Assessment of Factors Impacting the Perception of Online Content Trustworthiness by Age, Education and Gender
<p>Percentage of each social networking sites’ users who get news on the site in 2013, 2016, and 2018 (data from [<a href="#B31-societies-12-00061" class="html-bibr">31</a>,<a href="#B32-societies-12-00061" class="html-bibr">32</a>,<a href="#B33-societies-12-00061" class="html-bibr">33</a>]).</p> "> Figure 2
<p>Showing that males place more weight (at combined “great deal” and “a lot” levels) on the publisher than do females.</p> "> Figure 3
<p>Showing that females place more weight (particularly at the combined “great deal” and “a lot” levels) on publication date than do males.</p> "> Figure 4
<p>Showing that the ideal level of focus on publication date declines with age.</p> "> Figure 5
<p>Showing that the weight placed on author’s political alignment increases with education level.</p> "> Figure 6
<p>Showing that the weight placed on publisher’s political alignment increases with education level.</p> "> Figure 7
<p>Showing that the ideal weight that is placed on author’s political alignment increases with education level.</p> "> Figure 8
<p>Showing greater interest article virality by females (self-perception).</p> "> Figure 9
<p>Showing greater ideal interest in article virality by females.</p> "> Figure 10
<p>Showing the level of focus given by respondents to articles’ authors.</p> "> Figure 11
<p>Showing the level of focus that should, ideally, be given to articles’ authors.</p> "> Figure 12
<p>Showing the level of focus given by respondents to articles’ publishers’ political alignment.</p> "> Figure 13
<p>Showing the level of focus that should, ideally, be given to articles’ publishers’ political alignment.</p> "> Figure A1
<p>Impact of article title on respondents’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A2
<p>Impact of article title on others’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A3
<p>Ideal impact of article title on peoples’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A4
<p>Impact of publisher on respondents’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A5
<p>Impact of publisher on others’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A6
<p>Ideal impact of publisher on peoples’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A7
<p>Impact of publication date on respondents’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A8
<p>Impact of publication date on others’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A9
<p>Ideal impact of publication date on peoples’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A10
<p>Impact of article author on respondents’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A11
<p>Impact of article author on others’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A12
<p>Ideal impact of article author on peoples’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A13
<p>Impact of article sponsors on respondents’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A14
<p>Impact of article sponsors on others’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A15
<p>Ideal impact of article sponsors on peoples’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A16
<p>Impact of author’s political alignment on respondents’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A17
<p>Impact of author’s political alignment on others’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A18
<p>Ideal impact of author’s political alignment on peoples’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A19
<p>Impact of publisher’s political alignment on respondents’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A20
<p>Impact of publisher’s political alignment on others’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A21
<p>Ideal impact of publisher’s political alignment on peoples’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A22
<p>Impact of sponsor’s political alignment on respondents’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A23
<p>Impact of sponsor’s political alignment on others’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A24
<p>Ideal impact of sponsor’s political alignment on peoples’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A25
<p>Impact of opinion statement quantity on respondents’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A26
<p>Impact of opinion statement quantity on others’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A27
<p>Ideal impact of opinion statement quantity on peoples’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A28
<p>Impact of article virality on respondents’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A29
<p>Impact of article virality on others’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A30
<p>Ideal impact of article virality on peoples’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A31
<p>Impact of article controversy level on respondents’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A32
<p>Impact of article controversy level on others’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A33
<p>Ideal impact of article controversy level on peoples’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A34
<p>Impact of article reading level on respondents’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A35
<p>Impact of article reading level on others’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> "> Figure A36
<p>Ideal impact of article reading level on peoples’ perspective of article trustworthiness and credibility by: (<b>a</b>) age (top), (<b>b</b>) educational level (middle) and (<b>c</b>) gender (bottom).</p> ">
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Background
2.1. Defining Fake News from 1475 to 2022
2.2. Fake News and Deceptive Online Content
2.3. Identification and Classification of Fake News
2.4. The Fake News Problem
3. Survey, Data Collection Process, Respondents and Methodology
3.1. Survey Instrument
- How much of an impact does the title of an article have on your personal perceptions of trustworthiness and or credibility of an article?
