- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Shank3 deletion in PV neurons is associated with abnormal behaviors and neuronal functions that are rescued by increasing GABAergic signaling
Molecular Autism volume 14, Article number: 28 (2023)
Abstract
Background
Phelan–McDermid syndrome (PMS) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by developmental delay, intellectual disability, and autistic-like behaviors and is primarily caused by haploinsufficiency of SHANK3 gene. Currently, there is no specific treatment for PMS, highlighting the need for a better understanding of SHANK3 functions and the underlying pathophysiological mechanisms in the brain. We hypothesize that SHANK3 haploinsufficiency may lead to alterations in the inhibitory system, which could be linked to the excitatory/inhibitory imbalance observed in models of autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Investigation of these neuropathological features may shed light on the pathogenesis of PMS and potential therapeutic interventions.
Methods
We recorded local field potentials and visual evoked responses in the visual cortex of Shank3∆11−/− mice. Then, to understand the impact of Shank3 in inhibitory neurons, we generated Pv-cre+/− Shank3Fl/Wt conditional mice, in which Shank3 was deleted in parvalbumin-positive neurons. We characterized the phenotype of this murine model and we compared this phenotype before and after ganaxolone administration.
Results
We found, in the primary visual cortex, an alteration of the gain control of Shank3 KO compared with Wt mice, indicating a deficit of inhibition on pyramidal neurons. This alteration was rescued after the potentiation of GABAA receptor activity by Midazolam. Behavioral analysis showed an impairment in grooming, memory, and motor coordination of Pv-cre+/− Shank3Fl/Wt compared with Pv-cre+/− Shank3Wt/Wt mice. These deficits were rescued with ganaxolone, a positive modulator of GABAA receptors. Furthermore, we demonstrated that treatment with ganaxolone also ameliorated evocative memory deficits and repetitive behavior of Shank3 KO mice.
Limitations
Despite the significant findings of our study, some limitations remain. Firstly, the neurobiological mechanisms underlying the link between Shank3 deletion in PV neurons and behavioral alterations need further investigation. Additionally, the impact of Shank3 on other classes of inhibitory neurons requires further exploration. Finally, the pharmacological activity of ganaxolone needs further characterization to improve our understanding of its potential therapeutic effects.
Conclusions
Our study provides evidence that Shank3 deletion leads to an alteration in inhibitory feedback on cortical pyramidal neurons, resulting in cortical hyperexcitability and ASD-like behavioral problems. Specifically, cell type-specific deletion of Shank3 in PV neurons was associated with these behavioral deficits. Our findings suggest that ganaxolone may be a potential pharmacological approach for treating PMS, as it was able to rescue the behavioral deficits in Shank3 KO mice. Overall, our study highlights the importance of investigating the role of inhibitory neurons and potential therapeutic interventions in neurodevelopmental disorders such as PMS.
Background
Haploinsufficiency of SHANK3 is widely recognized as the major cause of Phelan–McDermid syndrome (PMS), a complex neurodevelopmental disorder [1]. Major clinical features of the syndrome include global developmental delay, moderate-to-severe intellectual impairment, absent or delayed speech and neonatal hypotonia. Over 80% of patients with PMS exhibit autistic-like behavior including impaired communication and social interaction, poor eye contact, anxiety and self-stimulatory conduct, and decreased perception of pain [2, 3]. In addition, more than 25% of the PMS cases present epileptic symptoms, such as grand mal, focal and absence seizures [2, 4, 5]. Of note numerous studies have linked the SHANK3 gene with ASD [6,7,8], and it is estimated that mutations or deletions in the SHANK3 gene account for approximately 1% of all ASD cases [8].
Despite extensive research on the synaptic functions of Shank3, the lack of effective therapies for PMS and ASD underscores the critical need to identify the underlying pathogenic mechanisms of these disorders. Brain activity relies on the interplay between excitation (E) and inhibition (I) via synaptic communication between glutamatergic and GABAergic neuron. Although GABAergic neurons comprise only about 20% of cortical neurons, they have an important role in modulating cortical functions and plasticity; indeed, GABAergic neurons are essential for synchronizing network activity and maintaining the excitatory and inhibitory dynamics [9,10,11,12]. In addition to their role in modulating cortical functions and plasticity, GABAergic neurons are also critical for the maturation of neural circuits [13] and are implicated in developmental processes, such as regulation of neuronal proliferation and migration [14, 15]. Therefore, loss or dysfunction of these neurons has been implicated in several neurological disorders including epilepsy, schizophrenia, and autism [12, 16,17,18]. Interestingly, preferential deletion of Shank3 in GABAergic cortical neurons has been associated with pyramidal neuron hyperexcitability and sensory hypersensitivity [19]. Among GABAergic neurons, parvalbumin-positive (PV) neurons have been implicated in the etiology of neuropsychiatric disorders based on a growing body of evidence [16, 20].
The PV neurons, which constitute 50% of GABAergic cells, include fast-spiking basket cells and chandelier cells. Fast-spiking PV neurons are capable of generating action potentials at high-frequency mediating fast and precise inhibition of target neurons. These cells are involved in important functions, including the regulation of plasticity, the control of gain control in the cortex and the establishment and maintenance of cortical rhythms [9, 19, 21, 22]. The activation of PV neurons is particularly critical for the generations of gamma frequency oscillations (30–80 Hz), which are a key feature of pyramidal cell synchronization and are implicated in higher brain functions, such as sensory perception, motor behavior and memory formation [13, 22,23,24]. Shank3 is expressed in PV GABAergic neurons, and its absence results in reduced PV expression in the striatum of Shank3 KO mice [25]. Additionally, null mutant Shank3 exhibits delayed circuit maturation during development particularly in perisomatic PV neuronal circuit function [26].
In this study, we recorded local field potentials and visual evoked responses in the visual cortex of Shank3∆11−/− (Shank3 KO) mice [27] and observed that responses to visual stimuli had a limited dynamic range likely resulting from impaired gain control in Shank3 KO compared with Wt mice. Since gain adaptation is due to the feedback from PV interneurons [28], we hypothesized that this was due to reduced inhibition. Our hypothesis was supported by the fact that gain adaptation was restored following the potentiation of GABAA receptor activity with Midazolam, suggesting a role for Shank3 in the modulation of GABAergic neurons.
In order to investigate whether deletion of Shank3 in PV-expressing neurons leads to behavioral deficits in mice, we generated a new mouse model by crossing Shank3 floxed mice [29] with Pv-Cre mice. Our findings indicate that the heterozygous deletion of Shank3 specifically in PV neurons is sufficient to induce specific behavioral alterations, including repetitive behaviors, motor problems and memory impairments. Additionally, LFP recordings of Pv-Cre +/− Shank3Wt/Fl mice revealed that selective deletion of Shank3 in PV neurons caused a severe phenotype that led to cortical hyperexcitability. Finally, we demonstrated that the potentiation of GABAA receptor activity may represent a possible therapeutic strategy to ameliorate some of the behavioral symptoms caused by Shank3 deletion.
Methods
Mice
Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl mice were generated by breeding Shank3 floxed mice (kindly provided by Tobias Böeckers, from the Institute for Anatomy and Cell Biology of Ulm University, Germany) with Pv-Cre+/− mice (PValbtm1(cre)Arbr, The Jackson laboratory). To visualize PV+ interneurons, we generated Pv-Cre+/− TdTomatoFl/− Shank3Fl/Wt mice using TdTomato+/+ knock-in mice (Gt(ROSA)26Sortm14(CAG−tdTomato)Hze, The Jackson laboratory). The Shank3Δ11−/− mice were generated as previously described by Schemeisser et al., 2012 [27] and re-derived in a C57BL/6 background (Charles River Laboratories, Calco, Italy). These mice have already been characterized by [30]. All experiments conducted for this project involved mice that were housed in an animal facility maintained at a constant temperature (22 +/− 1 °C) and humidity (50%), with a 12-h light/dark cycle, and provided with ad libitum access to food and water. The experiments were performed in accordance with the guidelines approved by the European Communities Council and the Italian Ministry of Health (Rome, Italy) for the ethical use of laboratory animals in research. All possible efforts were taken to minimize the number of mice used and reduce their suffering. Animals from both sexes were included in all experiments, and subsequently, the data were combined, as no statistical differences were observed between the two groups.
