Nothing Special   »   [go: up one dir, main page]

Jump to content

Parallel processing (psychology): Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
m Fixed a PMC parameter in a citation. Please see Category:CS1 maint: PMC format.
Citation bot (talk | contribs)
Alter: title. Add: isbn, year, date, pmc, pmid, jstor, doi, s2cid, authors 1-1. Removed parameters. Some additions/deletions were parameter name changes. | Use this bot. Report bugs. | Suggested by Abductive | Category:Articles with specifically marked weasel-worded phrases from May 2022 | #UCB_Category 33/399
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Short description|Brain processing function}}
{{Short description|Brain processing function}}
In psychology, '''parallel processing''' is the ability of the [[brain]] to simultaneously process incoming stimuli of differing quality.<ref>{{cite journal|last=LaBerge|first=David|last2=Samuels|first2=S.Jay|year=1974|title=Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading|journal=Cognitive Psychology|publisher=Elsevier BV|volume=6|issue=2|pages=293–323|doi=10.1016/0010-0285(74)90015-2|issn=0010-0285}}</ref> Parallel processing is associated with the [[visual system]] in that the brain divides what it sees into four components: [[color]], [[Motion (physics)|motion]], [[shape]], and [[Distance|depth]]. These are individually analyzed and then compared to stored [[Memory|memories]], which helps the brain identify what you are viewing.<ref>{{cite book|last=Hinton|first=Geoffrey|title=Parallel models of associative memory|publisher=Psychology Press|year=2014|isbn=978-1-315-80799-7|publication-place=New York}}</ref> The brain then combines all of these into the field of view that then seen and comprehended.<ref name="Wässle20042">{{cite journal|last1=Wässle|first1=Heinz|year=2004|title=Parallel processing in the mammalian retina|journal=Nature Reviews Neuroscience|volume=5|issue=10|pages=747–757|doi=10.1038/nrn1497|issn=1471-003X|pmid=15378035}}</ref> Parallel processing has been linked, by some experimental psychologists, to the [[stroop effect]]. This is a continual and seamless operation. for example: if one is standing between two different groups of people who are simultaneously carrying on two different conversations, one may be able to pick up only some information of both conversation at the same time.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Cohen|first=J. D.|last2=Dunbar|first2=K.|last3=McClelland|first3=J. L.|date=1988-06-16|title=On the Control of Automatic Processes: A Parallel Distributed Processing Model of the Stroop Effect|url=http://dx.doi.org/10.21236/ada218914|location=Fort Belvoir, VA}}</ref>
In psychology, '''parallel processing''' is the ability of the [[brain]] to simultaneously process incoming stimuli of differing quality.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=LaBerge|first1=David|last2=Samuels|first2=S.Jay|year=1974|title=Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading|journal=Cognitive Psychology|publisher=Elsevier BV|volume=6|issue=2|pages=293–323|doi=10.1016/0010-0285(74)90015-2|issn=0010-0285}}</ref> Parallel processing is associated with the [[visual system]] in that the brain divides what it sees into four components: [[color]], [[Motion (physics)|motion]], [[shape]], and [[Distance|depth]]. These are individually analyzed and then compared to stored [[Memory|memories]], which helps the brain identify what you are viewing.<ref>{{cite book|last=Hinton|first=Geoffrey|title=Parallel models of associative memory|publisher=Psychology Press|year=2014|isbn=978-1-315-80799-7|publication-place=New York}}</ref> The brain then combines all of these into the field of view that then seen and comprehended.<ref name="Wässle20042">{{cite journal|last1=Wässle|first1=Heinz|year=2004|title=Parallel processing in the mammalian retina|journal=Nature Reviews Neuroscience|volume=5|issue=10|pages=747–757|doi=10.1038/nrn1497|issn=1471-003X|pmid=15378035|s2cid=10518721 }}</ref> Parallel processing has been linked, by some experimental psychologists, to the [[stroop effect]]. This is a continual and seamless operation. for example: if one is standing between two different groups of people who are simultaneously carrying on two different conversations, one may be able to pick up only some information of both conversation at the same time.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Cohen|first1=J. D.|last2=Dunbar|first2=K.|last3=McClelland|first3=J. L.|date=1988-06-16|title=On the Control of Automatic Processes: A Parallel Distributed Processing Model of the Stroop Effect|url=http://dx.doi.org/10.21236/ada218914|location=Fort Belvoir, VA|doi=10.21236/ada218914 }}</ref>


