Abstract
The magnitude of nocturnal melatonin suppression depends upon the spectrum, amount, and duration of light exposure. The functional relationship between melatonin suppression and the light spectrum and amount have been previously described. Only one duration-dependent parameter was needed to extend this functional relationship to predict nocturnal melatonin suppression during the early biological night from a variety of published studies. Those predictions suggest that ambient lighting commonly found in North American homes will not suppress melatonin for durations up to 3 h, whereas extended use of self-luminous displays in the home prior to sleep can.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
The circadian system is perhaps one of the most important non-visual systems affected by retinal light exposure. The retinohypothalamic tract (RHT) is the direct neural pathway from the retina to the master biological clock, the suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN) in the hypothalamus. It is now known that the spectral, temporal, spatial, and absolute sensitivity characteristics of the RHT neural channel stimulating the SCN are quite different from those exhibited by the optic nerve leading to visual functioning by the thalamus and visual cortex. This is true even though all retinal photoreceptors, including the intrinsically photosensitive retina ganglion cells (ipRGCs), participate in the various phototransduction processes for visual and non-visual systems1,2,3,4,5,6. To quantify light as a stimulus for the circadian system, it is necessary to develop a functional relationship between optical radiation incident on the retina and the spectral, temporal, and absolute responses of the SCN. Toward that end, classic psychophysical methods can be used7.
Defining light: the photopic luminous efficiency function, V(λ)
Psychophysics is a technique used to derive functional relationships between physical (Φ) stimuli and psychological (Ψ) responses (Fig. 1). Like physical quantities, these psychological responses are always in the form of measurable behavior, such as reaction times, subjective judgments, or nocturnal melatonin suppression.
Radiometry is the physical measurement of optical radiation. A wide variety of behavioral responses can be measured for the very same optical radiation incident on the retina in psychophysical experiments. For example, an exposure to monochromatic wavelength can evoke a reaction time, a subjective report of apparent brightness, a reported hue sensation, or suppression of melatonin synthesis. Each of those behavioral responses will have a different functional relationship to the monochromatic wavelength exposure.
The goal of one set of psychophysical experiments conducted in the early part of the twentieth century was to define light as a physical quantity for the emerging lighting industry by establishing a functional relationship between the radiant power of monochromatic wavelengths to the relative sensitivity of the human visual system to those wavelengths. Since sensitivity cannot be measured directly, the basic rationale for these experiments was to measure psychological judgements of equality for two monochromatic wavelengths and then piece together a relative sensitivity function from all wavelengths that had been judged as equal. Specifically, if any two monochromatic wavelengths were reported to be equal, it was assumed that both would have been stimulated by the same physical amount of light.
Two equality techniques were used. One was based upon direct comparisons of the perceived brightness of two wavelengths while the other was based upon the disappearance of flicker for two rapidly oscillating wavelengths. For both techniques the radiant power of a reference wavelength was fixed and visually compared to a test wavelength with variable radiant power. The radiant power of the test was varied until the two wavelengths were judged equal (i.e., equal apparent brightness or no apparent flicker). The relative amount of radiant power needed by the test wavelength to be perceived as equal to the radiant power of the reference wavelength was then measured. Both of these equality methods were applied for pairs of wavelengths across the spectrum. From these psychophysically determined equal wavelengths, the wavelength requiring the least amount of radiant power to be equal to the others was at 555 nm. This was the wavelength to which people were most sensitive and therefore the peak of the spectral sensitivity function that would define light for the Commission Internationale de l'Éclairage (CIE). Once the CIE adopted this, so-called photopic luminous efficiency function, V(λ), in 1924 to define light, a system of measuring optical radiation as a visual stimulus was possible. Thus, through psychophysics one aspect of the psychological domain (Ψ), a spectral sensitivity function, became a member of the physical (Φ) domain, the photopic luminous efficiency function (Fig. 2).
This 1924 system of photometry has been an extremely useful simplification for the lighting industry because the spectral characteristics of the (implicit) visual stimulus can be described in terms of a single quantity (e.g., luminance) without consideration of the spectral power distribution of the light source. Thus, intensity distributions (luminous intensity) and recommended light levels (illuminance) can all been defined in terms of a system of photometry based upon V(λ). Indeed, for many visual tasks that are processed by the fovea, like reading or on-axis detection, V(λ) is an excellent rectifying measure of the spectral characteristics of the visual stimulus8. In graphical terms, the psychophysically determined functional relationship can be shown as a plot of the physical quantity represented on the abscissa, in appropriate units (e.g., wavelength in nm), and the psychological response on the ordinate (e.g., reciprocal of the radiant watts seen as equal). For other behavioral responses like subjective brightness, however, V(λ) is not a suitable rectifying measure of the spectral characteristics of the visual stimulus (Fig. 3)9. Short wavelengths, discounted by V(λ), strongly affect brightness response.
It should be noted that specification of the spectral characteristics of the visual stimulus does not represent a full specification of the stimulus. Additional psychophysical experiments need to be performed to establish a functional relationship between a set of physically measured stimuli and a psychological response. For example, to predict on-axis detection, the solid angle of the visual target and its contrast as well as the absolute luminance of the target background must be known. All of these stimulus characteristics provide input into the relative visual performance (RVP) model, which has been validated for reading speed and for a variety of other on-axis responses10,11,12,13,14. Once the functional relationship between the physical stimulus and psychological response is established, however, the response metric (e.g., RVP) becomes part of the physical domain (Φ) even if the physical stimulus is complex.
