Nothing Special   »   [go: up one dir, main page]

Dendritic spine

(Redirected from Dendritic spines)

A dendritic spine (or spine) is a small membrane protrusion from a neuron's dendrite that typically receives input from a single axon at the synapse. Dendritic spines serve as a storage site for synaptic strength and help transmit electrical signals to the neuron's cell body. Most spines have a bulbous head (the spine head), and a thin neck that connects the head of the spine to the shaft of the dendrite. The dendrites of a single neuron can contain hundreds to thousands of spines. In addition to spines providing an anatomical substrate for memory storage and synaptic transmission, they may also serve to increase the number of possible contacts between neurons.[1] It has also been suggested that changes in the activity of neurons have a positive effect on spine morphology.[2]

Dendritic spine
Spiny dendrite of a striatal medium spiny neuron.
Common types of dendritic spines.
Details
Identifiers
Latingemmula dendritica
MeSHD049229
THH2.00.06.1.00036
Anatomical terms of microanatomy

Structure

edit

Dendritic spines are small with spine head volumes ranging 0.01 μm3 to 0.8 μm3. Spines with strong synaptic contacts typically have a large spine head, which connects to the dendrite via a membranous neck. The most notable classes of spine shape are "thin", "stubby", "mushroom", and "bifurcated". Electron microscopy studies have shown that there is a continuum of shapes between these categories.[3] The variable spine shape and volume is thought to be correlated with the strength and maturity of each spine-synapse.

Distribution

edit

Dendritic spines usually receive excitatory input from axons, although sometimes both inhibitory and excitatory connections are made onto the same spine head.[4] Excitatory axon proximity to dendritic spines is not sufficient to predict the presence of a synapse, as demonstrated by the Lichtman lab in 2015.[5]

Spines are found on the dendrites of most principal neurons in the brain, including the pyramidal neurons of the neocortex, the medium spiny neurons of the striatum, and the Purkinje cells of the cerebellum. Dendritic spines occur at a density of up to 5 spines/1 μm stretch of dendrite. Hippocampal and cortical pyramidal neurons may receive tens of thousands of mostly excitatory inputs from other neurons onto their equally numerous spines, whereas the number of spines on Purkinje neuron dendrites is an order of magnitude larger.

Cytoskeleton and organelles

edit

The cytoskeleton of dendritic spines is particularly important in their synaptic plasticity; without a dynamic cytoskeleton, spines would be unable to rapidly change their volumes or shapes in responses to stimuli. These changes in shape might affect the electrical properties of the spine. The cytoskeleton of dendritic spines is primarily made of filamentous actin (F-actin). tubulin Monomers and microtubule-associated proteins (MAPs) are present, and organized microtubules are present.[6] Because spines have a cytoskeleton of primarily actin, this allows them to be highly dynamic in shape and size. The actin cytoskeleton directly determines the morphology of the spine, and actin regulators, small GTPases such as Rac, RhoA, and CDC42, rapidly modify this cytoskeleton. Overactive Rac1 results in consistently smaller dendritic spines.

In addition to their electrophysiological activity and their receptor-mediated activity, spines appear to be vesicularly active and may even translate proteins. Stacked discs of the smooth endoplasmic reticulum (SERs) have been identified in dendritic spines. Formation of this "spine apparatus" depends on the protein synaptopodin and is believed to play an important role in calcium handling. "Smooth" vesicles have also been identified in spines, supporting the vesicular activity in dendritic spines. The presence of polyribosomes in spines also suggests protein translational activity in the spine itself, not just in the dendrite.

Morphogenesis

edit

The morphogenesis of dendritic spines is critical to the induction of long-term potentiation (LTP).[7][8] The morphology of the spine depends on the states of actin, either in globular (G-actin) or filamentous (F-actin) forms. The role of Rho family of GTPases and its effects in the stability of actin and spine motility[9] has important implications for memory. If the dendritic spine is the basic unit of information storage, then the spine's ability to extend and retract spontaneously must be constrained. If not, information may be lost. Rho family of GTPases makes significant contributions to the process that stimulates actin polymerization, which in turn increases the size and shape of the spine.[10] Large spines are more stable than smaller ones and may be resistant to modification by additional synaptic activity.[11] Because changes in the shape and size of dendritic spines are correlated with the strength of excitatory synaptic connections and heavily depend on remodeling of its underlying actin cytoskeleton,[12] the specific mechanisms of actin regulation, and therefore the Rho family of GTPases, are integral to the formation, maturation, and plasticity of dendritic spines and to learning and memory.