== Background ==
== Background ==
Parallel Distributed Processing Models are '<nowiki/>''neurally inspired''', emulating the organisational structure of nervous systems of living organisms.<ref name=":02">{{Cite book|last=Rumelhart|first=David E.|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/12837549|title=Parallel distributed processing : explorations in the microstructure of cognition|date=1986|publisher=MIT Press|others=James L. McClelland, San Diego. PDP Research Group University of California|isbn=0-262-18120-7|location=Cambridge, Mass.|oclc=12837549}}</ref> A general mathematical framework is provided for them.
Parallel Distributed Processing Models are '<nowiki/>''neurally inspired''', emulating the organisational structure of nervous systems of living organisms.<ref name=":02">{{Cite book|last=Rumelhart|first=David E.|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/12837549|title=Parallel distributed processing : explorations in the microstructure of cognition|date=1986|publisher=MIT Press|others=James L. McClelland, San Diego. PDP Research Group University of California|isbn=0-262-18120-7|location=Cambridge, Mass.|oclc=12837549}}</ref> A general mathematical framework is provided for them.


Parallel processing models assume that information is represented in the brain using patterns of activation. Information processing encompasses the interactions of [[neuron]] like '''units''<nowiki/>' linked by [[synapse]] like “connections”. These can be either [[Excitatory synapse|excitatory]] or [[Inhibitory postsynaptic potential|inhibitory]]. Every individual unit's activation level is updated using a [[Function (mathematics)|function]] of connection strengths and activation level of other units. A set of response units is activated by the propagation of activation patterns. The connection weights are eventually adjusted using learning.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Holyoak|first=Keith J.|date=1987|editor-last=Rumelhart|editor-first=David E.|editor2-last=McClelland|editor2-first=James L.|editor3-last=Group|editor3-first=PDP Research|title=A Connectionist View of Cognition|url=https://www.jstor.org/stable/1699673|journal=Science|volume=236|issue=4804|pages=992–996|issn=0036-8075}}</ref>
Parallel processing models assume that information is represented in the brain using patterns of activation. Information processing encompasses the interactions of [[neuron]] like '''units''<nowiki/>' linked by [[synapse]] like “connections”. These can be either [[Excitatory synapse|excitatory]] or [[Inhibitory postsynaptic potential|inhibitory]]. Every individual unit's activation level is updated using a [[Function (mathematics)|function]] of connection strengths and activation level of other units. A set of response units is activated by the propagation of activation patterns. The connection weights are eventually adjusted using learning.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Holyoak|first=Keith J.|date=1987|editor-last=Rumelhart|editor-first=David E.|editor2-last=McClelland|editor2-first=James L.|editor3-last=Group|editor3-first=PDP Research|title=A Connectionist View of Cognition|url=https://www.jstor.org/stable/1699673|journal=Science|volume=236|issue=4804|pages=992–996|doi=10.1126/science.236.4804.992 |jstor=1699673 |issn=0036-8075}}</ref>


=== Serial vs parallel processing ===
=== Serial vs parallel processing ===
In contrast to parallel processing, serial processing involves sequential processing of information, without any overlap of processing times.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Townsend|first=James T.|date=January 1990|title=Serial vs. Parallel Processing: Sometimes They Look like Tweedledum and Tweedledee but they can (and Should) be Distinguished|url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00067.x|journal=Psychological Science|volume=1|issue=1|pages=46–54|doi=10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00067.x|issn=0956-7976}}</ref>
In contrast to parallel processing, serial processing involves sequential processing of information, without any overlap of processing times.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Townsend|first=James T.|date=January 1990|title=Serial vs. Parallel Processing: Sometimes They Look like Tweedledum and Tweedledee but they can (and Should) be Distinguished|url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00067.x|journal=Psychological Science|volume=1|issue=1|pages=46–54|doi=10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00067.x|s2cid=51773777 |issn=0956-7976}}</ref>


==== Visual search ====
==== Visual search ====
In case of serial processing, the elements are searched one after the other in a serial order to find the target. When the target is found, the search terminates. Alternatively, it continues to the end to ensure that the target is not present. This results in reduced accuracy and increased time for displays with more objects.
In case of serial processing, the elements are searched one after the other in a serial order to find the target. When the target is found, the search terminates. Alternatively, it continues to the end to ensure that the target is not present. This results in reduced accuracy and increased time for displays with more objects.