Mice genotyping
All primers were provided by Thermo Fisher; the REDExtract-N-Amp PCR Reaction Mix™
(Sigma-Aldrich) and the MyFi™ DNA Polymerase (Meridian Bioscience) were used for the polymerase reaction. PCR genotyping was performed using the following sets of oligonucleotide primers: parvalbumin for Wt allele forward 5′-CAGAGCAGGCATGGTGACTA-3′, for Wt allele reverse 5′-AGT ACCAAGCAGGCAGGAGA-3′, for mutant allele forward 5′-GCGGTCTGGCAGTAAAAACTATC-3′, for mutant allele reverse 5′-GTGAAACAGCATTGCTGTCACTT-3′; Shank3 floxed forward 5′-GTCTCTGTGGTTGGGGTGTC-3′, reverse 5′-CAGTGAAGAAGCCCCAGAAG-3′ for both Wt and mutant allele; TdTomato for Wt allele forward 5′- AAGGGAGCTGCAGTGGAGTA-3′, for Wt allele reverse 5′-CCGAAAATCTGTGGGAAGTC-3′, for mutant allele forward 5′- CTGTTCCTGTACGGCATGG -3′, for mutant allele reverse 5′- GGCATTAAAGCAGCGTATCC -3′; Shank3Δ11−/− for Wt allele forward 5′-CAAGTTCATCGCTGTGAAGG-3′, for mutant allele forward 5′-CCTCTAGGCCTGCTAGCTGTT-3′, reverse 5′-AAGAAGCCCCAGAAGTGACA-3′ for both Wt and mutant allele.
Surgical procedures
Mice were anesthetized with a solution of 20% urethane in physiological solution (0.9% NaCl) with a final dose of 0.8 ml/hg ([23, 24, 31, 32]; 1.6 g/kg). The depth of anesthesia was evaluated by monitoring the pinch withdrawal reflex and other physical signs (respiratory and heart rate). Additional doses (10% of initial dose) were intraperitoneally administered to maintain the level of anesthesia if necessary. The brain surface was routinely moistened with the addition of artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) at body temperature. The head of the mouse was fixed in a stereotaxic apparatus, and a portion of the skull overlying the visual cortex (0.0 mm anteroposterior and 2.7 mm lateral to the lambda suture) was drilled on the right hemisphere. A chamber was created around the craniotomy applying a thin layer of dental cement (Vertex Dental). The mouse was placed with its left eye in front of the monitor at 30 cm, oriented 45° with respect to the medial sagittal plane of the animal. Local field potentials (LFP) were recorded with glass micropipettes (impedance ~ 2 MΩ), filled with ACSF solution and connected to the amplifier head stage with an Ag/AgCl electrode. The microelectrode was positioned into the visual cortex at a depth of 250–300 μm (II/III layer) with a motorized micromanipulator. A common reference Ag/AgCl electrode was placed on the cortical surface in the ACSF cortical bath. Electrophysiological signal was amplified 1000-fold with a multichannel differential amplifier (EXT-02F, NPI), band-pass-filtered (0.1–1000 Hz) and sampled at 2 kHz with 16-bit precision by a National Instruments (NI-usb6251) ADC board controlled by custom-made LabView software. Line frequency 50 Hz noise was removed by means of a linear noise eliminator (Humbug, Quest Scientific). LFP recording lasted around 45 min, after which some animals have been superfused over the visual cortex with Midazolam (5 mg/ml in ACSF; 200 µl volume; BI- Istituto Biochimico Italiano Giovanni Lorenzini S.p.A.) or its vehicle (ACSF). After administration, the mouse was left resting, in the dark, for 1 h. Then, a second round of LFP recording was performed. Visual stimuli were provided via an LCD monitor (120 Hz frame rate, Asus). The stimuli were generated with a custom-made MATLAB program based on Psychophysics Toolbox 3 [33]. The stimulus consisted in a black and white contrast-reversing checkerboard (0.5 Hz; 0.04 cycles/degree). The checkerboard contrast was changed between different traces. The contrast values were calculated using the formula:
where K indicates contrast; L and l indicate, respectively, the maximum and minimum luminance of the stimulus; Lmax and lmax indicate, respectively, the maximum and minimum luminance of the highest luminance stimulus and Lmin and lmin the maximum and minimum luminance of the lowest luminance stimulus. Screen luminance was measured using a photometer (Konica Minolta). Six contrast values were used from maximum to minimum and randomly alternated (100%, 28%, 5.7%, 2.3%, 1.8% and 0%).
Up states (US) and down states (DS) detection
Analysis of slow-wave oscillations was performed computing the short-time root mean square (RMS) power of the data band passed in the gamma band (25–80 Hz). The time course of the RMS power was computed on a moving window of 250 ms width and 150 ms overlap. The distribution of the logarithm of the time-resolved gamma power was bimodal, reflecting high gamma activity during USs and low gamma activity during down states. This distribution was then fitted with a double Gaussian function and the threshold for the discrimination of USs was chosen, minimizing false positives and false negatives, by computing the ROC curve. A cutoff in the minimum up (down) states duration was set to 100 ms, and up (down) state shorter than the cut-off was assigned to the ongoing down (up) state (see [32] for details).
Spectral analysis
Spectrograms have been computed using the mtspecgramc function of the Chronux toolbox, with an overlapping Hammer window of 150 ms width and 50 ms overlap. Multitaper power spectra have been calculated on 90-s traces recorded from anesthetized mice with no visual stimulation, using the mtspectrumc function of the Chronux toolbox [25].
Analysis of visual evoked responses
The visual evoked potential (VEP) of each mouse was calculated as the average of 120–180 trials. The peak amplitude was measured in a window from 90 to 500 ms after the stimulus, with respect to a baseline level computed in the 30–80 ms window. Contrast transfer functions obtained for every mouse have been fitted with a Michaelis Menten function:\(A(k) =\frac{{A}_{\mathrm{max}}*k}{{k}_{H}+k}\), where A is the response amplitude, k is contrast, Amax is the response at "saturating" contrast levels and kH is the contrast at which the amplitude is equal to half M. The VEP slope was calculated as the angular coefficient of a line fitting the VEP in a window of 80 ms centered on the half-peak value. Gamma band power (25–80 Hz) during the response was calculated for every trial as the RMS power of the filtered trace in two different time windows. The windows were 30–80 ms and 90–500 ms after the stimulus, called, respectively, "baseline" and "response"; then the base-10 logarithm of the ratio between response and baseline gamma power has been calculated and averaged among trials.
Statistical analysis for electrophysiology
In the figure legends are reported the number of replicates (n) and the statistical analyses used for every experiment. Based on the number of condition and the number of groups in the comparison, appropriate statistical tests were used to analyze the data. First was analyzed the data distribution by the Shapiro–Wilk normality test. For the comparisons, parametric tests including Student t-tests and one-way and two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with appropriate post hoc test were used if the data were normally distributed; nonparametric test including the Mann–Whitney test and Kruskal–Wallis test with appropriate post hoc test were used for data with non-normal distribution. Results are presented as box plots presenting the 1st and 3rd quartile, the median and the mean. Since each data point represents an experimental mouse, the total number of data points is limited and the box plots do not include whiskers, but all data points are presented, thus allowing a proper understanding of scattering and range. Statistics were computed as described in the figure legends.
All electrophysiological data analysis was performed with custom-made software suite written in MATLAB (https://github.com/DidiLamers/PCDH19_ZebraExplore) [34].
Immunohistochemistry
Mice were anesthetized and perfused transcardially with sucrose 5% and paraformaldehyde 4%. Brains were removed and post-fixed overnight in paraformaldehyde 4%. The day after three washes with PBS were performed and brains were incubated for at least eight hours in 30% sucrose. Finally, brains were included in cryomolds with Tissue-Tek OCT compound (Bio-Optica) and put at − 80 °C until cryostat sectioning. Brains were cut with a cryostat and 20 μm-thick coronal slices were collected on polysine microscope adhesion slides (ThermoFisher). Slices were incubated first in blocking solution (3%BSA, 10% goat serum, 0.4% Triton-X-100, diluted in PBS) for at least 30 min and then they were incubated with primary antibodies overnight at 4 °C. Subsequently, three wash (10 min each) with PBS were performed and brain slices were incubated with fluorophore-conjugated secondary antibodies (Jackson ImmunoResearch Laboratories) for 1 h at room temperature. After the antibody incubation, brain slices were washed and 4',6diamidino-2-phenylindole (DAPI) staining (ThermoFisher) was carried out (DAPI diluted in PBS to a final concentration of 0.5 μg/ml). Finally, another washing step is performed before mounting the coverslips with Mounting Medium (Vecta Shield). Primary antibodies used were: anti-Parvalbumin (Swant, GP72), Shank3 (homemade SHANK3rb [35]).
Microscopic analysis
Brain areas were selected according to the mouse brain atlas of Paxinos and Franklin (Paxinos & Franklin, 2008) from each of the following brain regions at the following bregma coordinates: prefrontal cortex (from 2.4 to 1.7 mm), visual cortex (from − 2.06 to − 2.70 mm), hippocampus (from − 1.06 to − 2.2 mm). Confocal images were obtained using LSM800 confocal microscope (Carl Zeiss) with Zeiss 10 × objective at a resolution of 1024 × 1024 pixels, under the same condition across different mice and brain slices. PV-positive cells were counted in the bilateral areas of each section using ImageJ software.