RhoA pathway

edit

One of the major Rho GTPases involved in spine morphogenesis is RhoA, a protein that also modulates the regulation and timing of cell division. In the context of activity in neurons, RhoA is activated in the following manner: once calcium has entered a cell through NMDA receptors, it binds to calmodulin and activates CaMKII, which leads to the activation of RhoA.[10] The activation of the RhoA protein will activate ROCK, a RhoA kinase, which leads to the stimulation of LIM kinase, which in turn inhibits the protein cofilin. Cofilin's function is to reorganize the actin cytoskeleton of a cell; namely, it depolymerizes actin segments and thus inhibits the growth of growth cones and the repair of axons.[13]

A study conducted by Murakoshi et al. in 2011 implicated the Rho GTPases RhoA and Cdc42 in dendritic spine morphogenesis. Both GTPases were quickly activated in single dendritic spines of pyramidal neurons in the CA1 region of the rat hippocampus during structural plasticity brought on by long-term potentiation stimuli. Concurrent RhoA and Cdc42 activation led to a transient increase in spine growth of up to 300% for five minutes, which decayed into a smaller but sustained growth for thirty minutes.[10] The activation of RhoA diffused around the vicinity of the spine undergoing stimulation, and it was determined that RhoA is necessary for the transient phase and most likely the sustained phase as well of spine growth.

Cdc42 pathway

edit

Cdc42 has been implicated in many different functions including dendritic growth, branching, and branch stability.[14] Calcium influx into the cell through NMDA receptors binds to calmodulin and activates the Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinases II (CaMKII). In turn, CaMKII is activated and this activates Cdc42, after which no feedback signaling occurs upstream to calcium and CaMKII. If tagged with monomeric-enhanced green fluorescent protein, one can see that the activation of Cdc42 is limited to just the stimulated spine of a dendrite. This is because the molecule is continuously activated during plasticity and immediately inactivates after diffusing out of the spine. Despite its compartmentalized activity, Cdc42 is still mobile out of the stimulated spine, just like RhoA. Cdc42 activates PAK, which is a protein kinase that specifically phosphorylates and, therefore, inactivates ADF/cofilin.[15] Inactivation of cofilin leads to increased actin polymerization and expansion of the spine's volume. Activation of Cdc42 is required for this increase in spinal volume to be sustained.

Observed changes in structural plasticity

edit
 
Calcium influx through NMDA receptors activates CAMKII. CAMKII then regulates several other signaling cascades that modulate the activity of the actin-binding proteins cofilin and profilin. These cascades can be divided into two primary pathways, the RhoA and Cdc42 pathways, which are mediated primarily by these members of the Rho family of GTPases. In the transient stage, the signaling cascade caused by synaptic activity results in LIMK1 phosphorylating ADF/cofilin via both the RhoA and Cdc42 pathways, which in turn inhibits the depolymerization of F-actin and increases the volume of the dendritic spine drastically while also inducing LTP.

Murakoshi, Wang, and Yasuda (2011) examined the effects of Rho GTPase activation on the structural plasticity of single dendritic spines elucidating differences between the transient and sustained phases.[10]

Transient changes in structural plasticity

edit

Applying a low-frequency train of two-photon glutamate uncaging in a single dendritic spine can elicit rapid activation of both RhoA and Cdc42. During the next two minutes, the volume of the stimulated spine can expand to 300 percent of its original size. However, this change in spine morphology is only temporary; the volume of the spine decreases after five minutes. Administration of C3 transferase, a Rho inhibitor, or glycyl-H1152, a Rock inhibitor, inhibits the transient expansion of the spine, indicating that activation of the Rho-Rock pathway is required in some way for this process.[10]

Sustained changes in structural plasticity

edit
 
In contrast, the sustained stage is focused more on activating the RhoA pathway, which ultimately results in a higher concentration of profilin, which prevents additional polymerization of actin and decreases the size of the dendritic spine from the transient stage, though still allows it to remain at an elevated level compared to an unpotentiated spine.

After the transient changes described above take place, the spine's volume decreases until it is elevated by 70 to 80 percent of the original volume. This sustained change in structural plasticity will last about thirty minutes. Once again, administration of C3 transferase and Glycyl-H1152 suppressed this growth, suggesting that the Rho-Rock pathway is necessary for more persistent increases in spinal volume. In addition, administration of the Cdc42 binding domain of Wasp or inhibitor targeting Pak1 activation-3 (IPA3) decreases this sustained growth in volume, demonstrating that the Cdc42-Pak pathway is needed for this growth in spinal volume as well. This is important because sustained changes in structural plasticity may provide a mechanism for the encoding, maintenance, and retrieval of memories. The observations made may suggest that Rho GTPases are necessary for these processes.[10]