On the other hand, in the case of parallel processing, all objects are processed simultaneously but the completion times may vary. This may or may not reduce the accuracy, but the time courses are similar irrespective of the size of the display.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Dosher|first=Barbara Anne|last2=Han|first2=Songmei|last3=Lu|first3=Zhong-Lin|date=2010|title=Information-limited parallel processing in difficult heterogeneous covert visual search.|url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0020366|journal=Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance|volume=36|issue=5|pages=1128–1144|doi=10.1037/a0020366|issn=1939-1277}}</ref>
On the other hand, in the case of parallel processing, all objects are processed simultaneously but the completion times may vary. This may or may not reduce the accuracy, but the time courses are similar irrespective of the size of the display.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Dosher|first1=Barbara Anne|last2=Han|first2=Songmei|last3=Lu|first3=Zhong-Lin|date=2010|title=Information-limited parallel processing in difficult heterogeneous covert visual search.|url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0020366|journal=Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance|volume=36|issue=5|pages=1128–1144|doi=10.1037/a0020366|pmid=20873936 |pmc=3929106 |issn=1939-1277}}</ref>


However, there are concerns about the efficiency of parallel processing models in case of complex tasks which are discussed ahead in this article.
However, there are concerns about the efficiency of parallel processing models in case of complex tasks which are discussed ahead in this article.
Line 46: Line 46:


=== Environmental representation ===
=== Environmental representation ===
In PDP models, the environment is represented as a ''time-varying [[Stochastic simulation|stochastic function]]'' over the space of input patterns.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Snodgrass|first=Joan Gay|last2=Townsend|first2=James T.|last3=Ashby|first3=F. Gregory|date=1985|title=Stochastic Modeling of Elementary Psychological Processes|url=http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1422636|journal=The American Journal of Psychology|volume=98|issue=3|pages=480|doi=10.2307/1422636|issn=0002-9556}}</ref> This means that at any given point, there is a possibility that any of the possible set of input patterns is impinging on the input units.  <ref name=":02" />
In PDP models, the environment is represented as a ''time-varying [[Stochastic simulation|stochastic function]]'' over the space of input patterns.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Snodgrass|first1=Joan Gay|last2=Townsend|first2=James T.|last3=Ashby|first3=F. Gregory|date=1985|title=Stochastic Modeling of Elementary Psychological Processes|url=http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1422636|journal=The American Journal of Psychology|volume=98|issue=3|pages=480|doi=10.2307/1422636|jstor=1422636 |issn=0002-9556}}</ref> This means that at any given point, there is a possibility that any of the possible set of input patterns is impinging on the input units.  <ref name=":02" />