For Shank3 staining, imaging was performed on a laser-scanning microscope (Leica DMi8) with 63 × oil DIC immersion objective (ACS Apo, NA 1.3) with xy resolution 159 nm and z resolution 345 nm with a stack size of 1.73 µm. Deconvolution of the images was performed using Huygens Essentials 22.04 software (Scientific Volume Imaging B.V.). Subsequently, an analysis of the images was performed with surface and spot tool with Imaris 9.9 Software (Oxford Instruments). Parvalbumin-positive neurons within a specific region of interest (ROI) were selected with the surface tool by tdTomato signal. With the spot tool Shank3-positive spots close to the surfaces (If < or equal to 0.5 µm distance) were determined. Number of spots close to surface within one ROI was normalized on the surface volume within the ROI. Two sections (3 ROIs per section) were analyzed for each hemisphere per animals.
Biochemistry
Cortex and hippocampi were dissected from mouse brain of both sexes. Tissues were homogenized in buffer containing 10 mM Hepes pH 7.4, 2 mM EDTA, protease inhibitors (Roche), and phosphatase inhibitors (Sigma, P8340). Samples were centrifuged at 500 × g for 5 min at 4 °C. Resulting supernatants were centrifuged at 10,000 × g for 15 min at 4 °C. After the centrifugation, pellets were resuspended in buffer composed by 50 mM Hepes pH 7.4, 2 mM EDTA, 2 mM EGTA, 1% triton-X-100 (Sigma), protease inhibitors, and phosphatase inhibitors and centrifuged at 20,000 × g for 80 min at 4 °C. Finally, pellets were resuspended in buffer containing 50 mM Tris pH 9, 1% sodium deoxicholate (Sigma, D6750). Samples were quantified by BCA protein assay (EuroClone) to assess protein concentration. Equal amounts of each sample were separated using SDS-PAGE and subsequently blotted on nitrocellulose membranes using the Trans- Blot Turbo System (BioRad). Membranes were washed in Tris-buffered saline-Tween (TBS-T) (200 mM Tris pH 7.4, 1.5 M NaCl (both Sigma-Aldrich) and 0.1% Tween 20% (BioRad). After 1 h blocking at room temperature with 5% bovine serum albumin or milk in TBS-T, membranes were incubated overnight at 4 °C with primary antibody (Shank3, Santa Cruz Biotechnology, cat. H-160; GABA-A-R-alpha1, Neuromab, cat. 75136; Parvalbumin, Swant, cat. GP72; β3-tubulin, Sigma, cat. T8578). Membranes were washed in TBS-T and then incubated with HRP-conjugated secondary antibodies (Jackson ImmunoResearch) for 1 h at room temperature. After three washes, chemiluminescence was induced using an ECL Western Blotting Substrate kit and further detected using a ChemiDoc XRS + machine. All signals were quantified using ImageLab software and normalized against the values of the respective signal for βIII-tubulin.
Behavioral analysis
Mice of P60-P90 were used for behavioral experiments. Animals were housed in groups of four or five individuals. All of the tests were conducted during the light portion of the cycle. The sample size of animals required for behavioral analysis was estimated using the G*Power 3.1 statistical analysis program, setting a 95% confidence limit (α err. prob. = 0.05) and a power of 80%. The effect size was calculated using the formula Δ/SD (Δ = difference between the groups; SD = standard deviation). Means and standard deviations were estimated based on previous experiments in the laboratory. Based on these parameters, we determined that a sample size of at least 5 animals was necessary to detect any differences between groups.
Repetitive self-grooming test
The spontaneous self-grooming behavior was assayed as described in [36]. Single mouse was placed into a standard cylinder (46 × 23.5 × 20 cm). Cylinders were empty to eliminate digging in the bedding, which is a potentially competing behavior. The room was illuminated at about 40 lx. A front-mounted CC TV camera (Security Cameras Direct) was placed at circa 1 m from the cages to record the sessions. Sessions were video-taped for 20 min. The first 10 min of habituation was not scored. Cumulative time spent grooming all the body regions during the second 10 min of the test session was measured.
Novel object recognition test
The novel object recognition test was performed in an open plastic arena (60 × 50 × 30 cm). The test had three phases: the habituation on the first day, when mice were accustomed to the test arena for 10 min; the familiarization; and the novel object recognition the day after. In the familiarization phase, two identical objects were placed in the middle of the arena equidistant from the walls and from each other. Mice were placed between the two objects until it had completed 30 s of cumulative object exploration (20 min cut-off). Experimenter measured the time mice was within approximately 1 cm of an object with its nose toward the object. Climbing the object or pointing the nose toward ceiling near the object were not considered exploring behaviors. After familiarization, mice returned to the home cage until they were tested for novel recognition after 5 min, 120 min and 24 h. In the novel recognition phase, a novel object (never seen before) took the place of one of the two familiars. Scoring of object recognition was performed as during the familiarization phase. For each mouse, the role (familiar or new object) as well as the relative position of the two objects were randomly permuted. The objects used for the test were white plastic cylinders and colored plastic Lego stacks of different shapes. The arena was cleaned with 70% ethanol after each trial. Performance was analyzed by calculating the discrimination index (N − F/N + F), where N = the time spent exploring the novel object, and F = the time spent exploring the familiar object.
Spatial object recognition test
Spatial object recognition test was performed in an arena according to [30], with minor modifications that consisted in an opaque white Plexiglass cage (58 × 50 × 43 cm) that was dimly lit from above (27 lx) and two visual cues that were placed above two adjacent walls. The visual cues consisted of a black and white stripped pattern (21 × 19.5 cm) that was affixed to the center of the northern wall and a black and gray checkered pattern (26.5 × 20 cm) that was placed in the center of the western wall. Objects were placed across the visual cues. Mice were habituated to the arena for 10 min the day before the test. Twenty-four hours later, it is initially performed a training session where mice were allowed to familiarize with two different objects. The experimenter measured the time spent in sniffing both objects until the mouse completed 30 s in exploring objects (cut-off 20 min). Exploring behavior was defined as mouse having its nose directed toward the object and within approximately 1 cm of the object [37]; climbing or sitting were not considered exploration behaviors. After 5 min, 120 min and 24 h, mice were allowed to re-explore the cage where one object was moved in a new position. Between two sessions, mice returned to their home cage. Cage and object were carefully cleaned with 70% ethanol before and after all behavioral procedures. Performance was analyzed by calculating a discrimination index (N − F/N + F), where N = the time spent exploring the moved object during the test, and F = the time spent exploring the unmoved object during the test.
Balance beam walking test
The beam apparatus consisted of 1 m beams with a flat surface of 12 mm or 6 mm width resting 50 cm above the table top on two poles. A black box was placed at the end of the beam as the finish point. Nesting material from home cages was placed in the black box to attract the mouse to the finish point. A lamp (with 60-W light bulb) was used to shine light above the start point and served as an aversive stimulus. A video camera was set on a tripod to record the performance. On training days, each mouse crossed the 12 mm beam 3 times and then the 6 mm beam 3 times. The time required to cross to the escape box at the other end (80 cm away) was measured with a stopwatch. The stopwatch started when the nose of the mouse began to cross the beam, and stopped when the animal reaches the escape box. Once the mice are in the safe box, they are allowed some time (~ 15 secs) to rest there. Before the next trial the mice rest for 10 min in their home cages between training sessions on the two beams. On the test day, times to cross each beam were recorded. Two successful trials in which the mouse did not stall on the beam are averaged. The beams and box were cleaned with towels soaked with 70% ethanol and then water before the next beam was placed on the apparatus.
Rotarod test
The rotarod apparatus (Ugo Basile, Biological Research Apparatus, Varese, Italy) was used to measure fore and hindlimb motor coordination and balance. During the training period, each mouse was placed on the rotarod at a constant speed (12 and 32 rpm) for a maximum of 120 s, and the latency to fall off the rotarod within this time period was recorded. Mice received four trials per day for 4 straight days. The fourth trial of each day was evaluated for statistical analysis.
Pole test
In the pole test, the mouse was placed on a vertical wire-mesh pole (90 cm length, 1 cm diameter) with its head facing upwards. Mice were habituated to descend the pole in 2 trials per day for 2 days. On test day (third day) mice were subjected to 5 trials: the total time taken to turn the body and to descend the pole was recorded according to [38]. A cutoff of 60 s was given. Data were shown as mean of 5 trials evaluated during the test day.