Physiology

edit

Receptor activity

edit

Dendritic spines express glutamate receptors (e.g. AMPA receptor and NMDA receptor) on their surface. The TrkB receptor for BDNF is also expressed on the spine surface, and is believed to play a role in spine survival. The tip of the spine contains an electron-dense region referred to as the "postsynaptic density" (PSD). The PSD directly apposes the active zone of its synapsing axon and comprises ~10% of the spine's membrane surface area; neurotransmitters released from the active zone bind receptors in the postsynaptic density of the spine. Half of the synapsing axons and dendritic spines are physically tethered by calcium-dependent cadherin, which forms cell-to-cell adherent junctions between two neurons.

Glutamate receptors (GluRs) are localized to the postsynaptic density, and are anchored by cytoskeletal elements to the membrane. They are positioned directly above their signalling machinery, which is typically tethered to the underside of the plasma membrane, allowing signals transmitted by the GluRs into the cytosol to be further propagated by their nearby signalling elements to activate signal transduction cascades. The localization of signalling elements to their GluRs is particularly important in ensuring signal cascade activation, as GluRs would be unable to affect particular downstream effects without nearby signallers.

Signalling from GluRs is mediated by the presence of an abundance of proteins, especially kinases, that are localized to the postsynaptic density. These include calcium-dependent calmodulin, CaMKII (calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II), PKC (Protein Kinase C), PKA (Protein Kinase A), Protein Phosphatase-1 (PP-1), and Fyn tyrosine kinase. Certain signallers, such as CaMKII, are upregulated in response to activity.

Spines are particularly advantageous to neurons by compartmentalizing biochemical signals. This can help to encode changes in the state of an individual synapse without necessarily affecting the state of other synapses of the same neuron. The length and width of the spine neck has a large effect on the degree of compartmentalization, with thin spines being the most biochemically isolated spines.

Plasticity

edit

Dendritic spines are very "plastic", that is, spines change significantly in shape, volume, and number in small time courses. Because spines have a primarily actin cytoskeleton, they are dynamic, and the majority of spines change their shape within seconds to minutes because of the dynamicity of actin remodeling. Furthermore, spine number is very variable and spines come and go; in a matter of hours, 10-20% of spines can spontaneously appear or disappear on the pyramidal cells of the cerebral cortex, although the larger "mushroom"-shaped spines are the most stable.

Spine maintenance and plasticity is activity-dependent[16] and activity-independent. BDNF partially determines spine levels,[17] and low levels of AMPA receptor activity is necessary to maintain spine survival, and synaptic activity involving NMDA receptors encourages spine growth. Furthermore, two-photon laser scanning microscopy and confocal microscopy have shown that spine volume changes depending on the types of stimuli that are presented to a synapse.

Importance to learning and memory

edit

Evidence of importance

edit
A depiction of spine formation and elimination. 
Experience-dependent spine formation and elimination

Spine plasticity is implicated in motivation, learning, and memory.[18][19][20] In particular, long-term memory is mediated in part by the growth of new dendritic spines (or the enlargement of pre-existing spines) to reinforce a particular neural pathway. Because dendritic spines are plastic structures whose lifespan is influenced by input activity,[21] spine dynamics may play an important role in the maintenance of memory over a lifetime.

Age-dependent changes in the rate of spine turnover suggest that spine stability impacts developmental learning. In youth, dendritic spine turnover is relatively high and produces a net loss of spines.[1][22][23] This high rate of spine turnover may characterize critical periods of development and reflect learning capacity in adolescence—different cortical areas exhibit differing levels of synaptic turnover during development, possibly reflecting varying critical periods for specific brain regions.[19][22] In adulthood, however, most spines remain persistent, and the half-life of spines increases.[1] This stabilization occurs due to a developmentally regulated slow-down of spine elimination, a process which may underlie the stabilization of memories in maturity.[1][22]

Experience-induced changes in dendritic spine stability also point to spine turnover as a mechanism involved in the maintenance of long-term memories, though it is unclear how sensory experience affects neural circuitry. Two general models might describe the impact of experience on structural plasticity. On the one hand, experience and activity may drive the discrete formation of relevant synaptic connections that store meaningful information in order to allow for learning. On the other hand, synaptic connections may be formed in excess, and experience and activity may lead to the pruning of extraneous synaptic connections.[1]