- How much of an impact do you believe the title of an article has on other people’s perception of the trustworthiness and or credibility of an article?
- If you were acting in an ideal manner, to what extent should the title of an article impact your perception of the trustworthiness and or credibility of an article?
● A great deal | ● A lot | ● A moderate amount | ● A little | ● None at all |
3.2. Data Collection
3.3. Respondent Demographics
3.4. Methodology
4. Analysis of the Impact of Article Title, Author, Publisher and Metadata on Perceptions of Content Trustworthiness
4.1. Article Title
4.2. Article Publisher
4.3. Publication Date
4.4. Article Author
4.5. Article Sponsors
4.6. Author’s Political Alignment
4.7. Publisher’s Political Alignment
4.8. Sponsor’s Political Alginment
5. Analysis of the Impact of Article Characteristics on Perceptions of Content Trustworthiness
5.1. Opinion Statement Quantity
5.2. Virality
5.3. Controversy Level
5.4. Reading Level
6. Implications of Analysis
7. Conclusions and Future Work
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
References
- Keyes, R. The Post-Truth Era: Dishonesty and Deception in Contemporary Life; St. Martin’s Press: New York, NY, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Dorling, D. Brexit: The decision of a divided country. BMJ 2016, 354, i3697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bastos, M.T.; Mercea, D. The Brexit Botnet and User-Generated Hyperpartisan News. Soc. Sci. Comput. Rev. 2017, 37, 38–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- McGaughey, E. Could Brexit be Void? King’s Law J. 2018, 29, 331–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Allcott, H.; Gentzkow, M. Social Media and Fake News in the 2016 Election. J. Econ. Perspect. 2017, 31, 211–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Grinberg, N.; Joseph, K.; Friedland, L.; Swire-Thompson, B.; Lazer, D. Fake news on Twitter during the 2016 U.S. presidential election. Science 2019, 363, 374–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bovet, A.; Makse, H.A. Influence of fake news in Twitter during the 2016 US presidential election. Nat. Commun. 2019, 10, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cunha, E.; Magno, G.; Caetano, J.; Teixeira, D.; Almeida, V. Fake News as We Feel It: Perception and Conceptualization of the Term “Fake News” in the Media. Int. Conf. Soc. Inform. 2018, 11185, 151–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Egelhofer, J.L.; Aaldering, L.; Eberl, J.M.; Galyga, S.; Lecheler, S. From Novelty to Normalization? How Journalists Use the Term “Fake News” in their Reporting. J. Stud. 2020, 21, 1323–1343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van der Linden, S.; Panagopoulos, C.; Roozenbeek, J. You are fake news: Political bias in perceptions of fake news. Media Cult. Soc. 2020, 42, 460–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tong, C.; Gill, H.; Li, J.; Valenzuela, S.; Rojas, H. “Fake News Is Anything They Say!”—Conceptualization and Weaponization of Fake News among the American Public. Mass Commun. Soc. 2020, 23, 755–778. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lee, T. The global rise of “fake news” and the threat to democratic elections in the USA. Public Adm. Policy 2019, 22, 15–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Silva, R.M.; Santos, R.L.S.; Almeida, T.A.; Pardo, T.A.S. Towards automatically filtering fake news in Portuguese. Expert Syst. Appl. 2020, 146, 113199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Collins, B.; Hoang, D.T.; Nguyen, N.T.; Hwang, D. Trends in combating fake news on social media—A survey. J. Inf. Telecommun. 2020, 5, 247–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Griffiths, J. The Great Firewall of China: How to Build and Control an Alternative Version of the Internet, 2nd ed.; Zed Books: London, UK, 2021. [Google Scholar]