Wire hanging test
The limb force was tested by positioning a mouse on the top of a wire cage lid (19 × 29 cm) that was turned upside down at approximately 25 cm above a surface with the bedding material. The grip of the mouse was ensured by gently waving three times before rotating the lid as described in [39]. The latency to fall onto the bedding was recorded over a maximum period of 300 s.
Sociability and social novelty test
The sociability tests were performing in an apparatus called three-chambered box. It is formed by a rectangular transparent polycarbonate apparatus with three-chamber (width = 42.5 cm, height = 22.2 cm, central chamber length = 17.8 cm, and side chamber lengths = 19.1 cm) as previously described in [36]. The three-chamber box was illuminated by diffuse incandescent lighting (15 lx), and a video camera was located directly over the center of the open field.
First, the test includes a habituation phase during which the mouse was placed in the middle chamber free to explore all the compartments for 10 min. After this period, novel conspecific mouse, that had no previous interaction with the test mouse, was placed in one of the side chambers. The doors were unblocked and the subject mouse was given the possibility to explore for 10 min either an empty chamber or a chamber containing the stranger mouse. In the 10-min trial, the duration of contact with the counterpart was recorded. Immediately after sociability test, without cleaning the apparatus, it was performed the social novelty test putting an unfamiliar mouse in the empty wire cage. Time spent in each chamber was recorded. The data were expressed in sociability index (SI) and social novelty preference index (SNI) as follows: SI = (time exploring novel mouse 1 – time exploring empty cage) / (time exploring novel mouse 1 + time exploring empty cage) and SNI = (time exploring novel mouse 2 – time exploring familiar mouse) / (time exploring novel mouse 2 + time exploring familiar mouse).
Pharmacological treatment
Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Wt, Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Fl/Wt, Shank3 Wt and Shank3 KO mice were treated with a selective positive allosteric modulator of GABAA receptors: Ganaxolone. Ganaxolone (Tocris Cat. No. 2531) was resuspended in ethanol and betaciclodextrin (Sigma-Aldrich 332607). Mice received an intraperitoneal injection of ganaxolone (5 mg/Kg) or vehicle 30 min before each behavioral test.
Results
Gain control is impaired in Shank3 KO mice
As the visual cortex serves as a prototypical model for studying changes in brain circuitry associated with various forms of ASD [40,41,42,43], we decided to analyze sensory processing in the visual cortex in Shank3Δ11−/− [27] (referred as Shank3 KO) mice by conducting local field potential (LFP) recordings. The mice were anesthetized with urethane to induce a slow-wave-like sleep as in [32, 44]. During this state, the brain cortex exhibited oscillations between states of very low neuronal activity (known as down states, DS) and states of high neuronal activity (known as up states, US). Our results demonstrate that USs in Shank3 KO mice had a longer duration and a higher gamma power (25–80 Hz) compared to controls. However, there were no significant differences in the frequency or median amplitude of the up states between the two groups (Fig. 1A and Additional file 1: Fig. 1). This result is consistent with an enhancement of the spectral power for the frequency band 4–100 Hz, as represented in Fig. 1B, and suggests a reduction of the inhibitory drive. To further investigate the role of Shank3 in regulating inhibition, we measured visual contrast gain control, which is heavily reliant on the inhibitory feedback of fast-spiking interneurons on pyramidal neurons. This feedback is essential for adapting the limited dynamic range of pyramidal cell firing to the large dynamic range of sensory stimuli [45, 46]. We recorded local field potentials (LFP) in response to a checkerboard contrast-reversal stimulus and measured the amplitude of the visual evoked potentials (VEPs) in relation to contrast levels (Fig. 1C). Our findings indicate that the response of Shank3 KO mice reached a saturation level for lower contrast values, compared to the wild-type mice (Fig. 1D). The gain of the visual response was estimated for each mouse by fitting the contrast curves with a Michaelis–Menten function. The distribution of the two parameters (AM, asymptotic response amplitude and K50, half-saturating contrast) was compared between Shank3 KO and Wt mice (Additional file 1: Fig. 2A–C). We found that K50 was lower in the Shank3 KO compared to Wt mice, indicating the presence of saturation at a lower contrast and increased contrast gain in Shank3 KO. Additionally, the recorded VEPs were steeper in Shank3 KO mice compared to Wt mice, as represented in the analysis of the slope (Additional file 1: Fig. 2C). Spectrograms of the VEP recordings show a power increase during visual response in Shank3 KO mice (Additional file 1: Fig. 2D–E). We quantified the power increase in the gamma spectral band both during response and in presence of a blank stimulus as a function of the stimulus contrast (Additional file 1: Fig. 2E). These curves appear to be similar to those obtained when measuring VEP amplitudes (Fig. 1D), confirming that contrast gain is greater in the Shank3 KO mice. The increased gain we measured in response to visual stimuli is suggestive of an impairment of inhibition from fast-spiking PV interneurons, which are responsible for gain control in the visual cortex [45]. Therefore, our data suggests that the major cause of the alterations in the visual response is due to inhibitory impairment. To test this hypothesis, we measured contrast gain in Shank3 KO mice before and after superfusion with the GABAA receptor agonist midazolam (5 mg/ml). As shown in Fig. 1E–G, pharmacological activation of GABAA receptor was sufficient to rescue gain control in Shank3 KO mice compared to Shank3 KO mice treated with vehicle only (ACSF). Midazolam superfusion per se acts on resting activity reducing the high excitability apparent in Shank3 KO mice as shown in Additional file 1: Fig. 3A–D, with no differences in US duration, frequency and amplitude. Given that Shank3 is localized on excitatory synapses only, we can rule out that the inhibition impairment originates in decreased number or strength of inhibitory synapses. Far more likely is that the excitatory drive on PV interneurons is defective, leading to reduced recruitment of PV interneurons and to impaired excitation/inhibition balance.
Preferential deletion of Shank3 in PV neurons causes specific behavioral alterations
Our results align with substantial evidence suggesting a role for PV interneurons in the etiology of neuropsychiatric disorders [16, 47, 48]. Therefore, to determine the specific contribution of Shank3 deletion in these neurons to the pathogenesis of PMS and ASD, we utilized Cre-lox technology to generate mice with selective deletion of Shank3 in PV neurons. We first bred Shank3 floxed mice with transgenic mice carrying TdTomato gene which can only be expressed after the excision of a loxP-flanked STOP cassette by Cre-mediated recombination. Subsequently, we crossed these mice with the line expressing Cre-recombinase under the PV promoter. This allowed us to easily visualize PV neurons with the deletion of Shank3 using TdTomato. PCR analysis confirmed the genotype of these mice (Fig. 2A). Furthermore, immunofluorescence showed that TdTomato expression increased during brain development (Fig. 2B) and that cells expressing TdTomato were also positive for parvalbumin expression (Fig. 2C). We confirmed that Shank3 was downregulated in Pv-Cre+/− TdTomatoWt/fl Shank3Wt/Fl mice compared to controls (Fig. 2D). Subsequently, we examined the impact of selective deletion of Shank3 in PV neurons on mouse behavior. Surprisingly, our analysis revealed that the deletion of a single Shank3 allele in PV neurons was sufficient to induce behavioral changes, which contrasts with the absence of cognitive deficits in Shank3 full KO mice in heterozygosity [30]. Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl spent significantly more time in doing grooming compared to Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Wt (referred as control mice) suggesting that Shank3 deletion in PV neurons is sufficient to cause repetitive behavior (Fig. 3A). Memory performance was also affected by selective deletion of Shank3 in PV neurons, as Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl exhibited deficits in both the novel object recognition test (Fig. 3B) and the spatial object recognition test (Fig. 3C). Furthermore, Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl displayed impaired motor coordination taking longer to cross the beam than control mice (Fig. 3D) and displayed some motor problems, as demonstrated by the results of the rotarod test (Fig. 3E). However, they did not exhibit any impairments during the pole test (Fig. 3F) nor a reduction in muscular strength (Fig. 3G). The three-chamber test did not reveal any differences between Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl mice and control mice in sociability or social novelty (Fig. 3H). Social performances were also unaffected in the homozygous Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Fl/Fl mice, indicating that Shank3 deletion only in PV neurons was insufficient to affect mice sociability (Additional file 1: Fig. 4A). Finally, we investigated whether homozygous Shank3 deletion in PV neurons exacerbates the phenotype observed in Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl mice. However, as demonstrated by the results of the grooming test and novel object recognition test, we observed only a slight deterioration in behavior compared to heterozygous mice, which did not appear to be significant (Additional file 1: Fig. 4B and C).