In lab animals of all ages, environmental enrichment has been related to dendritic branching, spine density, and overall number of synapses.[1] In addition, skill training has been shown to lead to the formation and stabilization of new spines while destabilizing old spines,[18][24] suggesting that the learning of a new skill involves a rewiring process of neural circuits. Since the extent of spine remodeling correlates with success of learning, this suggests a crucial role of synaptic structural plasticity in memory formation.[24] In addition, changes in spine stability and strengthening occur rapidly and have been observed within hours after training.[18][19]

Conversely, while enrichment and training are related to increases in spine formation and stability, long-term sensory deprivation leads to an increase in the rate of spine elimination[1][22] and therefore impacts long-term neural circuitry. Upon restoring sensory experience after deprivation in adolescence, spine elimination is accelerated, suggesting that experience plays an important role in the net loss of spines during development.[22] In addition, other sensory deprivation paradigms—such as whisker trimming—have been shown to increase the stability of new spines.[25]

Research in neurological diseases and injuries shed further light on the nature and importance of spine turnover. After stroke, a marked increase in structural plasticity occurs near the trauma site, and a five- to eightfold increase from control rates in spine turnover has been observed.[26] Dendrites disintegrate and reassemble rapidly during ischemia—as with stroke, survivors showed an increase in dendritic spine turnover.[27] While a net loss of spines is observed in Alzheimer's disease and cases of intellectual disability, cocaine and amphetamine use have been linked to increases in dendritic branching and spine density in the prefrontal cortex and the nucleus accumbens.[28] Because significant changes in spine density occur in various brain and spinal cord diseases, this suggests a balanced state of spine dynamics in normal circumstances, which may be susceptible to disequilibrium under varying pathological conditions.[28][29]

There is also some evidence for loss of dendritic spines as a consequence of aging. One study using mice has noted a correlation between age-related reductions in spine densities in the hippocampus and age-dependent declines in hippocampal learning and memory.[30] Emerging evidence has also shown dendritic spine abnormalities in the pain processing regions of the spinal cord nociceptive system, including superficial and intermediate zones of the dorsal horn.[31][29][32][33]

Overall, the evidence suggests that dendritic spines are crucial for normal brain and spinal cord function. Alterations in spine morphology may not only influence synaptic plasticity and information processing but also have a key role in many neurological diseases. Furthermore, even subtle changes in dendritic spine densities or sizes can affect neuronal network properties,[34] which could lead to cognitive or mood alterations, impaired learning and memory, as well as pain hypersensitivity.[29] Moreover, the findings suggest that maintaining spine health through therapies such as exercise, cognitive stimulation and lifestyle modifications may be helpful in preserving neuronal plasticity and improving neurological symptoms.

Importance contested

edit

Despite experimental findings that suggest a role for dendritic spine dynamics in mediating learning and memory, the degree of structural plasticity's importance remains debatable. For instance, studies estimate that only a small portion of spines formed during training actually contribute to lifelong learning.[24] In addition, the formation of new spines may not significantly contribute to the connectivity of the brain, and spine formation may not bear as much of an influence on memory retention as other properties of structural plasticity, such as the increase in size of spine heads.[35]

Modeling

edit

Theoreticians have for decades hypothesized about the potential electrical function of spines, yet our inability to examine their electrical properties has until recently stopped theoretical work from progressing too far. Recent advances in imaging techniques along with increased use of two-photon glutamate uncaging have led to a wealth of new discoveries; we now suspect that there are voltage-dependent sodium,[36] potassium,[37] and calcium[38] channels in the spine heads.[39]

Cable theory provides the theoretical framework behind the most "simple" method for modelling the flow of electrical currents along passive neural fibres. Each spine can be treated as two compartments, one representing the neck, the other representing the spine head. The compartment representing the spine head alone should carry the active properties.

Baer and Rinzel's continuum model

edit

To facilitate the analysis of interactions between many spines, Baer & Rinzel formulated a new cable theory for which the distribution of spines is treated as a continuum.[40] In this representation, spine head voltage is the local spatial average of membrane potential in adjacent spines. The formulation maintains the feature that there is no direct electrical coupling between neighboring spines; voltage spread along dendrites is the only way for spines to interact.

Spike-diffuse-spike model

edit

The SDS model was intended as a computationally simple version of the full Baer and Rinzel model.[41] It was designed to be analytically tractable and have as few free parameters as possible while retaining those of greatest significance, such as spine neck resistance. The model drops the continuum approximation and instead uses a passive dendrite coupled to excitable spines at discrete points. Membrane dynamics in the spines are modelled using integrate and fire processes. The spike events are modelled in a discrete fashion with the wave form conventionally represented as a rectangular function.