== Depth ==
== Depth ==
When we{{who?|date=May 2022}} sense [[Depth perception|depth]], we use both eyes to see three dimensional objects. This sense is present in birth in humans and some animals, such as cats, dogs, owls, and monkeys.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Nityananda |first=Vivek |last2=Read |first2=Jenny C. A. |date=2017-07-15 |title=Stereopsis in animals: evolution, function and mechanisms |url=https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.143883 |journal=Journal of Experimental Biology |volume=220 |issue=14 |pages=2502–2512 |doi=10.1242/jeb.143883 |issn=0022-0949 |pmc=5536890 |pmid=28724702}}</ref> Animals with wider-set eyes have a harder time establishing depth, such as horses and cows. A special depth test was used on infants, named [[Visual cliff|The Visual Cliff]].<ref name=":1">{{Cite book |last=author. |first=Myers, David G. |url=http://worldcat.org/oclc/1273977481 |title=Exploring Psychology |isbn=978-1-319-42980-5 |oclc=1273977481}}</ref> This test consisted of a table, half coated in a checkerboard pattern, and the other half a clear plexiglass sheet, revealing a second checkerboard platform about a foot below. Although the plexiglass was safe to climb on, the infants refused to cross over due to the perception of a visual cliff. This test proved that most infants already have a good sense of depth. This phenomenon is similar to how adults perceive heights.
When we{{who?|date=May 2022}} sense [[Depth perception|depth]], we use both eyes to see three dimensional objects. This sense is present in birth in humans and some animals, such as cats, dogs, owls, and monkeys.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Nityananda |first1=Vivek |last2=Read |first2=Jenny C. A. |date=2017-07-15 |title=Stereopsis in animals: evolution, function and mechanisms |url=https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.143883 |journal=Journal of Experimental Biology |volume=220 |issue=14 |pages=2502–2512 |doi=10.1242/jeb.143883 |issn=0022-0949 |pmc=5536890 |pmid=28724702}}</ref> Animals with wider-set eyes have a harder time establishing depth, such as horses and cows. A special depth test was used on infants, named [[Visual cliff|The Visual Cliff]].<ref name=":1">{{Cite book |last=author. |first=Myers, David G. |url=http://worldcat.org/oclc/1273977481 |title=Exploring Psychology |date=16 September 2021 |isbn=978-1-319-42980-5 |oclc=1273977481}}</ref> This test consisted of a table, half coated in a checkerboard pattern, and the other half a clear plexiglass sheet, revealing a second checkerboard platform about a foot below. Although the plexiglass was safe to climb on, the infants refused to cross over due to the perception of a visual cliff. This test proved that most infants already have a good sense of depth. This phenomenon is similar to how adults perceive heights.


Certain cues help us establish our depth perception. [[Depth perception|Binocular cues]] are made by our two eyes, which are subconsciously compared to calculate distance.<ref>{{Cite book |last=W. |first=Eysenck, Michael |url=http://worldcat.org/oclc/1150999656 |title=Cognitive psychology : a student's handbook |isbn=978-1-138-48221-0 |oclc=1150999656}}</ref> This idea of two separate images is used by 3-D and VR filmmakers to give two dimensional footage the element of depth. [[Monocular cues]] can be used by a single eye with hints from the environment. These hints include relative height, relative size, linear perspective, lights and shadows, and relative motion.<ref name=":1" /> Each hint helps us establish small facts about a scene that work together to form a perception of depth. Binocular cues and monocular cues are used constantly and subconsciously to sense depth.
Certain cues help us establish our depth perception. [[Depth perception|Binocular cues]] are made by our two eyes, which are subconsciously compared to calculate distance.<ref>{{Cite book |last=W. |first=Eysenck, Michael |url=http://worldcat.org/oclc/1150999656 |title=Cognitive psychology : a student's handbook |year=2020 |isbn=978-1-138-48221-0 |oclc=1150999656}}</ref> This idea of two separate images is used by 3-D and VR filmmakers to give two dimensional footage the element of depth. [[Monocular cues]] can be used by a single eye with hints from the environment. These hints include relative height, relative size, linear perspective, lights and shadows, and relative motion.<ref name=":1" /> Each hint helps us establish small facts about a scene that work together to form a perception of depth. Binocular cues and monocular cues are used constantly and subconsciously to sense depth.


== Limitations ==
== Limitations ==
Limitations of parallel processing have been brought up in several analytical studies. The main limitations highlighted include capacity limits of the brain, [[attentional blink]] rate interferences, limited processing capabilities, and information limitations in visual searches.
Limitations of parallel processing have been brought up in several analytical studies. The main limitations highlighted include capacity limits of the brain, [[attentional blink]] rate interferences, limited processing capabilities, and information limitations in visual searches.


There are processing limits to the brain in the execution of complex tasks like [[Visual object recognition (animal test)|object recognition]]. All parts of the brain cannot process at full capacity parallelly. Attention controls the allocation of resources to the tasks. To work efficiently, attention must be guided from object to object.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Wolfe|first=Jeremy M.|date=August 1992|title=The Parallel Guidance of Visual Attention|url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep10769733|journal=Current Directions in Psychological Science|volume=1|issue=4|pages=124–128|doi=10.1111/1467-8721.ep10769733|issn=0963-7214}}</ref>
There are processing limits to the brain in the execution of complex tasks like [[Visual object recognition (animal test)|object recognition]]. All parts of the brain cannot process at full capacity parallelly. Attention controls the allocation of resources to the tasks. To work efficiently, attention must be guided from object to object.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Wolfe|first=Jeremy M.|date=August 1992|title=The Parallel Guidance of Visual Attention|url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep10769733|journal=Current Directions in Psychological Science|volume=1|issue=4|pages=124–128|doi=10.1111/1467-8721.ep10769733|s2cid=15808488 |issn=0963-7214}}</ref>