Shank3 deletion in PV neurons induces cortical hyperexcitability
Next, we recorded the resting state LFP in the visual cortex of Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl mice under urethane anesthesia. Power spectra analysis showed that Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl mice had a significantly higher spectral power compared to controls at both low (< 5 Hz) and high frequencies (> 100 Hz Fig. 4A–C). Further detailed analysis of the SWA revealed that there was no significant difference in the frequency and duration of up states between the two groups (Fig. 4E, F). However, the amplitude of up states was larger in Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl mice, suggesting higher neuronal firing during up states in this group (Fig. 4 G). Additionally, the measured power during USs was significantly higher for Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl mice in HF band (> 100 Hz) (Fig. 4I). These findings are all indicative of a higher degree of excitability, suggesting that selective deletion of Shank3 in PV neurons caused a specific excitation/inhibition (E/I) balance that leads to cortical hyperexcitability.
We subsequently conducted a quantitative analysis to determine the density of parvalbumin (PV) neurons in different brain regions that exhibit high levels of Shank3 expression. We conducted an analysis of the number of Tdt + and Pv + cells and the percentage of colocalization across multiple brain regions, including the mPFC, CA1, CA3, DG, and visual cortex. Our results indicate that in all brain regions examined, 77% of Tdt + cells also express Pv + , suggesting high specificity of the model. In the hippocampus of Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl mice, we observed a significant decrease in the number of PV neurons specifically in the dentate gyrus, whereas no significant difference was detected in the CA1 and CA3 regions (Fig. 5A). Similarly, in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) of Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl mice, we observed a reduction in the number of PV neurons when compared to Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Wt mice (Fig. 5B). However, in the visual cortex, no discernible difference was observed between Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl and Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Wt mice (Fig. 5C). Similar results were also obtained when we focused on the cells that express both TdTomato and Parvalbumin (Tdt + Pv + cells) (Additional file 1: Fig. 5) confirming the Cre specificity.
Potentiating inhibitory activity reverses behavioral deficits caused by Shank3 deletion
We discovered that the deletion of Shank3 in PV neurons leads to brain hyperexcitability. To investigate whether positive allosteric modulation of GABAA receptors by the neurosteroid ganaxolone could improve the deficits in Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl mice we administered ganaxolone (5 mg/Kg) or vehicle intraperitoneally to mice before conducting behavioral tests. Due to their broad availability and safety, benzodiazepines and other positive allosteric modulators of GABAA receptors could be a near-term strategy to improve the symptoms of PMS patients. However, one difficulty in using benzodiazepines is their sedative properties. For this reason, for the in vivo experiments we chose ganaxolone because it has fewer side effects. We first assessed whether ganaxolone could ameliorate stereotyped behavior by performing the grooming test and found that ganaxolone administration resulted in a significant reduction in grooming time compared to the vehicle-injected mice (Fig. 6A). In addition, ganaxolone treatment improved memory deficits in Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl mice as shown by the results of novel object recognition test (Fig. 6B) and spatial object recognition test (Fig. 6C). Moreover, ganaxolone treatment rescued motor coordination deficits in Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl (Fig. 6D).
Finally, to better characterize how Shank3 deletion in the entire brain affect PV neurons we quantified the number of PV neurons in the hippocampus, medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), and visual cortex of Shank3 knockout (KO) mice. Surprisingly, we did not observe any significant differences in the number of PV neurons in these brain regions compared to wild-type (Wt) mice (Fig. 7A–C). However, we did observe a significant reduction in the expression of the GABA-A-R-alpha1 subunit in the postsynaptic density (PSD)-enriched fraction from the hippocampus of Shank3 KO mice compared to Wt and Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl (Fig. 7D), suggesting that ganaxolone treatment might have a beneficial effect also in Shank3 KO mice. In PSD-enriched fraction from the cortex we did not detect any alteration in GABA-A-R-alpha1 subunit (Fig. 7E); moreover, similar to the total cortex, we did not observe any significant differences in GABA-A-R-alpha1 expression between the groups analyzed in PSD-enriched fraction from the visual cortex (data not shown). To evaluate the effects of ganaxolone treatment on Wt and Shank3 KO mice, we injected the mice with ganaxolone (5 mg/Kg i.p.) or vehicle 30 min before conducting behavioral tests. Our results demonstrate that ganaxolone treatment rescued memory impairment in Shank3 KO mice at 5-min, 120-min and 24-h delay, as evidenced by the results of novel object recognition test (Fig. 7F). Additionally, acute treatment with ganaxolone was also able to rescue repetitive behavior in Shank3 KO mice to the levels of Wt mice (Fig. 7G). In conclusion, our findings demonstrated that PV neurons activity is sensitive to Shank3 ablation and that restoring their function with ganaxolone may represent a possible pharmaceutical approach for ameliorating Shank3-related behavioral alterations.
Discussion
This study provides significant evidence demonstrating that Shank3 plays a crucial role in modulating the activity of inhibitory circuit in vivo. Our data indicate that Shank3 deletion leads to a severe impairment in the recruitment of PV interneurons and that the deletion of Shank3 specifically in PV-positive neurons is sufficient to recapitulate some of the behavioral deficits observed in individuals with PMS, such as repetitive behavior, memory impairments and motor problems. Additionally, local field potential recordings of Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl demonstrated that these behavioral deficits are associated to cortical hyperexcitability. Finally, we showed that enhancing the activity of GABAA receptors with ganaxolone may be a viable strategy for ameliorating some of the behavioral symptoms resulting from Shank3 deletion. Previous research has shown that alterations in the balance between excitation and inhibition (E-I) are present in various models of neurodevelopmental disorders and have been strongly linked to epilepsy [17, 49,50,51,52]. Notably, at least a third of individuals with PMS develop epilepsy [3] which is consistent with the impairment of the PV inhibitory feedback observed in our study.
Although GABAergic neurons represent a minority cell type, they control the activity level of principal neurons in the brain. The loss or dysfunction of interneurons has been implicated in numerous neuropsychiatric disorders [49, 53,54,55] and multiple studies support the hypothesis that specific symptoms of ASDs may be caused by an increase in the ratio of excitatory to inhibitory synaptic transmission [56, 57].
Numerous studies have provided evidence that Shank family proteins are extensively expressed in GABAergic neurons where they seem to be crucial for their function and development [10, 58]. Preferential deletion of Shank3 from interneurons has been linked to hyperexcitability of pyramidal neurons and increased sensitivity to sensory stimuli [19]. Recently, the visual cortex has emerged as a valuable model for evaluating cortical processing in mouse models of neurodevelopmental disorders [40,41,42,43, 59, 60]. Altered visual cortical function has been observed in Rett syndrome patients and in mouse models carrying mutations in MeCP2 or FOXG1, thereby supporting the notion that VEPs can serve as a dependable biomarker for assessing the pathological state of the brain [61, 62]. Although we did not observe spontaneous epileptic seizures in Shank3 KO mice, we found a higher degree of excitability in anesthetized mice both in resting state and in response to visual stimulation. In resting state, the increased excitability of the Shank3 KO mice manifested as up states that were longer and had higher power in the 25–80 Hz range. During up states, cortical cell membrane potential is depolarized to subthreshold level and cells are more excitable [63] as clearly demonstrated by the transients observed in the spectrograms (Fig. 1A). The increased duration of up states is reflected by an increased power in the 10–100 Hz range (Fig. 1B). Our findings align with those of a mouse model of Fragile X Syndrome, which also displayed longer up states and hyperexcitability [64]. In addition to studying slow-wave activity, we have also investigated gain control, a fundamental property of neurons that allows to scale the response to excitatory inputs by means of feed-forward inhibition, thus reducing the incremental change of the response as the input strength increases [28]. Our results reveal that contrast gain control is impaired in the Shank3 KO mice, and we hypothesized that this is due to a reduction in the recruitment of inhibitory interneurons by excitatory neurons. Indeed, the administration of midazolam, a positive modulator of GABA currents, reversed the gain control deficit observed in Shank3 KO mice. Previous research has demonstrated that contrast sensitivity gain control is regulated by the feedback of fast-spiking PV interneurons that are pivotal regulators of contrast transfer function in V1 [45]. Interestingly, the changes in spectral power observed in the Shank3 KO and in the Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl are complementary (Fig. 4A). In the Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl the spectral power in the gamma band is similar to that of the control group, while there is a clear increase in power at low (< 5 Hz) and high frequencies (> 100 Hz). The increased power at low frequencies is likely due to the larger amplitude of up states that determine a larger component of the fundamental harmonic of slow-wave activity. Furthermore, EEG monitoring in both animal models and humans has shown that some epileptic conditions are associated with enhanced slow oscillations at frequencies < 0.5 Hz [65, 66]. On the opposite end of the Fourier spectra, increased power at frequencies > 100 Hz may indicate the presence of an epileptic focus. Indeed, intracranial recordings from the epileptic hippocampus (both in animal and human models) have reported fast ripples associated with increased epileptogenicity [67].