Modeling spine calcium transients

edit

Calcium transients in spines are a key trigger for synaptic plasticity.[42] NMDA receptors, which have a high permeability for calcium, only conduct ions if the membrane potential is sufficiently depolarized. The amount of calcium entering a spine during synaptic activity therefore depends on the depolarization of the spine head. Evidence from calcium imaging experiments (two-photon microscopy) and from compartmental modelling indicates that spines with high resistance necks experience larger calcium transients during synaptic activity.[39][43]

Development

edit

Dendritic spines can develop directly from dendritic shafts or from dendritic filopodia.[44] During synaptogenesis, dendrites rapidly sprout and retract filopodia, small membrane organelle-lacking membranous protrusions. Recently, I-BAR protein MIM was found to contribute to the initiation process.[45] During the first week of birth, the brain is predominated by filopodia, which eventually develop synapses. However, after this first week, filopodia are replaced by spiny dendrites but also small, stubby spines that protrude from spiny dendrites. In the development of certain filopodia into spines, filopodia recruit presynaptic contact to the dendrite, which encourages the production of spines to handle specialized postsynaptic contact with the presynaptic protrusions.

Spines, however, require maturation after formation. Immature spines have impaired signaling capabilities, and typically lack "heads" (or have very small heads), only necks, while matured spines maintain both heads and necks.

Clinical significance

edit

Emerging research has indicate abnormalities in spine density in anxiety disorders.[4]

Cognitive disorders such as ADHD, Alzheimer's disease, autism, intellectual disability, and fragile X syndrome, may be resultant from abnormalities in dendritic spines, especially the number of spines and their maturity.[46][47] The ratio of matured to immature spines is important in their signaling, as immature spines have impaired synaptic signaling. Fragile X syndrome is characterized by an overabundance of immature spines that have multiple filopodia in cortical dendrites.

History

edit

Dendritic spines were first described at the end of the 19th century by Santiago Ramón y Cajal on cerebellar neurons.[48] Ramón y Cajal then proposed that dendritic spines could serve as contacting sites between neurons. This was demonstrated more than 50 years later thanks to the emergence of electron microscopy.[49] Until the development of confocal microscopy on living tissues, it was commonly admitted that spines were formed during embryonic development and then would remain stable after birth. In this paradigm, variations of synaptic weight were considered as sufficient to explain memory processes at the cellular level. But since about a decade ago, new techniques of confocal microscopy demonstrated that dendritic spines are indeed motile and dynamic structures that undergo a constant turnover, even after birth.[50][51][44]