- Spradling, M.; Straub, J.; Strong, J. Protection from ‘Fake News’: The Need for Descriptive Factual Labeling for Online Content. Futur. Internet 2021, 13, 142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lazer, D.M.J.; Baum, M.A.; Benkler, Y.; Berinsky, A.J.; Greenhill, K.M.; Menczer, F.; Metzger, M.J.; Nyhan, B.; Pennycook, G.; Rothschild, D.; et al. The science of fake news. Science 2018, 359, 1094–1096. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fuhr, N.; Giachanou, A.; Grefenstette, G.; Gurevych, I.; Hanselowski, A.; Jarvelin, K.; Jones, R.; Liu, Y.; Mothe, J.; Nejdl, W.; et al. An Information Nutritional Label for Online Documents. ACM SIGIR Forum 2018, 51, 46–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Higdon, N. Anatomy of Fake News; University of California Press: Berkeley, CA, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Ott, B. Some Good News about the News: 5 Reasons Why ‘Fake’ News is Better than Fox ‘News’. Flow 2005, 2, 7. [Google Scholar]
- Balmas, M. When Fake News Becomes Real: Combined Exposure to Multiple News Sources and Political Attitudes of Inefficacy, Alienation, and Cynicism. Communic. Res. 2014, 41, 430–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, L.S.; Dierberg, J. Late-Night Comedy as a Source of Religion News. In The Oxford Handbook of Religion and the American News Media; Oxford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Marimon, N. Shutting Down the Turbine: How the News Industry and News Aggregators can Coexist in a Post-Barclays v. theflyonthewall.com World. Fordham Intellect. Prop. Media Entertain. Law J. 2012, 23, 1441. [Google Scholar]
- Jones, L. All caught up in the kayfabe: Understanding and appreciating pro-wrestling. J. Philos. Sport 2019, 46, 276–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Saez-Trumper, D. Fake Tweet Buster: A Webtool to Identify Users Promoting Fake News on Twitter. In Proceedings of the HT’14, Santiago, Chile, 1–4 September 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Conroy, N.J.; Rubin, V.L.; Chen, Y. Automatic Deception Detection: Methods for Finding Fake News. In Proceedings of the ASIST, St. Louis, MO, USA, 6–10 October 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Khaldarova, I.; Pantti, M. Fake News: The narrative battle over the Ukrainian conflict. J. Pract. 2016, 10, 891–901. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Peters, J.W. Wielding Claims of ‘Fake News,’ Conservatives Take Aim at Mainstream Media; New York Times: New York, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Momsen, K.; Ohndorf, M. Information avoidance, selective exposure, and fake (?) news: Theory and experimental evidence on green consumption. J. Econ. Psychol. 2022, 88, 102457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tandoc, E.C.; Lim, W.; Ling, R. Defining “Fake News” A typology of scholarly definitions. Digit. J. 2017, 6, 137–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holcomb, J.; Gottfried, J.; Mitchell, A. News Use Across Social Media Platforms; Pew Research Center: Washington, DC, USA, 2013; Available online: https://www.pewresearch.org/journalism/2013/11/14/news-use-across-social-media-platforms/ (accessed on 23 March 2022).
- Gottfried, J.; Shearer, E. News Use Across Social Media Platforms 2016; Pew Research Center: Washington, DC, USA, 2016; Available online: https://www.pewresearch.org/journalism/2016/05/26/news-use-across-social-media-platforms-2016/ (accessed on 23 March 2022).
- Shearer, E.; Matsa, K.E. News Use Across Social Media Platforms 2018; Pew Research Center: Washington, DC, USA, 2018; Available online: https://www.pewresearch.org/journalism/2018/09/10/news-use-across-social-media-platforms-2018/ (accessed on 23 March 2022).
- Shearer, E.; Mitchell, A. News Use Across Social Media Platforms in 2020; Pew Research Center: Washington, DC, USA, 2020; Available online: https://www.pewresearch.org/journalism/2021/01/12/news-use-across-social-media-platforms-in-2020/ (accessed on 23 March 2022).
- Zhang, X.; Ghorbani, A.A. An overview of online fake news: Characterization, detection, and discussion. Inf. Process. Manag. 2020, 57, 102025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhao, J.; Cao, N.; Wen, Z.; Song, Y.; Lin, Y.R.; Collins, C. #FluxFlow: Visual analysis of anomalous information spreading on social media. IEEE Trans. Vis. Comput. Graph. 2014, 20, 1773–1782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davis, C.A.; Varol, O.; Ferrara, E.; Flammini, A.; Menczer, F. BotOrNot: A System to Evaluate Social Bots. Available online: https://dl.acm.org/doi/abs/10.1145/2872518.2889302 (accessed on 29 January 2022).
