Abstract
This article describes a Windows program that enables users to obtain a broad range of statistics concerning the properties of word and nonword stimuli in Spanish, including word frequency, syllable frequency, bigram and biphone frequency, orthographic similarity, orthographic and phonological structure, concreteness, familiarity, imageability, valence, arousal, and age-of-acquisition measures. It is designed for use by researchers in psycholinguistics, particularly those concerned with recognition of isolated words. The program computes measures of orthographic similarity online, with respect to either a default vocabulary of 31,491 Spanish words or a vocabulary specified by the user. In addition to providing standard orthographic and phonological neighborhood measures, the program can be used to obtain information about other forms of orthographic similarity, such as transposed-letter similarity and embedded-word similarity. It is available, free of charge, from the following Web site: www.maccs.mq.edu.au/~colin/B-Pal.
Article PDF
Similar content being viewed by others
Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.
References
Alameda, J. R., &Cuetos, F. (1995).Diccionario de frecuencia de las unidades lingüísticas del castellano. Oviedo: Universidad de Oviedo.
Álvarez, C. J., Carreiras, M., &Perea, M. (2004). Are syllables phonological units in visual word recognition?Language & Cognitive Processes,19, 427–452.
Álvarez, C. J., Carreiras, M., &Taft, M. (2001). Syllables and morphemes: Contrasting frequency effects in Spanish.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,27, 545–555.
Andrews, S. (1996). Lexical retrieval and selection processes: Effects of transposed-letter confusability.Journal of Memory & Language,35, 775–800.
Andrews, S. (1997). The effect of orthographic similarity on lexical retrieval: Resolving neighborhood conflicts.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,4, 439–461.
Balota, D. A., Pilotti, M., &Cortese, M. J. (2001). Subjective frequency estimates for 2,938 monosyllabic words.Memory & Cognition,29, 639–647.
Bowers, J. S., Davis, C. J., &Hanley, D. A. (2005). Automatic semantic activation of embedded words: Is there a “hat” in “that”?Journal of Memory & Language,52, 131–143.
Carreiras, M., Alvarez, C. J., &de Vega, M. (1993). Syllable frequency and visual word recognition in Spanish.Journal of Memory & Language,32, 766–780.
Carreiras, M., &Perea, M. (2002). Masked priming effects with syllabic neighbors in the lexical decision task.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,28, 1228–1242.
Carreiras, M., &Perea, M. (2004). Naming pseudowords in Spanish: Effects of syllable frequency.Brain & Language,90, 393–400.
Chambers, S. M. (1979). Letter and order information in lexical access.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,18, 225–241.
Coltheart, M. (1981). The MRC Psycholinguistic Database,Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,33A, 497–505.
Coltheart, M., Davelaar, E., Jonasson, J. T., &Besner, D. (1977). Access to the internal lexicon. In S. Dornic (Ed.),Attention and performance VI (pp. 535–555). New York: Academic Press.
Cuetos, F., Ellis, A. W., &Álvarez, B. (1999). Naming times for the Snodgrass and Vanderwart pictures in Spanish.Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers,31, 650–658.
Davis, C. J. (2005). N-Watch: A program for deriving neighborhood size and other psycholinguistic statistics.Behavior Research Methods,37, 65–70.
Davis, C. J., &Taft, M. (2005). More words in the neighborhood: Interference in lexical decision due to deletion neighbors.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,12, 904–910.
Gernsbacher, M. A. (1984). Resolving 20 years of inconsistent interactions between lexical familiarity and orthography, concreteness, and polysemy.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,113, 256–281.
Grainger, J., O’Regan, J. K., Jacobs, A. M., &Segui, J. (1989). On the role of competing word units in visual word recognition: The neighborhood frequency effect.Perception & Psychophysics,45, 189–195.
Hermans, D., De Houwer, J., &Eelen, P. (2001). A time course analysis of the affective priming effect.Cognition & Emotion,15, 143–165.