References

edit
  1. ^ a b c d e f g Alvarez VA, Sabatini BL (2007). "Anatomical and physiological plasticity of dendritic spines". Annual Review of Neuroscience. 30: 79–97. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094222. PMID 17280523.
  2. ^ Tackenberg C, Ghori A, Brandt R (June 2009). "Thin, stubby or mushroom: spine pathology in Alzheimer's disease". Current Alzheimer Research. 6 (3): 261–8. doi:10.2174/156720509788486554. PMID 19519307.
  3. ^ Ofer N, Berger DR, Kasthuri N, Lichtman JW, Yuste R (July 2021). "Ultrastructural analysis of dendritic spine necks reveals a continuum of spine morphologies". Developmental Neurobiology. 81 (5): 746–757. doi:10.1002/dneu.22829. PMC 8852350. PMID 33977655. S2CID 234472935.
  4. ^ a b Evrard MR, Li M, Shen H, Smith SS (October 2021). "Preventing adolescent synaptic pruning in mouse prelimbic cortex via local knockdown of α4βδ GABAA receptors increases anxiety response in adulthood". Scientific Reports. 11 (1): 21059. Bibcode:2021NatSR..1121059E. doi:10.1038/s41598-021-99965-8. PMC 8548505. PMID 34702942.
  5. ^ Kasthuri N, Hayworth KJ, Berger DR, Schalek RL, Conchello JA, Knowles-Barley S, et al. (July 2015). "Saturated Reconstruction of a Volume of Neocortex". Cell. 162 (3): 648–661. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2015.06.054. PMID 26232230.
  6. ^ Kapitein LC, Schlager MA, Kuijpers M, Wulf PS, van Spronsen M, MacKintosh FC, Hoogenraad CC (February 2010). "Mixed microtubules steer dynein-driven cargo transport into dendrites". Current Biology. 20 (4): 290–9. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.12.052. PMID 20137950. S2CID 12180359.
  7. ^ Kim CH, Lisman JE (June 1999). "A role of actin filament in synaptic transmission and long-term potentiation". The Journal of Neuroscience. 19 (11): 4314–4324. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-11-04314.1999. PMC 6782630. PMID 10341235.
  8. ^ Krucker T, Siggins GR, Halpain S (June 2000). "Dynamic actin filaments are required for stable long-term potentiation (LTP) in area CA1 of the hippocampus". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 97 (12): 6856–6861. Bibcode:2000PNAS...97.6856K. doi:10.1073/pnas.100139797. PMC 18765. PMID 10823894.
  9. ^ Tashiro A, Yuste R (July 2004). "Regulation of dendritic spine motility and stability by Rac1 and Rho kinase: evidence for two forms of spine motility". Molecular and Cellular Neurosciences. 26 (3): 429–440. doi:10.1016/j.mcn.2004.04.001. PMID 15234347. S2CID 21100601.
  10. ^ a b c d e f Murakoshi H, Wang H, Yasuda R (April 2011). "Local, persistent activation of Rho GTPases during plasticity of single dendritic spines". Nature. 472 (7341): 100–104. Bibcode:2011Natur.472..100M. doi:10.1038/nature09823. PMC 3105377. PMID 21423166.
  11. ^ Kasai H, Matsuzaki M, Noguchi J, Yasumatsu N, Nakahara H (July 2003). "Structure-stability-function relationships of dendritic spines". Trends in Neurosciences. 26 (7): 360–368. doi:10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00162-0. PMID 12850432. S2CID 18436944.
  12. ^ Hotulainen P, Hoogenraad CC (May 2010). "Actin in dendritic spines: connecting dynamics to function". The Journal of Cell Biology. 189 (4): 619–629. doi:10.1083/jcb.201003008. PMC 2872912. PMID 20457765.
  13. ^ Kiss C, Li J, Szeles A, Gizatullin RZ, Kashuba VI, Lushnikova T, et al. (1 January 1997). "Assignment of the ARHA and GPX1 genes to human chromosome bands 3p21.3 by in situ hybridization and with somatic cell hybrids". Cytogenetics and Cell Genetics. 79 (3–4): 228–230. doi:10.1159/000134729. PMID 9605859.
  14. ^ Scott EK, Reuter JE, Luo L (April 2003). "Small GTPase Cdc42 is required for multiple aspects of dendritic morphogenesis". The Journal of Neuroscience. 23 (8): 3118–3123. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-08-03118.2003. PMC 6742332. PMID 12716918.
  15. ^ Calabrese B, Wilson MS, Halpain S (February 2006). "Development and regulation of dendritic spine synapses". Physiology. 21 (1): 38–47. doi:10.1152/physiol.00042.2005. PMID 16443821.
  16. ^ De Roo M, Klauser P, Mendez P, Poglia L, Muller D (January 2008). "Activity-dependent PSD formation and stabilization of newly formed spines in hippocampal slice cultures". Cerebral Cortex. 18 (1): 151–161. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm041. PMID 17517683.
  17. ^ Kaneko M, Xie Y, An JJ, Stryker MP, Xu B (April 2012). "Dendritic BDNF synthesis is required for late-phase spine maturation and recovery of cortical responses following sensory deprivation". The Journal of Neuroscience. 32 (14): 4790–4802. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4462-11.2012. PMC 3356781. PMID 22492034.