- Shu, K.; Sliva, A.; Wang, S.; Tang, J.; Liu, H. Fake News Detection on Social Media: A Data Mining Perspective. ACM SIGKDD Explor. Newsl. 2017, 19, 22–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Monsees, L. ‘A war against truth’—Understanding the fake news controversy. Crit. Stud. Secur. 2020, 8, 116–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Albright, J. Welcome to the Era of Fake News. Media Commun. 2017, 5, 87–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marchi, R. With Facebook, Blogs, and Fake News, Teens Reject Journalistic “Objectivity”. J. Commun. Inq. 2012, 36, 246–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gillin, J. How Pizzagate Went from Fake News to a Real Problem for a D.C. Business. PolitiFact. 2016. Available online: https://www.politifact.com/article/2016/dec/05/how-pizzagate-went-fake-news-real-problem-dc-busin/ (accessed on 23 March 2022).
- Tandoc, E.C. The facts of fake news: A research review. Sociol. Compass 2019, 13, e12724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Loos, E.; Ivan, L.; Leu, D. “Save the Pacific Northwest tree octopus”: A hoax revisited. Or: How vulnerable are school children to fake news? Inf. Learn. Sci. 2018, 119, 514–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fatilua, J. Who trusts social media? Comput. Human Behav. 2018, 81, 303–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wineburg, S.; McGrew, S. Why Students Can’t Google Their Way to the Truth. Available online: https://www.edweek.org/teaching-learning/opinion-why-students-cant-google-their-way-to-the-truth/2016/11 (accessed on 23 March 2022).
- Wineburg, S.; McGrew, S.; Breakstone, J.; Ortega, T. Evaluating Information: The Cornerstone of Civic Online Reasoning; Stanford Digital Repository: Stanford, CA, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Loos, E.; Nijenhuis, J. Consuming Fake News: A Matter of Age? The Perception of Political Fake News Stories in Facebook Ads. Int. Conf. Hum. Comput. Interact. 2020, 12209, 69–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Allen, J.; Howland, B.; Mobius, M.; Rothschild, D.; Watts, D.J. Evaluating the fake news problem at the scale of the information ecosystem. Sci. Adv. 2020, 6, eaay3539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Richardson, N. Fake News and Journalism Education. Asia Pac. Media Educ. 2017, 27, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lees, C. Fake news: The global silencer. Index Censorsh. 2018, 47, 88–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, F.A.; Trivax, G.; Zuehlke, D.A.; Lowinger, P.; Nghiem, T.L. Health Information during a Week of Television. N. Engl. J. Med. 1972, 286, 516–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benkler, Y.; Faris, R.; Roberts, H. Network Propaganda: Manipulation, Disinformation, and Radicalization in American Politics; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Faris, R.; Clark, J.; Etling, B.; Kaiser, J.; Roberts, H.; Schmitt, C.; Tilton, C.; Benkler, Y. Polarization and the Pandemic: American Political Discourse, March–May 2020; Berkman Klein Center Research Publication: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marshall, S. Five News Sites Can Now Embed Public Facebook Posts. Available online: https://www.journalism.co.uk/news/five-news-sites-can-now-embed-public-facebook-posts/s2/a553700/ (accessed on 27 February 2022).