James, C. T. (1975). The role of semantic information in lexical decisions.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,1, 130–136.
Juilland, A., &Chang-Rodríguez, E. (1964).Frequency dictionary of Spanish words. The Hague: Mouton.
Mathey, S., &Zagar, D. (2000). The neighborhood distribution effect in visual word recognition: Words with single and twin neighbors.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,26, 184–205.
New, B., Pallier, C., Brysbaert, M., &Ferrand, L. (2004).Lexique 2: A new French lexical database.Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers,36, 516–524.
Perea, M., &Carreiras, M. (1998). Effects of syllable frequency and syllable neighborhood frequency in visual word recognition.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,24, 134–144.
Perea, M., & Carreiras, M. (in press). Do transposed-letter similarity effects occur at a phonological level?Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology.
Perea, M., &Lupker, S. J. (2003). Doesjugde activate COURT? Transposed-letter similarity effects in masked associative priming.Memory & Cognition,31, 829–841.
Perea, M., &Lupker, S. J. (2004). Can CANISO activate CASINO? Transposed-letter similarity effects with nonadjacent letter positions.Journal of Memory & Language,51, 231–246.
Perea, M., &Rosa, E. (2000). The effects of orthographic neighborhood in reading and laboratory word identification tasks: A review.Psicológica,21, 327–340.
Pérez, M. A. (2004).Influencia del orden de adquisición del léxico en el reconocimiento de palabras. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Universidad de Murcia.
Pérez, M. A., Alameda, J. R., &Cuetos, F. (2003). Frecuencia, longitud y vecindad ortográfica de las palabras de 3 a 16 letras del Diccionario de la Lengua Española (RAE, 1992).Revista Electrónica de Metodología Aplicada,8, 1–10.
Pugh, K., Rexer, K., &Katz, L. (1994). Evidence of flexible coding in visual word recognition.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,20, 807–825.
Rastle, K., &Coltheart, M. (2000). Lexical and nonlexical print-to-sound translation of disyllabic words and nonwords.Journal of Memory & Language,42, 342–364.
Real Academia de la Lengua (1995).Diccionario de la lengua Española. Madrid: Espasa Calpe.
Redondo, J., Fraga, I., Comesaña, M., &Perea, M. (2005). Estudio normativo del valor afectivo de 478 palabras españolas.Psicológica,26, 317–326.
Santiago, J., Justicia, F., Palma, A., Huertas, D., &Gutiérrez, N. (1996). LEX I and II: Two databases of surface word forms for psycholinguistic research in Spanish.Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers,28, 418–426.
Schoonbaert, S., &Grainger, J. (2004). Letter position coding in printed word perception: Effects of repeated and transposed letters.Language & Cognitive Processes,19, 333–367.
Sebastián-Gallés, N., Martí, M. A., Cuetos, F., &Carreiras, M. (2000).LEXESP: Léxico informatizado del español. Barcelona: Edicions de la Universitat de Barcelona.
Taft, M., &van Graan, F. (1998). Lack of phonological mediation in a semantic categorization task.Journal of Memory & Language,38, 203–224.
van Heuven, W. J. B., &Dijkstra, T. (2005).Extended neighborhood effects in visual word recognition. Poster presented at the XIV meeting of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology, Leiden.
Whaley, C. P. (1978). Word-nonword classification time.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,17, 143–154.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding authors
Additional information
This work was partially supported by an Australian Research Council Post-Doctoral Grant to the first author. The authors thank Manuel Carreiras, Fernando Cuetos, Maria Antonia Martí, Jaime Redondo, and Núria Sebaştián-Gallés for their permission to include lexical information they had previously collected within this software package.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Davis, C.J., Perea, M. BuscaPalabras: A program for deriving orthographic and phonological neighborhood statistics and other psycholinguistic indices in Spanish. Behavior Research Methods 37, 665–671 (2005). https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192738
Received:
Accepted:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192738