  18. ^ a b c Xu T, Yu X, Perlik AJ, Tobin WF, Zweig JA, Tennant K, et al. (December 2009). "Rapid formation and selective stabilization of synapses for enduring motor memories". Nature. 462 (7275): 915–919. Bibcode:2009Natur.462..915X. doi:10.1038/nature08389. PMC 2844762. PMID 19946267.
  19. ^ a b c Roberts TF, Tschida KA, Klein ME, Mooney R (February 2010). "Rapid spine stabilization and synaptic enhancement at the onset of behavioural learning". Nature. 463 (7283): 948–952. Bibcode:2010Natur.463..948R. doi:10.1038/nature08759. PMC 2918377. PMID 20164928.
  20. ^ Tschida KA, Mooney R (March 2012). "Deafening drives cell-type-specific changes to dendritic spines in a sensorimotor nucleus important to learned vocalizations". Neuron. 73 (5): 1028–1039. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.12.038. PMC 3299981. PMID 22405211.
  21. ^ De Roo M, Klauser P, Muller D (September 2008). "LTP promotes a selective long-term stabilization and clustering of dendritic spines". PLOS Biology. 6 (9): e219. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0060219. PMC 2531136. PMID 18788894.
  22. ^ a b c d e Zuo Y, Lin A, Chang P, Gan WB (April 2005). "Development of long-term dendritic spine stability in diverse regions of cerebral cortex". Neuron. 46 (2): 181–189. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.04.001. PMID 15848798. S2CID 16232150.
  23. ^ Holtmaat AJ, Trachtenberg JT, Wilbrecht L, Shepherd GM, Zhang X, Knott GW, Svoboda K (January 2005). "Transient and persistent dendritic spines in the neocortex in vivo". Neuron. 45 (2): 279–291. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.01.003. PMID 15664179. S2CID 13320649.
  24. ^ a b c Yang G, Pan F, Gan WB (December 2009). "Stably maintained dendritic spines are associated with lifelong memories". Nature. 462 (7275): 920–924. Bibcode:2009Natur.462..920Y. doi:10.1038/nature08577. PMC 4724802. PMID 19946265.
  25. ^ Holtmaat A, Wilbrecht L, Knott GW, Welker E, Svoboda K (June 2006). "Experience-dependent and cell-type-specific spine growth in the neocortex". Nature. 441 (7096): 979–983. Bibcode:2006Natur.441..979H. doi:10.1038/nature04783. PMID 16791195. S2CID 4428322.
  26. ^ Brown CE, Li P, Boyd JD, Delaney KR, Murphy TH (April 2007). "Extensive turnover of dendritic spines and vascular remodeling in cortical tissues recovering from stroke". The Journal of Neuroscience. 27 (15): 4101–4109. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4295-06.2007. PMC 6672555. PMID 17428988.
  27. ^ Brown CE, Murphy TH (April 2008). "Livin' on the edge: imaging dendritic spine turnover in the peri-infarct zone during ischemic stroke and recovery". The Neuroscientist. 14 (2): 139–146. doi:10.1177/1073858407309854. PMID 18039977. S2CID 46267737.
  28. ^ a b Bhatt DH, Zhang S, Gan WB (2009). "Dendritic spine dynamics". Annual Review of Physiology. 71: 261–282. doi:10.1146/annurev.physiol.010908.163140. PMID 19575680.
  29. ^ a b c Benson, Curtis A.; King, Jared F.; Reimer, Marike L.; Kauer, Sierra D.; Waxman, Stephen G.; Tan, Andrew M. (2022-12-03). "Dendritic Spines and Pain Memory". The Neuroscientist: 107385842211382. doi:10.1177/10738584221138251. ISSN 1073-8584. PMID 36461773.
  30. ^ von Bohlen und Halbach O, Zacher C, Gass P, Unsicker K (March 2006). "Age-related alterations in hippocampal spines and deficiencies in spatial memory in mice". Journal of Neuroscience Research. 83 (4): 525–531. doi:10.1002/jnr.20759. PMID 16447268. S2CID 30838296.
  31. ^ Benson, Curtis A.; Fenrich, Keith K.; Olson, Kai-Lan; Patwa, Siraj; Bangalore, Lakshmi; Waxman, Stephen G.; Tan, Andrew M. (2020-05-27). "Dendritic Spine Dynamics after Peripheral Nerve Injury: An Intravital Structural Study". The Journal of Neuroscience. 40 (22): 4297–4308. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2858-19.2020. ISSN 0270-6474. PMC 7252482. PMID 32371602.
  32. ^ Cao, Xiaoyu C; Pappalardo, Laura W; Waxman, Stephen G; Tan, Andrew M (January 2017). "Dendritic spine dysgenesis in superficial dorsal horn sensory neurons after spinal cord injury". Molecular Pain. 13: 174480691668801. doi:10.1177/1744806916688016. ISSN 1744-8069. PMC 5302173. PMID 28326929.
  33. ^ Zhao, Peng; Hill, Myriam; Liu, Shujun; Chen, Lubin; Bangalore, Lakshmi; Waxman, Stephen G.; Tan, Andrew M. (2016-06-01). "Dendritic spine remodeling following early and late Rac1 inhibition after spinal cord injury: evidence for a pain biomarker". Journal of Neurophysiology. 115 (6): 2893–2910. doi:10.1152/jn.01057.2015. ISSN 0022-3077. PMC 4922610. PMID 26936986.