These limits to attentional resources sometimes lead to ''serial bottlenecks'' in parallel processing, meaning that parallel processing is obstructed by serial processing in between. However, there is evidence for coexistence of serial and parallel processes.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Sigman|first=Mariano|last2=Dehaene|first2=Stanislas|date=2008-07-23|title=Brain Mechanisms of Serial and Parallel Processing during Dual-Task Performance|url=https://www.jneurosci.org/content/28/30/7585|journal=Journal of Neuroscience|language=en|volume=28|issue=30|pages=7585–7598|doi=10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0948-08.2008|issn=0270-6474|pmid=18650336}}</ref>
These limits to attentional resources sometimes lead to ''serial bottlenecks'' in parallel processing, meaning that parallel processing is obstructed by serial processing in between. However, there is evidence for coexistence of serial and parallel processes.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Sigman|first1=Mariano|last2=Dehaene|first2=Stanislas|date=2008-07-23|title=Brain Mechanisms of Serial and Parallel Processing during Dual-Task Performance|url=https://www.jneurosci.org/content/28/30/7585|journal=Journal of Neuroscience|language=en|volume=28|issue=30|pages=7585–7598|doi=10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0948-08.2008|issn=0270-6474|pmid=18650336|pmc=6670853 }}</ref>


=== Feature Integration Theory ===
=== Feature Integration Theory ===
Line 64: Line 64:


# ''Detection of features-'' This stage occurs instantaneously and uses parallel processing. In this step, we pick up all the basic features of a display simultaneously, even if we are paying attention to a specific object.
# ''Detection of features-'' This stage occurs instantaneously and uses parallel processing. In this step, we pick up all the basic features of a display simultaneously, even if we are paying attention to a specific object.
# ''Integration of features-'' This step is more time-consuming and uses serial processing. It leads to the [[perception]] of whole objects and patterns.<ref>{{Citation|title=Features and Objects in Visual Processing|date=2002|url=http://dx.doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/3080.003.0025|work=Foundations of Cognitive Psychology|publisher=The MIT Press|access-date=2022-02-16}}</ref>
# ''Integration of features-'' This step is more time-consuming and uses serial processing. It leads to the [[perception]] of whole objects and patterns.<ref>{{Citation|title=Features and Objects in Visual Processing|date=2002|url=http://dx.doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/3080.003.0025|work=Foundations of Cognitive Psychology|publisher=The MIT Press|doi=10.7551/mitpress/3080.003.0025 |isbn=9780262278263 |access-date=2022-02-16}}</ref>


==See also==
==See also==

Revision as of 21:55, 9 June 2022

In psychology, parallel processing is the ability of the brain to simultaneously process incoming stimuli of differing quality.[1] Parallel processing is associated with the visual system in that the brain divides what it sees into four components: color, motion, shape, and depth. These are individually analyzed and then compared to stored memories, which helps the brain identify what you are viewing.[2] The brain then combines all of these into the field of view that then seen and comprehended.[3] Parallel processing has been linked, by some experimental psychologists, to the stroop effect. This is a continual and seamless operation. for example: if one is standing between two different groups of people who are simultaneously carrying on two different conversations, one may be able to pick up only some information of both conversation at the same time.[4]

Background

Parallel Distributed Processing Models are 'neurally inspired', emulating the organisational structure of nervous systems of living organisms.[5] A general mathematical framework is provided for them.

Parallel processing models assume that information is represented in the brain using patterns of activation. Information processing encompasses the interactions of neuron like 'units' linked by synapse like “connections”. These can be either excitatory or inhibitory. Every individual unit's activation level is updated using a function of connection strengths and activation level of other units. A set of response units is activated by the propagation of activation patterns. The connection weights are eventually adjusted using learning.[6]

Serial vs parallel processing

In contrast to parallel processing, serial processing involves sequential processing of information, without any overlap of processing times.[7]

In case of serial processing, the elements are searched one after the other in a serial order to find the target. When the target is found, the search terminates. Alternatively, it continues to the end to ensure that the target is not present. This results in reduced accuracy and increased time for displays with more objects.