- Zucker, H.A. Tackling Online Misinformation: A Critical Component of Effective Public Health Response in the 21st Century. Am. J. Public Health 2020, 110, S269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dentith, M.R.X. The Problem of Fake News. Public Reason 2016, 8, 65–79. [Google Scholar]
- Greg, S. Fake News: As the Problem or a Symptom of a Deeper Problem? Соціокомунікативне Середовище Теорія та Історія 2017, 4, 44. [Google Scholar]
- Savino, E.M. Fake News: No One Is Liable, and That Is a Problem. Buffalo Law Rev. 2017, 65, 1101. [Google Scholar]
- Guo, L. China’s “Fake News” Problem: Exploring the Spread of Online Rumors in the Government-Controlled News Media. Digit. J. 2020, 8, 992–1010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bernal, P. Facebook: Why Facebook Makes the Fake News Problem Inevitable. North. Irel. Leg. Q. 2018, 69, 513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Suttle, R.; Hogan, S.; Aumaugher, R.; Spradling, M.; Merrigan, Z.; Straub, J. University Community Members’ Perceptions of Labels for Online Media. Future Internet 2021, 13, 281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Straub, J.; Spradling, M. Americans’ Perspectives on Online Media Warning Labels. Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
18–24 | 25–29 | 30–34 | 35–39 | 40–44 | 45–49 | 50–54 | 55–59 | 60–64 | 65 and Older |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
10.57% | 10.93% | 11.29% | 10.04% | 8.96% | 6.63% | 6.09% | 12.54% | 12.19% | 10.75% |
59 | 61 | 63 | 56 | 50 | 37 | 34 | 70 | 68 | 60 |
Some High School (No Degree) | High School Degree | Some College (No Degree) | Associate’s Degree | Bachelor’s Degree | Master’s Degree | Doctoral Degree |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
4.68% | 25.72% | 23.20% | 11.51% | 22.12% | 10.25% | 2.52% |
26 | 143 | 129 | 64 | 123 | 57 | 14 |
Self-Usage | Others’ Usage | Ideal Usage | Belief Paradigm |
---|---|---|---|
Low | Low | Low | Metric should not be used, and it is not |
Low | High | Low | Metric should not be used, but others do |
Low | Low | High | Metric should be used, but no one does |
Low | High | High | Metric should be used, but I do not |
High | Low | Low | Metric should not be used, but I do |
High | High | Low | Metric should not be used, but we all do |
High | Low | High | Metric should be used, but others do not |
High | High | High | Metric should be used, and we all do |
Age | Education | Gender | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Title | Self | Declines from 18–24 to 35–39; resurgence at 40–44 | Negative correlation | Minimal variation |
Others | Negative correlation 18–24 to 40–49 | Increasing importance from some high school to some college, declines after this point | Minimal variation | |
Ideal | Increased weight from 18–24 to 30–34, decline afterwards | No clear pattern | Minimal difference | |
Publisher | Self | Declines with age in younger age groups | Increases with education level at lower education levels | Males place more weight on publisher than females |
Others | No obvious trend | Increases with education level at lower education levels | Males indicate that others place more weight on publisher than females | |
Ideal | Declines with age in younger age groups | Increases with education level at combined “great deal” and “a lot” levels | Same at “great deal” and “a lot” levels; females place more weight at “moderate” level | |
Publication Date | Self | Declines with age at younger age groups; increases with age in oldest groups | Increases with education | Females place more weight on publication date |
Others | No clear overall pattern | Increases with education at lower education levels, then decreases at higher ones | Females place more weight on publication date | |
Ideal | Declines with age across most age groups | Increases with education at lower education levels, then decreases at higher ones | Females place more weight on publication date | |
Author | Self | No clear trend | Increased weight with education level | No major difference |
Others | No clear trend | Increased weight with education level | No major difference | |
Ideal | Conflicting trends by level | Rises with education level at lower levels and then falls with increased education level at higher levels | No major difference | |
Article Sponsors | Self | No clear trend | Increased weight with education level | No major difference |
Others | No clear trend | Increased weight with education level | No major difference | |
Ideal | No clear trend | Increased weight with education level | No major difference | |
Author’s Political Alignment | Self | Increased weight with age, with decline between 40-44 and 55-59 age groups | Increased weight with education level | Males give more weight to this than females |
Others | Increases with age - two trends with reversal between them | Increased weight with education level | Males give more weight to this than females at “great deal” and “a lot” levels | |
Ideal | Increases with age—two trends with reversal between them | Increased weight with education level | Males give more weight to this than females at “great deal” and “a lot” levels | |
