (2005). Effects of caffeine and caffeine withdrawal on mood and cognitive performance degraded by sleep restriction. Psychopharmacology 179, 742–752. 10.1007/s00213-004-2097-y, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenthal L., Roehrs T., Zwyghuizen-Doorenbos A., Plath D., Roth T. (1991). Alerting effects of caffeine after normal and restricted sleep. Neuropsychopharmacology 4, 103–108. PMID: [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Ruggiero J. S., Redeker N. S. (2014). Effects of napping on sleepiness and sleep-related performance deficits in night-shift workers: a systematic review. Biol. Res. Nurs. 16, 134–142. 10.1177/1099800413476571, PMID: [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Rupp T. L., Wesensten N. J., Bliese P. D., Balkin T. J. (2009). Banking sleep: realization of benefits during subsequent sleep restriction and recovery. Sleep 32, 311–321. 10.1093/sleep/32.3.311, PMID: [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Saletin J. M., Goldstein-Piekarski A. N., Greer S. M., Stark S., Stark C. E., Walker M. P. (2016). Human hippocampal structure: a novel biomarker predicting mnemonic vulnerability to, and recovery from, sleep deprivation. J. Neurosci. 36, 2355–2363. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3466-15.2016, PMID: [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Schacht A., Werheid K., Sommer W. (2008). The appraisal of facial beauty is rapid but not mandatory. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 8, 132–142. 10.3758/cabn.8.2.132 [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider W., Fisk A. D. (1984). Automatic category search and its transfer. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 10, 1–15. 10.1037//0278-7393.10.1.1, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Skurvydas A., Zlibinaite L., Solianik R., Brazaitis M., Valanciene D., Baranauskiene N., et al. . (2020). One night of sleep deprivation impairs executive function but does not affect psychomotor or motor performance. Biol. Sport 37, 7–14. 10.5114/biolsport.2020.89936, PMID: [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Smallwood J., Brown K., Baird B., Schooler J. W. (2012). Cooperation between the default mode network and the frontal-parietal network in the production of an internal train of thought. Brain Res. 1428, 60–70. 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.03.072, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Spano G. M., Banningh S. W., Marshall W., de Vivo L., Bellesi M., Loschky S. S., et al. . (2019). Sleep deprivation by exposure to novel objects increases synapse density and axon-spine Interface in the hippocampal CA1 region of adolescent mice. J. Neurosci. 39, 6613–6625. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0380-19.2019, PMID: [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Stickgold R. (2005). Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature 437, 1272–1278. 10.1038/nature04286 [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Stroemel-Scheder C., Kundermann B., Lautenbacher S. (2020). The effects of recovery sleep on pain perception: a systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 113, 408–425. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.03.028, PMID: [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Studte S., Bridger E., Mecklinger A. (2015). Nap sleep preserves associative but not item memory performance. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 120, 84–93. 10.1016/j.nlm.2015.02.012 [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Verleger R., Jaśkowski P., Wascher E. (2005). Evidence for an integrative role of P3b in linking reaction to perception. J. Psychophysiol. 19, 165–181. 10.1027/0269-8803.19.3.165 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Walker M. P., Stickgold R. (2004). Sleep-dependent learning and memory consolidation. Neuron 44, 121–133. 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.031 [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Wen X., Liu Y., Yao L., Ding M. (2013). Top-down regulation of default mode activity in spatial visual attention. J. Neurosci. 33, 6444–6453. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4939-12.2013, PMID: [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang Q., Liao Y., Qi J., Zhao Y., Zhu T., Liu Z., et al. . (2014). A visual ERP study of impulse inhibition following a zaleplon-induced nap after sleep deprivation. PLoS One 9:e95653. 10.1371/journal.pone.0095653, PMID: [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang L., Shao Y., Liu Z., Li C., Chen Y., Zhou Q. (2019). Decreased information replacement of working memory after sleep deprivation: evidence from an event-related potential study. Front. Neurosci. 13:408. 10.3389/fnins.2019.00408, PMID: [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
Articles from Frontiers in Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA
Full text links
Read article at publisher's site: https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2023.1056788
Read article for free, from open access legal sources, via Unpaywall: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2023.1056788/pdf
Similar Articles
To arrive at the top five similar articles we use a word-weighted algorithm to compare words from the Title and Abstract of each citation.
Decrease in the P2 Amplitude of Object Working Memory after 8 h-Recovery Sleep Following 36 h-Total Sleep Deprivation: An ERP Study.
Brain Sci, 13(10):1470, 18 Oct 2023
Cited by: 0 articles | PMID: 37891837 | PMCID: PMC10605542
Effect of Sleep Deprivation on the Working Memory-Related N2-P3 Components of the Event-Related Potential Waveform.
Front Neurosci, 14:469, 19 May 2020
Cited by: 18 articles | PMID: 32508572 | PMCID: PMC7248549
Modafinil ameliorates the decline in pronunciation-related working memory caused by 36-h acute total sleep deprivation: An ERP study.
Neurobiol Learn Mem, 192:107625, 30 Apr 2022
Cited by: 5 articles | PMID: 35504555
Total Sleep Deprivation Impairs Lateralization of Spatial Working Memory in Young Men.
Front Neurosci, 14:562035, 06 Oct 2020
Cited by: 10 articles | PMID: 33122988 | PMCID: PMC7573126