Different studies have shown that Shank proteins play a significant role in regulating synaptic transmission in parvalbumin (PV) neurons. Specifically, Shank1 has been shown to be highly expressed in PV neurons and is involved in the regulation of excitatory synaptic transmission in PV basket cells [68]. Additionally, deletion of Shank2 in PV neurons has been found to cause hyperactivity, increased self-grooming, and suppressed brain excitation [69]. Similarly, Shank3 has been found to be expressed in PV neurons and its knockout has been associated with reduced numbers of PV neurons in the striatum of mice [25].
To directly address the roles of Shank3 in regulating E/I balance and better understand the mechanisms responsible for the hyperexcitable state observed in the Shank3 KO mouse, we used a PV-restricted conditional knockout approach. Our findings revealed that cell type-specific heterozygous deletion of Shank3 in PV neurons was sufficient to induce behavioral alterations in contrast to what we found in Shank3 full KO mice that did not exhibit cognitive defects in heterozygosis [30], strengthening the importance of the modulation of E-I balance during brain maturation [70]. Our study revealed that conditional deletion of Shank3 in PV neurons caused an increase in repetitive behavior, reduced memory performance and alterations in motor coordination.
Surprisingly, the selective knockdown of Shank3 in PV neurons did not significantly affect social behavior, despite the crucial role played by PV-positive neurons in regulating social behavior [68, 71, 72] and the strong social deficits observed in Shank3 KO mice [30]. This suggests that the expression of Shank3 in PV cells may not be critical for regulating social behavior in mice, or that compensatory mechanisms may be at play in the conditional knockout (cKO) mice, which could mask the effects of Shank3 deletion in PV cells on social behavior. It is possible that other GABAergic or excitatory neurons could compensate for the loss of Shank3 in PV cells, thereby maintaining social behavior in the cKO mice. Interestingly, a similar observation has been made regarding Shank2, where the deletion of Shank2 specifically in PV-positive neurons has minimal impact on social interaction and communication, despite global Shank2 knockout mice exhibiting pronounced impairments in social behavior [69]. This highlights the complexity of the role of Shank proteins in regulating behavior and the need for further investigation to fully understand their functions in different neuronal populations.
The electrophysiological phenotype of the Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl mice is complex. Up states have a larger amplitude with a high spectral power only at frequencies > 100 Hz compared to Shank3 KO mice. However, Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl mice don’t exhibit increased power in the 25–80 Hz gamma band range compared to Wt (Fig. 4). We frequently observed large amplitude events (> 1 mV) suggestive of hypersynchronous, interictal events. Thus, the partial deletion of Shank3 in a subset of cortical neurons causes a more severe hyperexcitable phenotype than the complete KO. On the other hand, the two models have subtle differences in phenotype: the functional and behavioral phenotype in the Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl mouse is associated with hyperexcitability, while the Shank3 KO mouse presents a more complex ASD-like picture but no clear signs of hyperexcitability except for the deficit in gain control. A possible interpretation of this difference is that the loss of Shank3 may lead to impairments in excitatory synapses, which in the complete KO mouse, could result in a reduction of excitatory drive on both pyramidal neurons and interneurons, thus partially compensating for the E-I balance. In contrast, the PV model with Shank3 deletion only in PV interneurons has reduced excitatory drive on these specific interneurons, leading to a stronger imbalance toward excitation. However, further studies are needed to fully understand the mechanisms underlying the observed differences in phenotype between these two models.
The reduction in PV neurons observed in the hippocampus and mPFC of PV-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl mice highlights the importance of GABAergic signaling in proper brain development and function. This finding also suggests that compounds targeting deficits in the GABAergic system, such as ganaxolone, may have therapeutic potential for treating brain disorders characterized by E/I imbalances, such as ASD [57, 73]. Indeed, the administration of midazolam showed that pharmacological reinforcement of inhibition was sufficient to restore gain control in Shank3 KO mice, possibly normalizing the E/I imbalance. However, although being effective GABAA receptor agonists, benzodiazepines often cause side effects including sedation. Ganaxolone is a synthetic neurosteroid that acts as a positive allosteric modulator of GABAA receptors, enhancing the activity of the inhibitory neurotransmitter GABA. Importantly, ganaxolone has been shown to be effective in reducing seizures and improving behavior in animal models of ASD [74, 75]. Additionally, ganaxolone has a more favorable side effect profile than benzodiazepines [76, 77] making it a promising candidate for clinical use. We demonstrated that a single dose of ganaxolone was effective in restoring normal behavior in Pv-Cre+/− Shank3Wt/Fl mice. Additionally, while we did not observe any changes in the number of PV neurons in Shank3 KO mice, our results showed that Shank3 knockdown led to a significant reduction in the expression of GABA-A-R-alpha1 in the hippocampus. Treatment with ganaxolone improved memory deficits and repetitive behaviors in Shank3 KO mice, indicating that modulation of the GABAergic system may be a viable therapeutic strategy for individuals with Shank3 mutations.
Although a more comprehensive molecular characterization remains to be determined, our results suggest that that targeting the GABAergic system, specifically by enhancing GABAergic transmission through allosteric modulation of GABAA receptors, may represent a potential therapeutic strategy for treating some symptoms associated with Shank3-related disorders. Our study demonstrated that Shank3 is involved in the modulation of PV neurons activity and emphasize the challenges and importance of defining the specific neuronal population and circuits causative of behavioral deficits due to Shank3 deletion in order to develop effective therapies for PMS and ASD.
Future studies should focus on identifying the molecular mechanisms underlying the effects of Shank3 deletion on PV neurons and its impact on other GABAergic neurons. Finally, chronic treatment with ganaxolone should be further evaluated to assess its long-term effects on behavior in Shank3 KO mice.
Limitations
A major limitation of this study is that the neurobiological mechanisms linking Shank3 deletion in PV neurons and behavioral alterations remain to be investigated. While our study provides evidence for a specific E/I imbalance and cortical hyperexcitability, more comprehensive electrophysiological and molecular analyses of PV inhibitory neurons in different brain areas are necessary to establish a causal link between cellular and synaptic deficits and ASD-like behaviors. Additionally, although PV neurons make up 50% of GABAergic cells, the role of Shank3 in other classes of inhibitory neurons should be explored in future studies. Finally, while our study suggests that ganaxolone may represent a promising therapy for PMS and ASD, further studies are required to fully characterize its efficacy and safety profile. Additionally, future research is needed to investigate the effects of ganaxolone administration during brain development and in other Shank3 KO mouse models and in the full Shank3 KO mouse model [78].
Conclusions
Overall, our findings contribute to a better understanding of the role of Shank3 in the pathophysiology of PMS and ASD and pave the way for the development of new therapeutic strategies targeting the GABAergic system. However, further investigations are needed to fully elucidate the cellular and molecular mechanisms underlying Shank3-related synaptic deficits and to assess the efficacy of chronic treatment with ganaxolone or other GABAergic modulators in treating these disorders. Ultimately, these efforts may lead to the development of more effective and targeted treatments for PMS and ASD.
Availability of data and materials
All data are available in the main text or the Additional files. The materials that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
Abbreviations
- ASD:
-
Autism spectrum disorder
- KO:
-
Knockout
- LFP:
-
Local field potential
- PMS:
-
Phelan–McDermid syndrome
- PV:
-
Parvalbumin-positive neurons
- VEPs:
-
Visual evoked potentials
- Wt:
-
Wild-type
References
Bonaglia MC, Giorda R, Borgatti R, Felisari G, Gagliardi C, Selicorni A, et al. Disruption of the ProSAP2 gene in a t(12;22)(q24.1;q13.3) is associated with the 22q133 deletion syndrome. Am J Hum Genet. 2001;69(2):261–8.
Phelan K, Boccuto L, Powell CM, Boeckers TM, van Ravenswaaij-Arts C, Rogers RC, et al. Phelan-McDermid syndrome: a classification system after 30 years of experience. Orphanet J Rare Dis. 2022;17(1):27.
Phelan K, McDermid HE. The 22q13.3 deletion syndrome (Phelan-McDermid syndrome). Mol Syndromol. 2012;2(3–5):186–201.
Phelan MC, Rogers RC, Saul RA, Stapleton GA, Sweet K, McDermid H, et al. 22q13 deletion syndrome. Am J Med Genet. 2001;101(2):91–9.
Sarasua SM, Dwivedi A, Boccuto L, Chen CF, Sharp JL, Rollins JD, et al. 22q13.2q13.32 genomic regions associated with severity of speech delay, developmental delay, and physical features in Phelan-McDermid syndrome. Genet Med. 2014;16(4):318–28.
Durand CM, Perroy J, Loll F, Perrais D, Fagni L, Bourgeron T, et al. SHANK3 mutations identified in autism lead to modification of dendritic spine morphology via an actin-dependent mechanism. Mol Psychiatry. 2012;17(1):71–84.