On the other hand, in the case of parallel processing, all objects are processed simultaneously but the completion times may vary. This may or may not reduce the accuracy, but the time courses are similar irrespective of the size of the display.[8]

However, there are concerns about the efficiency of parallel processing models in case of complex tasks which are discussed ahead in this article.

Aspects of a parallel distributed processing model

There are eight major aspects of a parallel distributed processing model:

Processing units

These units may include abstract elements such as features, shapes and words, and are generally categorised into three types: input, output and hidden units.

  • Input units receive signals from either sensory stimuli or other parts of the processing system.
  • The output units send signals out of the system.
  • The hidden units function entirely inside the system.

Activation state

This is a representation of the state of the system. The pattern of activation is represented using a vector of N real numbers, over the set of processing units. It is this pattern that captures what the system is representing at any time.

Output functions

An output function maps the current state of activation to an output signal. The units interact with their neighbouring units by transmitting signals. The strengths of these signals are determined by their degree of activation. This in turn affects the degree to which they affect their neighbours.

Connectivity patterns

The pattern of connectivity determines how the system will react to an arbitrary input. The total pattern of connectivity is represented by specifying the weights for every connection. A positive weight represents an excitatory input and a negative weight represents an inhibitory input.

Propagation rule

A net input is produced for each type of input using rules that take the output vector and combine it with the connectivity matrices. In the case of a more complex pattern connectivity, the rules are more complex too.

Activation rule

A new state of activation is produced for every unit by joining the net inputs of impinging units combined and the current state of activation for that unit.

Learning rule

The patterns of connectivity are modified using experience. The modifications can be of three types: First, the development of new connections. Second, the loss of existing connection. Last, the modification of strengths of connections that already exist. The first two can be considered as special cases of the last one. When the strength of a connection is changed from zero to a positive or negative one, it is like forming a new connection. When the strength of a connection is changed to zero, it is like losing an existing connection.

Environmental representation

In PDP models, the environment is represented as a time-varying stochastic function over the space of input patterns.[9] This means that at any given point, there is a possibility that any of the possible set of input patterns is impinging on the input units.  [5]

Depth

When we[who?] sense depth, we use both eyes to see three dimensional objects. This sense is present in birth in humans and some animals, such as cats, dogs, owls, and monkeys.[10] Animals with wider-set eyes have a harder time establishing depth, such as horses and cows. A special depth test was used on infants, named The Visual Cliff.[11] This test consisted of a table, half coated in a checkerboard pattern, and the other half a clear plexiglass sheet, revealing a second checkerboard platform about a foot below. Although the plexiglass was safe to climb on, the infants refused to cross over due to the perception of a visual cliff. This test proved that most infants already have a good sense of depth. This phenomenon is similar to how adults perceive heights.

Certain cues help us establish our depth perception. Binocular cues are made by our two eyes, which are subconsciously compared to calculate distance.[12] This idea of two separate images is used by 3-D and VR filmmakers to give two dimensional footage the element of depth. Monocular cues can be used by a single eye with hints from the environment. These hints include relative height, relative size, linear perspective, lights and shadows, and relative motion.[11] Each hint helps us establish small facts about a scene that work together to form a perception of depth. Binocular cues and monocular cues are used constantly and subconsciously to sense depth.

Limitations

Limitations of parallel processing have been brought up in several analytical studies. The main limitations highlighted include capacity limits of the brain, attentional blink rate interferences, limited processing capabilities, and information limitations in visual searches.