Publisher’s Political Alignment | Self | Two growth in focus with increased age trends present | Increased weight with education level | Males give more weight to this than females at “great deal” and “a lot” levels |
Others | Growth with higher age in older respondents only | Increased weight with education level | Males give more weight to this than females at “great deal” and “a lot” levels | |
Ideal | No clear trend | Increased weight with education level | Males give more weight to this than females at “great deal” level | |
Article Sponsors’ Political Alignment | Self | No clear trend | Increased weight with education level | Males give more weight to this than females |
Others | No clear trend | Increased weight with education level | Males give more weight to this than females | |
Ideal | No clear trend | Increased weight with education level | Males give more weight to this than females at “great deal” and “a lot” levels |
Age | Education | Gender | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Quantity of Opinion Statements | Self | No clear trend | Small correlation between higher education level and weight | Higher female interest |
Others | Slight decrease in interest with increased age | No notable trend | No notable trend | |
Ideal | No clear trend | No notable trend | Slightly higher female interest | |
Virality | Self | No clear trend | Negative correlation between focus and educational level | Notably higher female interest |
Others | Slight decrease in focus with increased age | Two positive correlation trends with gap | Moderately higher female interest | |
Ideal | No clear trend | Negative correlation between focus and educational level | Notably higher female interest | |
Controversy Level | Self | No clear trend | No clear trend | Slightly higher female interest |
Others | Range with negative correlation | Range with positive correlation | No notable difference | |
Ideal | Two ranges with positive correlation | No clear trend | Higher female interest | |
Reading Level | Self | No clear trend | Two ranges with positive correlation | No notable difference |
Others | No clear trend | Range with negative correlation | No notable difference | |
Ideal | No clear trend | Range with positive correlation | No notable difference |
Age | Education | Gender | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Title | Self-Others | Others give more weight to title | Others place more focus than they do, at most levels | 25% of both genders say others place more weight on than them |
Self-Ideal | At many levels, more indicated self-belief than importance | No clear pattern | No notable difference | |
Others-Ideal | Others less across ages | Others less at all levels except Ph.D. (same) | Others less for both genders, females have slightly higher comparative others importance at “great deal” level | |
Publisher | Self-Others | Similar | Similar | Similar; less females reporting “great deal” |
Self-Ideal | More focus placed on publisher than ideal in 7 of 10 age groups | No clear trend | Similar; males have greater focus at “a lot” level | |
Others-Ideal | Others place more focus on publisher than ideal in 7 of 10 age groups | Others place more focus than ideal in 5 of 7 categories | Males perceive others having more focus than ideal; females perceive others having similar to ideal focus level | |
Publication Date | Self-Others | No clear pattern | Mostly similar | Greater importance to others reported among both genders; females similarly place greater importance |
Self-Ideal | More ideal focus than reported self-focus at most levels | No clear pattern | More ideal focus than self-focus for both genders at “great deal”, “a lot” and “moderate” levels | |
Others-Ideal | More ideal focus than reported others focus at all but one level | More ideal focus than reported others focus at all but one level | More ideal focus than reported others focus at “great deal”, “a lot” and “moderate” levels | |
Author | Self-Others | Seven of ten have higher self than others at combined “great deal” and “a lot” levels | All but Ph.D. level report higher for self than others | Similar for males; lower level of females reporting importance for others at combined “great deal” and “a lot” levels |
Self-Ideal | Six of ten report higher self than ideal focus level | No clear trend | Lower ideal than self for both genders | |
Others-Ideal | Six of ten report lower others than ideal focus | No clear trend | No clear trend for males; females report others less than ideal at “great deal” and “a lot” levels | |
Article Sponsors | Self-Others | Younger age groups report less focus than others; older age groups report more | No clear trend | Males have notably more “great deal” respondents for self than others; females have marginally more. |
Self-Ideal | Nine out of 10 report more focus than ideal | No clear trend | Males and females report more focus than ideal at “great deal” and “a lot” levels | |
Others-Ideal | Seven of 10 age groups report others have more focus than ideal | All but one education level, respondents report others have more than ideal focus | Both males and females say others have less “great deal” interest than ideal and more “a lot” interest than ideal | |