Leblond CS, Nava C, Polge A, Gauthier J, Huguet G, Lumbroso S, et al. Meta-analysis of SHANK mutations in autism spectrum disorders: a gradient of severity in cognitive impairments. PLoS Genet. 2014;10(9): e1004580.
Durand CM, Betancur C, Boeckers TM, Bockmann J, Chaste P, Fauchereau F, et al. Mutations in the gene encoding the synaptic scaffolding protein SHANK3 are associated with autism spectrum disorders. Nat Genet. 2007;39(1):25–7.
Rudy B, Fishell G, Lee S, Hjerling-Leffler J. Three groups of interneurons account for nearly 100% of neocortical GABAergic neurons. Dev Neurobiol. 2011;71(1):45–61.
Marín O. Interneuron dysfunction in psychiatric disorders. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13(2):107–20.
Mann EO, Paulsen O. Role of GABAergic inhibition in hippocampal network oscillations. Trends Neurosci. 2007;30(7):343–9.
Fritschy JM. Epilepsy, E/I balance and GABA(A) receptor plasticity. Front Mol Neurosci. 2008;1:5.
Nakazawa K, Zsiros V, Jiang Z, Nakao K, Kolata S, Zhang S, et al. GABAergic interneuron origin of schizophrenia pathophysiology. Neuropharmacology. 2012;62(3):1574–83.
Wonders CP, Anderson SA. The origin and specification of cortical interneurons. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7(9):687–96.
Cancedda L, Fiumelli H, Chen K, Poo MM. Excitatory GABA action is essential for morphological maturation of cortical neurons in vivo. J Neurosci. 2007;27(19):5224–35.
Ferguson BR, Gao WJ. PV interneurons: critical regulators of E/I balance for prefrontal cortex-dependent behavior and psychiatric disorders. Front Neural Circuits. 2018;12:37.
Heise C, Taha E, Murru L, Ponzoni L, Cattaneo A, Guarnieri FC, et al. eEF2K/eEF2 pathway controls the excitation/inhibition balance and susceptibility to epileptic seizures. Cereb Cortex. 2017;27(3):2226–48.
Chattopadhyaya B, Cristo GD. GABAergic circuit dysfunctions in neurodevelopmental disorders. Front Psychiatry. 2012;3:51.
Chen Q, Deister CA, Gao X, Guo B, Lynn-Jones T, Chen N, et al. Dysfunction of cortical GABAergic neurons leads to sensory hyper-reactivity in a Shank3 mouse model of ASD. Nat Neurosci. 2020;23(4):520–32.
Lawrence YA, Kemper TL, Bauman ML, Blatt GJ. Parvalbumin-, calbindin-, and calretinin-immunoreactive hippocampal interneuron density in autism. Acta Neurol Scand. 2010;121(2):99–108.
Butt SJ, Stacey JA, Teramoto Y, Vagnoni C. A role for GABAergic interneuron diversity in circuit development and plasticity of the neonatal cerebral cortex. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2017;43:149–55.
Xue M, Atallah BV, Scanziani M. Equalizing excitation-inhibition ratios across visual cortical neurons. Nature. 2014;511(7511):596–600.
Kann O. The interneuron energy hypothesis: implications for brain disease. Neurobiol Dis. 2016;90:75–85.
Korotkova T, Fuchs EC, Ponomarenko A, von Engelhardt J, Monyer H. NMDA receptor ablation on parvalbumin-positive interneurons impairs hippocampal synchrony, spatial representations, and working memory. Neuron. 2010;68(3):557–69.
Filice F, Vörckel KJ, Sungur A, Wöhr M, Schwaller B. Reduction in parvalbumin expression not loss of the parvalbumin-expressing GABA interneuron subpopulation in genetic parvalbumin and shank mouse models of autism. Mol Brain. 2016;9:10.
Gogolla N, Takesian AE, Feng G, Fagiolini M, Hensch TK. Sensory integration in mouse insular cortex reflects GABA circuit maturation. Neuron. 2014;83(4):894–905.
Schmeisser MJ, Ey E, Wegener S, Bockmann J, Stempel AV, Kuebler A, et al. Autistic-like behaviours and hyperactivity in mice lacking ProSAP1/Shank2. Nature. 2012;486(7402):256–60.
Pouille F, Marin-Burgin A, Adesnik H, Atallah BV, Scanziani M. Input normalization by global feedforward inhibition expands cortical dynamic range. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12(12):1577–85.
Garrido D, Beretta S, Grabrucker S, Bauer HF, Bayer D, Sala C, et al. Shank2/3 double knockout-based screening of cortical subregions links the retrosplenial area to the loss of social memory in autism spectrum disorders. Mol Psychiatry. 2022;8:1–13.
Vicidomini C, Ponzoni L, Lim D, Schmeisser MJ, Reim D, Morello N, et al. Pharmacological enhancement of mGlu5 receptors rescues behavioral deficits in SHANK3 knock-out mice. Mol Psychiatry. 2017;22(5):689–702.
de Vivo L, Landi S, Panniello M, Baroncelli L, Chierzi S, Mariotti L, Spolidoro M, Pizzorusso T, Maffei L, Ratto GM. Extracellular matrix inhibits structural and functional plasticity of dendritic spines in the adult visual cortex. Nat Commun. 2013;4:1484.
Petrucco L, Pracucci E, Brondi M, Ratto GM, Landi S. Epileptiform activity in the mouse visual cortex interferes with cortical processing in connected areas. Sci Rep. 2017;7:40054.
Brainard DH. The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision. 1997;10:433–6.
Lamers D, Landi S, Mezzena R, Baroncelli L, Pillai V, Cruciani F, et al. Perturbation of cortical excitability in a conditional model of PCDH19 disorder. Cells. 2022;11:12.
Lutz AK, Pfaender S, Incearap B, Ioannidis V, Ottonelli I, Föhr KJ, et al. Autism-associated SHANK3 mutations impair maturation of neuromuscular junctions and striated muscles. Sci Transl Med. 2020;12:547.
Mossa A, Pagano J, Ponzoni L, Tozzi A, Vezzoli E, Sciaccaluga M, et al. Developmental impaired Akt signaling in the Shank1 and Shank3 double knock-out mice. Mol Psychiatry. 2021;26:1928–44.
Bevins RA, Besheer J. Object recognition in rats and mice: a one-trial non-matching-to-sample learning task to study “recognition memory.” Nat Protoc. 2006;1(3):1306–11.
Hickey MA, Kosmalska A, Enayati J, Cohen R, Zeitlin S, Levine MS, et al. Extensive early motor and non-motor behavioral deficits are followed by striatal neuronal loss in knock-in Huntington’s disease mice. Neuroscience. 2008;157(1):280–95.
Oliván S, Calvo AC, Rando A, Muñoz MJ, Zaragoza P, Osta R. Comparative study of behavioural tests in the SOD1G93A mouse model of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Exp Anim. 2015;64(2):147–53.
Krishnan K, Wang BS, Lu J, Wang L, Maffei A, Cang J, et al. MeCP2 regulates the timing of critical period plasticity that shapes functional connectivity in primary visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2015;112(34):E4782–91.
Artoni P, Piffer A, Vinci V, LeBlanc J, Nelson CA, Hensch TK, et al. Deep learning of spontaneous arousal fluctuations detects early cholinergic defects across neurodevelopmental mouse models and patients. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2020;117(38):23298–303.
Tatavarty V, Torrado Pacheco A, Groves Kuhnle C, Lin H, Koundinya P, Miska NJ, et al. Autism-associated Shank3 is essential for homeostatic compensation in rodent V1. Neuron. 2020;106(5):769-77.e4.
Kissinger ST, Wu Q, Quinn CJ, Anderson AK, Pak A, Chubykin AA. Visual experience-dependent oscillations and underlying circuit connectivity changes are impaired in Fmr1 KO mice. Cell Rep. 2020;31(1): 107486.
Trovato F, Parra R, Pracucci E, Landi S, Cozzolino O, Nardi G, et al. Modelling genetic mosaicism of neurodevelopmental disorders in vivo by a Cre-amplifying fluorescent reporter. Nat Commun. 2020;11(1):6194.
Atallah BV, Bruns W, Carandini M, Scanziani M. Parvalbumin-expressing interneurons linearly transform cortical responses to visual stimuli. Neuron. 2012;73(1):159–70.
Bortone DS, Olsen SR, Scanziani M. Translaminar inhibitory cells recruited by layer 6 corticothalamic neurons suppress visual cortex. Neuron. 2014;82(2):474–85.
Wöhr M, Orduz D, Gregory P, Moreno H, Khan U, Vörckel KJ, et al. Lack of parvalbumin in mice leads to behavioral deficits relevant to all human autism core symptoms and related neural morphofunctional abnormalities. Transl Psychiatry. 2015;5: e525.