There are processing limits to the brain in the execution of complex tasks like object recognition. All parts of the brain cannot process at full capacity parallelly. Attention controls the allocation of resources to the tasks. To work efficiently, attention must be guided from object to object.[13]

These limits to attentional resources sometimes lead to serial bottlenecks in parallel processing, meaning that parallel processing is obstructed by serial processing in between. However, there is evidence for coexistence of serial and parallel processes.[14]

Feature Integration Theory

The Feature Integration Theory by Anne Treisman is one of the theories that integrates serial and parallel processing while taking into account attentional resources. It consists of two stages-

  1. Detection of features- This stage occurs instantaneously and uses parallel processing. In this step, we pick up all the basic features of a display simultaneously, even if we are paying attention to a specific object.
  2. Integration of features- This step is more time-consuming and uses serial processing. It leads to the perception of whole objects and patterns.[15]

See also

References

  1. ^ LaBerge, David; Samuels, S.Jay (1974). "Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading". Cognitive Psychology. 6 (2). Elsevier BV: 293–323. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(74)90015-2. ISSN 0010-0285.
  2. ^ Hinton, Geoffrey (2014). Parallel models of associative memory. New York: Psychology Press. ISBN 978-1-315-80799-7.
  3. ^ Wässle, Heinz (2004). "Parallel processing in the mammalian retina". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 5 (10): 747–757. doi:10.1038/nrn1497. ISSN 1471-003X. PMID 15378035. S2CID 10518721.
  4. ^ Cohen, J. D.; Dunbar, K.; McClelland, J. L. (1988-06-16). "On the Control of Automatic Processes: A Parallel Distributed Processing Model of the Stroop Effect". Fort Belvoir, VA. doi:10.21236/ada218914. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  5. ^ a b Rumelhart, David E. (1986). Parallel distributed processing : explorations in the microstructure of cognition. James L. McClelland, San Diego. PDP Research Group University of California. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. ISBN 0-262-18120-7. OCLC 12837549.
  6. ^ Holyoak, Keith J. (1987). Rumelhart, David E.; McClelland, James L.; Group, PDP Research (eds.). "A Connectionist View of Cognition". Science. 236 (4804): 992–996. doi:10.1126/science.236.4804.992. ISSN 0036-8075. JSTOR 1699673. {{cite journal}}: |editor3-last= has generic name (help)
  7. ^ Townsend, James T. (January 1990). "Serial vs. Parallel Processing: Sometimes They Look like Tweedledum and Tweedledee but they can (and Should) be Distinguished". Psychological Science. 1 (1): 46–54. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00067.x. ISSN 0956-7976. S2CID 51773777.
  8. ^ Dosher, Barbara Anne; Han, Songmei; Lu, Zhong-Lin (2010). "Information-limited parallel processing in difficult heterogeneous covert visual search". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 36 (5): 1128–1144. doi:10.1037/a0020366. ISSN 1939-1277. PMC 3929106. PMID 20873936.
  9. ^ Snodgrass, Joan Gay; Townsend, James T.; Ashby, F. Gregory (1985). "Stochastic Modeling of Elementary Psychological Processes". The American Journal of Psychology. 98 (3): 480. doi:10.2307/1422636. ISSN 0002-9556. JSTOR 1422636.
  10. ^ Nityananda, Vivek; Read, Jenny C. A. (2017-07-15). "Stereopsis in animals: evolution, function and mechanisms". Journal of Experimental Biology. 220 (14): 2502–2512. doi:10.1242/jeb.143883. ISSN 0022-0949. PMC 5536890. PMID 28724702.
  11. ^ a b author., Myers, David G. (16 September 2021). Exploring Psychology. ISBN 978-1-319-42980-5. OCLC 1273977481. {{cite book}}: |last= has generic name (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  12. ^ W., Eysenck, Michael (2020). Cognitive psychology : a student's handbook. ISBN 978-1-138-48221-0. OCLC 1150999656.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  13. ^ Wolfe, Jeremy M. (August 1992). "The Parallel Guidance of Visual Attention". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 1 (4): 124–128. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.ep10769733. ISSN 0963-7214. S2CID 15808488.
  14. ^ Sigman, Mariano; Dehaene, Stanislas (2008-07-23). "Brain Mechanisms of Serial and Parallel Processing during Dual-Task Performance". Journal of Neuroscience. 28 (30): 7585–7598. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0948-08.2008. ISSN 0270-6474. PMC 6670853. PMID 18650336.
  15. ^ "Features and Objects in Visual Processing", Foundations of Cognitive Psychology, The MIT Press, 2002, doi:10.7551/mitpress/3080.003.0025, ISBN 9780262278263, retrieved 2022-02-16