Author’s Political Alignment | Self-Others | Eight of 10 ages indicate greater others focus than self-focus | Five of 7 education levels report greater others focus than self-focus | Both males and females report greater others’ focus than self-focus |
Self-Ideal | Nine of 10 ages report more self-focus than ideal focus. | Six of 7 report more self-focus than ideal focus | Males and females report more self-focus than ideal focus; more significant difference for males | |
Others-Ideal | More others’ focus than ideal at all age levels | Six of 7 education levels report more others’ focus than ideal focus | Males and females report more others’ focus than ideal focus; more significant difference for males | |
Publisher’s Political Alignment | Self-Others | Six of ten report higher self than others’ focus level | No clear trend | No clear trend; females see more focus for others than self-focus |
Self-Ideal | More focus than ideal at all levels | Five of seven education levels have more self-focus than ideal | Both males and females report more self-focus than ideal | |
Others-Ideal | More focus by others than ideal at all levels | Five of seven education levels have more others’ focus than ideal | Both males and females report more others’ focus than ideal | |
Article Sponsors’ Political Alignment | Self-Others | Seven of 10 report more self than others’ focus | Similar - no clear trend | Similar - no clear trend |
Self-Ideal | Seven of 10 report more self than ideal focus | All education levels report more self-focus than ideal | Males report more self-focus than ideal; females report close to the same level | |
Others-Ideal | Seven of 10 report more others’ than ideal focus | All education levels report more others’ focus than ideal | Both males and females report more others’ focus than ideal; more pronounced difference for males |
Age | Education | Gender | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Quantity of Opinion Statements | Self-Others | Six of ten report lower self than others’ focus level | No notable trend | No notable trend |
Self-Ideal | Seven of ten report higher self than ideal focus level | Five of seven report higher self than ideal focus level | Both genders have higher self-focus than ideal focus | |
Others-Ideal | Eight of ten report higher other’s than ideal focus level | Six of seven report higher others’ than ideal focus level | Both genders have higher other’s focus than ideal; more pronounced difference for males. | |
Virality | Self-Others | Notably uncorrelated; eight of 10 age groups have lower self-focus than others’ focus | Notable difference at some high school level; six of seven have higher others’ interest than self-focus | Notably higher others’ interest than self-interest for both genders; larger difference for males |
Self-Ideal | Significant variations; Eight of ten groups have greater self-focus than ideal focus | All seven education levels have less ideal focus than self-focus | Both have higher self-interest than ideal interest; similar difference levels | |
Others-Ideal | Nine of ten report higher other’s than ideal focus level | Six of seven educational levels report higher other’s than ideal focus level | Both genders have higher other’s focus than ideal focus | |
Controversy Level | Self-Others | All 10 have higher others’ interest than self interest | Six of seven groups have higher others’ interest than self interest | Both genders have higher others’ interest than self-interest; more pronounced difference for males |
Self-Ideal | No clear trend | No clear trend | Slightly more self-interest than ideal interest | |
Others-Ideal | All 10 have higher others’ interest than ideal interest | Six of seven groups have higher others’ interest than ideal interest | Both genders have higher others’ interest than ideal interest; more pronounced difference for males | |
Reading Level | Self-Others | Seven of ten report higher self than others’ focus level | No clear trend | Males similar; females have greater self than others’ interest |
Self-Ideal | Nine of ten have more self-interest than ideal interest | No clear trend | Both genders have higher self-interest than ideal interest; larger difference for females | |
Others-Ideal | No clear trend | No clear trend | Both genders have higher others’ interest than ideal interest |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Straub, J.; Spradling, M.; Fedor, B. Assessment of Factors Impacting the Perception of Online Content Trustworthiness by Age, Education and Gender. Societies 2022, 12, 61. https://doi.org/10.3390/soc12020061
Straub J, Spradling M, Fedor B. Assessment of Factors Impacting the Perception of Online Content Trustworthiness by Age, Education and Gender. Societies. 2022; 12(2):61. https://doi.org/10.3390/soc12020061
Chicago/Turabian StyleStraub, Jeremy, Matthew Spradling, and Bob Fedor. 2022. "Assessment of Factors Impacting the Perception of Online Content Trustworthiness by Age, Education and Gender" Societies 12, no. 2: 61. https://doi.org/10.3390/soc12020061
APA StyleStraub, J., Spradling, M., & Fedor, B. (2022). Assessment of Factors Impacting the Perception of Online Content Trustworthiness by Age, Education and Gender. Societies, 12(2), 61. https://doi.org/10.3390/soc12020061