Kataoka Y, Kalanithi PS, Grantz H, Schwartz ML, Saper C, Leckman JF, et al. Decreased number of parvalbumin and cholinergic interneurons in the striatum of individuals with Tourette syndrome. J Comp Neurol. 2010;518(3):277–91.
Rubenstein JL, Merzenich MM. Model of autism: increased ratio of excitation/inhibition in key neural systems. Genes Brain Behav. 2003;2(5):255–67.
Braat S, Kooy RF. The GABAA receptor as a therapeutic target for neurodevelopmental disorders. Neuron. 2015;86(5):1119–30.
Centonze D, Rossi S, Mercaldo V, Napoli I, Ciotti MT, De Chiara V, et al. Abnormal striatal GABA transmission in the mouse model for the fragile X syndrome. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63(10):963–73.
Chao HT, Chen H, Samaco RC, Xue M, Chahrour M, Yoo J, et al. Dysfunction in GABA signalling mediates autism-like stereotypies and Rett syndrome phenotypes. Nature. 2010;468(7321):263–9.
Belmonte MK, Bourgeron T. Fragile X syndrome and autism at the intersection of genetic and neural networks. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9(10):1221–5.
Moy SS, Nadler JJ. Advances in behavioral genetics: mouse models of autism. Mol Psychiatry. 2008;13(1):4–26.
Zoghbi HY. Postnatal neurodevelopmental disorders: meeting at the synapse? Science. 2003;302(5646):826–30.
Gatto CL, Broadie K. Genetic controls balancing excitatory and inhibitory synaptogenesis in neurodevelopmental disorder models. Front Synaptic Neurosci. 2010;2:4.
Han S, Tai C, Westenbroek RE, Yu FH, Cheah CS, Potter GB, et al. Autistic-like behaviour in Scn1a+/- mice and rescue by enhanced GABA-mediated neurotransmission. Nature. 2012;489(7416):385–90.
Lim CS, Kim H, Yu NK, Kang SJ, Kim T, Ko HG, et al. Enhancing inhibitory synaptic function reverses spatial memory deficits in Shank2 mutant mice. Neuropharmacology. 2017;112(Pt A):104–12.
Van der Molen MJ, Van der Molen MW, Ridderinkhof KR, Hamel BC, Curfs LM, Ramakers GJ. Auditory change detection in fragile X syndrome males: a brain potential study. Clin Neurophysiol. 2012;123(7):1309–18.
Wallace ML, van Woerden GM, Elgersma Y, Smith SL, Philpot BD. loss increases excitability and blunts orientation tuning in the visual cortex of Angelman syndrome model mice. J Neurophysiol. 2017;118(1):634–46.
LeBlanc JJ, DeGregorio G, Centofante E, Vogel-Farley VK, Barnes K, Kaufmann WE, et al. Visual evoked potentials detect cortical processing deficits in Rett syndrome. Ann Neurol. 2015;78(5):775–86.
Boggio EM, Pancrazi L, Gennaro M, Lo Rizzo C, Mari F, Meloni I, et al. Visual impairment in FOXG1-mutated individuals and mice. Neuroscience. 2016;324:496–508.
Sato Y, Mizuno H, Matsumoto N, Ikegaya Y. Subthreshold membrane potential dynamics of posterior parietal cortical neurons coupled with hippocampal ripples. Physiol Int. 2021;108:54–65.
Gibson JR, Bartley AF, Hays SA, Huber KM. Imbalance of neocortical excitation and inhibition and altered UP states reflect network hyperexcitability in the mouse model of fragile X syndrome. J Neurophysiol. 2008;100(5):2615–26.
Ikeda A, Terada K, Mikuni N, Burgess RC, Comair Y, Taki W, et al. Subdural recording of ictal DC shifts in neocortical seizures in humans. Epilepsia. 1996;37(7):662–74.
Voipio J, Tallgren P, Heinonen E, Vanhatalo S, Kaila K. Millivolt-scale DC shifts in the human scalp EEG: evidence for a nonneuronal generator. J Neurophysiol. 2003;89(4):2208–14.
Worrell G, Gotman J. High-frequency oscillations and other electrophysiological biomarkers of epilepsy: clinical studies. Biomark Med. 2011;5(5):557–66.
Mao W, Watanabe T, Cho S, Frost JL, Truong T, Zhao X, et al. Shank1 regulates excitatory synaptic transmission in mouse hippocampal parvalbumin-expressing inhibitory interneurons. Eur J Neurosci. 2015;41(8):1025–35.
Lee S, Lee E, Kim R, Kim J, Park H, Yang E, et al. Shank2 deletion in parvalbumin neurons leads to moderate hyperactivity, enhanced self-grooming and suppressed seizure susceptibility in mice. Front Mol Neurosci. 2018;11:209.
Peixoto RT, Wang W, Croney DM, Kozorovitskiy Y, Sabatini BL. Early hyperactivity and precocious maturation of corticostriatal circuits in Shank3B(-/-) mice. Nat Neurosci. 2016;19(5):716–24.
Gogolla N, Leblanc JJ, Quast KB, Südhof TC, Fagiolini M, Hensch TK. Common circuit defect of excitatory-inhibitory balance in mouse models of autism. J Neurodev Disord. 2009;1(2):172–81.
Yizhar O, Fenno LE, Prigge M, Schneider F, Davidson TJ, O’Shea DJ, et al. Neocortical excitation/inhibition balance in information processing and social dysfunction. Nature. 2011;477(7363):171–8.
Han S, Tai C, Jones CJ, Scheuer T, Catterall WA. Enhancement of inhibitory neurotransmission by GABAA receptors having α2,3-subunits ameliorates behavioral deficits in a mouse model of autism. Neuron. 2014;81(6):1282–9.
Knight EMP, Amin S, Bahi-Buisson N, Benke TA, Cross JH, Demarest ST, et al. Safety and efficacy of ganaxolone in patients with CDKL5 deficiency disorder: results from the double-blind phase of a randomised, placebo-controlled, phase 3 trial. Lancet Neurol. 2022;21(5):417–27.
Kazdoba TM, Hagerman RJ, Zolkowska D, Rogawski MA, Crawley JN. Evaluation of the neuroactive steroid ganaxolone on social and repetitive behaviors in the BTBR mouse model of autism. Psychopharmacology. 2016;233(2):309–23.
Biagini G, Panuccio G, Avoli M. Neurosteroids and epilepsy. Curr Opin Neurol. 2010;23(2):170–6.
Reddy DS, Rogawski MA. Ganaxolone suppression of behavioral and electrographic seizures in the mouse amygdala kindling model. Epilepsy Res. 2010;89(2–3):254–60.
Wang X, Bey AL, Katz BM, Badea A, Kim N, David LK, et al. Altered mGluR5-Homer scaffolds and corticostriatal connectivity in a Shank3 complete knockout model of autism. Nat Commun. 2016;7:11459.
Funding
This work was supported by the Comitato Telethon Fondazione Onlus grant no. GGP16131 to CV and no. GGP1928 to GMR, to Regione Lombardia NeOn Progetto "NeOn" POR-FESR 2014-2020, ", ID 239047, CUP E47F17000000009 to CS and CV, to Fondation Lejeune project #1938 to CV and to Aisphem to CV.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
JP, SL, AS, GN, MA, HB, MS and EP performed all the experiments; JP, SL, GN, EP and MS prepared the figures; GMR and SL designed and supervised the electrophysiology experiments; GMR, GN and EP developed and implemented the software for the analysis of the electrophysiological data; CV supervised and decided all the experiments; CV, CS, GMR, TB, JP, SL, EP and GN were involved in drafting the work or revising it critically for important intellectual content, all authors contributed to edit the text and figures. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethical approval and consent to participate
All animal care and experimental procedures were performed in accordance with protocols approved by the directives of the Italian Ministry of Health (approval number 1026/2020-PR).
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing financial interest.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Additional file 1
. Supplementary materials. Suppl. Figure 1: Analysis of up states in Shank3 KO versus Wt mice. Suppl. Figure 2: Disrupted gain control in Shank3 KO mice: a further analysis. Suppl. Figure 3: Spectral analysis in Shank3 KO mice superfused with midazolam. Suppl. Figure 4: Shank3 full deletion specifically in inhibitory PV neurons does not exacerbate the phenotype. Suppl. Figure 5: Decrease of inhibitory interneurons in Pv-Cre+/- TdTomatoFl/- Shank3Fl/Wt mice.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
About this article
Cite this article
Pagano, J., Landi, S., Stefanoni, A. et al. Shank3 deletion in PV neurons is associated with abnormal behaviors and neuronal functions that are rescued by increasing GABAergic signaling. Molecular Autism 14, 28 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-023-00557-2
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-023-00557-2