  34. ^ Tan, Andrew M.; Choi, Jin-Sung; Waxman, Stephen G.; Hains, Bryan C. (October 2009). "Dendritic Spine Remodeling After Spinal Cord Injury Alters Neuronal Signal Processing". Journal of Neurophysiology. 102 (4): 2396–2409. doi:10.1152/jn.00095.2009. ISSN 0022-3077. PMID 19692517.
  35. ^ Harris KM, Fiala JC, Ostroff L (April 2003). "Structural changes at dendritic spine synapses during long-term potentiation". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 358 (1432): 745–748. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1254. PMC 1693146. PMID 12740121.
  36. ^ Araya R, Nikolenko V, Eisenthal KB, Yuste R (July 2007). "Sodium channels amplify spine potentials". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 104 (30): 12347–12352. Bibcode:2007PNAS..10412347A. doi:10.1073/pnas.0705282104. PMC 1924793. PMID 17640908.
  37. ^ Ngo-Anh TJ, Bloodgood BL, Lin M, Sabatini BL, Maylie J, Adelman JP (May 2005). "SK channels and NMDA receptors form a Ca2+-mediated feedback loop in dendritic spines". Nature Neuroscience. 8 (5): 642–649. doi:10.1038/nn1449. PMID 15852011. S2CID 385712.
  38. ^ Yuste R, Denk W (June 1995). "Dendritic spines as basic functional units of neuronal integration". Nature. 375 (6533): 682–684. Bibcode:1995Natur.375..682Y. doi:10.1038/375682a0. PMID 7791901. S2CID 4271356.
  39. ^ a b Bywalez WG, Patirniche D, Rupprecht V, Stemmler M, Herz AV, Pálfi D, et al. (February 2015). "Local postsynaptic voltage-gated sodium channel activation in dendritic spines of olfactory bulb granule cells". Neuron. 85 (3): 590–601. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.051. PMID 25619656.
  40. ^ Baer SM, Rinzel J (April 1991). "Propagation of dendritic spikes mediated by excitable spines: a continuum theory". Journal of Neurophysiology. 65 (4): 874–890. doi:10.1152/jn.1991.65.4.874. PMID 2051208.
  41. ^ Bressloff PC, Coombes S (2000). "Solitary Waves in a Model of Dendritic Cable with Active Spines". SIAM Journal on Applied Mathematics. 61 (2): 432–453. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.104.1307. doi:10.1137/s0036139999356600. JSTOR 3061734. S2CID 3058796.
  42. ^ Nevian T, Sakmann B (October 2006). "Spine Ca2+ signaling in spike-timing-dependent plasticity". The Journal of Neuroscience. 26 (43): 11001–11013. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1749-06.2006. PMC 6674669. PMID 17065442.
  43. ^ Grunditz A, Holbro N, Tian L, Zuo Y, Oertner TG (December 2008). "Spine neck plasticity controls postsynaptic calcium signals through electrical compartmentalization". The Journal of Neuroscience. 28 (50): 13457–13466. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2702-08.2008. PMC 6671740. PMID 19074019.
  44. ^ a b Yoshihara Y, De Roo M, Muller D (April 2009). "Dendritic spine formation and stabilization". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 19 (2): 146–53. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2009.05.013. PMID 19523814. S2CID 5054448.
  45. ^ Saarikangas J, Kourdougli N, Senju Y, Chazal G, Segerstråle M, Minkeviciene R, et al. (June 2015). "MIM-Induced Membrane Bending Promotes Dendritic Spine Initiation". Developmental Cell. 33 (6): 644–659. doi:10.1016/j.devcel.2015.04.014. PMID 26051541.
  46. ^ Pelucchi S, Stringhi R, Marcello E (January 2020). "Dendritic Spines in Alzheimer's Disease: How the Actin Cytoskeleton Contributes to Synaptic Failure". International Journal of Molecular Sciences. 21 (3): 908. doi:10.3390/ijms21030908. PMC 7036943. PMID 32019166.
  47. ^ Penzes P, Cahill ME, Jones KA, VanLeeuwen JE, Woolfrey KM (March 2011). "Dendritic spine pathology in neuropsychiatric disorders". Nature Neuroscience. 14 (3): 285–293. doi:10.1038/nn.2741. PMC 3530413. PMID 21346746.
  48. ^ Ramón y Cajal S (1888). "Estructura de los centros nerviosos de las aves". Rev. Trim. Histol. Norm. Pat. 1: 1–10.
  49. ^ Gray EG (June 1959). "Electron microscopy of synaptic contacts on dendrite spines of the cerebral cortex". Nature. 183 (4675): 1592–1593. Bibcode:1959Natur.183.1592G. doi:10.1038/1831592a0. PMID 13666826. S2CID 4258584.
  50. ^ Dailey ME, Smith SJ (May 1996). "The dynamics of dendritic structure in developing hippocampal slices". The Journal of Neuroscience. 16 (9): 2983–2994. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-09-02983.1996. PMC 6579052. PMID 8622128.
  51. ^ Bonhoeffer T, Yuste R (September 2002). "Spine motility. Phenomenology, mechanisms, and function". Neuron. 35 (6): 1019–1027. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00906-6. PMID 12354393. S2CID 10183317.

Further reading

edit
edit