Defining circadian light and circadian stimulus
Given the conventional measures of light based upon the photopic luminous efficiency function, V(λ), do not appropriately represent the circadian system’s response to different wavelengths, a new spectral sensitivity function was needed. Circadian light (CLA) was derived, in part, from binocular experiments15,16 using monochromatic light-induced nocturnal melatonin suppression as the outcome measure17,18. The pineal gland synthesizes melatonin at night and because there is only one major light-sensitive pathway from the SCN to the pineal gland19,20, nocturnal melatonin suppression is an ideal way to measure the otherwise unreachable response of the human SCN to different spectra and amounts of retinal light exposure.
Circadian light (CLA) was developed to be analogous to V(λ) but, as the name implies, to define light for the circadian system. Like V(λ), CLA was based upon a particular type of psychophysical experiment, specifically, light-dependent attenuation of melatonin synthesis by the pineal gland at night. Using a constant criterion methodology to establish equality of response, it was possible to quantify the relative radiant power needed to suppress nocturnal melatonin synthesis by monochromatic light sources15,16. Although these data provided an important starting point for the development of CLA, subsequent experiments with polychromatic sources showed that the response of the circadian system to broad-band spectra could not be predicted from the spectral sensitivity data obtained from monochromatic sources21,22,23. Specifically, under some conditions adding more light reduced the circadian system response; this is known as subadditivity. Consequently, a more complicated formulation of circadian light had to be developed to describe the spectral sensitivity of the circadian system to any spectral power distribution, monochromatic or polychromatic. This more complete formulation then, just like V(λ), moves CLA from the psychological domain (Ψ) to the physical domain (Φ).
As already discussed, defining circadian light is not a full specification of the stimulus for the circadian system. A more complete specification would also include the operating range of the circadian system from threshold to saturation in addition to its spectral sensitivity. The metric circadian stimulus (CS) was developed for this purpose. As with CLA, psychophysical experiments using nocturnal melatonin suppression informed the development of CS. The absolute sensitivity of the circadian system to luminous stimuli, quantified in terms of CLA, was functionally described by CS for a 1-h exposure duration24. Thus, CS moved from the psychological domain (Ψ) to the physical domain (Φ) for specifying luminous stimuli for the circadian system.
CS is not a complete specification of the luminous stimulus, however. The duration of exposure is also important for describing the circadian system’s response to optical radiation on the retina. The purpose of the present paper was to determine how the duration of exposure could be added to the CLA and CS formulations to predict nocturnal melatonin suppression during the early biological night. Specifically, melatonin suppression data for different binocular exposure durations recently reported by Nagare et al.25 were used to develop a more complete specification of the circadian stimulus.
Methods
Current CS model
According to the circadian phototransduction model (Eqs. 1 and 2) proposed by Rea et al.17,18,24, CLA represents the spectral sensitivity of the SCN to light and CS represents its absolute sensitivity. Although the CLA and CS formulations were based upon nocturnal melatonin suppression following a 1-h light exposure, the model is intended to represent the circadian system’s instantaneous response to light exposures. To accurately predict the amount of nocturnal melatonin suppression for light exposures other than 1 h, however, the duration of light exposure must also be known.
where,
CLA: Circadian light, \({E}_{\lambda }:\) light source spectral irradiance, CS: Circadian stimulus, \({Mc}_{\lambda }\): melanopsin sensitivity (corrected for crystalline lens transmittance), k = 0.2616, \({S}_{\lambda }:\) S-cone fundamental, \({a}_{b-y}=0.7\), \({mp}_{\lambda }\): macular pigment transmittance, \({a}_{rod}=3.3\), \({V}_{\lambda }:\) photopic luminous efficiency function, \(RodSat=6.5 W {m}^{-2}\), \({V{^{\prime}}}_{\lambda }:\) scotopic luminous efficiency function.
Test dataset
The data recently published by Nagare et al.25 (Table 1) were used to determine whether the CS formulation could be supplemented with an exposure duration term to predict nocturnal melatonin suppression in the early biological night. Nagare and colleagues measured light-induced nocturnal melatonin suppression in healthy adults and adolescents following binocular exposure to a wide range of light levels (40–1,000 lx), two white-light spectra (2,700 K and 6,500 K), and extended nighttime light exposure durations (0.5–3.0 h). (The α-opic irradiances (μW cm−2) for the lighting interventions, following the SI-compliant approach recommended by the CIE26 are provided in the original manuscript.) Statistical analysis showed that the main effect of participant age was not significant, nor were the two-way interactions between age and light level, spectrum or duration (p > 0.05), so the melatonin suppression values for the two age groups were averaged together for each combination of the four light levels, two spectra, and six durations.
Based upon the specific experimental setup as described in Nagare et al.25, each study participant either operated an electronic device (e.g., filtered laptop or smartphone) or read a physical book on the desk. Retinal light exposures for each participant were monitored using a Daysimeter (Model 12, Lighting Research Center, Troy, NY) mounted on a lensless eyeglasses frame throughout the 3-h light exposure. A simple exercise was conducted wherein photometric measurements were taken to address any systematic offset in effective corneal stimulus due to misalignment between the participant line of sight and the orientation of the Daysimeter sensor. The exercise revealed that the effective light level at the participants’ eye was reduced by an average of 23.4% (SD 2.5) and 16.8% (SD 3.2) while operating a smart phone or a laptop, respectively, and reduced by 24.3% (SD 3.7) when reading a book. Therefore, the CLA values in Table 1 were multiplied by a factor of 0.797 (aggregate), consequently reducing the effective CS levels for modeling purposes. Post hoc statistical analysis showed that the measured nocturnal melatonin suppression from Nagare et al.25 and effective CS did not differ significantly (p > 0.05; Supplementary Table S1).
Mathematical modeling
The statistical analyses reported in the original publication revealed that there was no significant interaction between light spectrum and duration (p > 0.05), suggesting that over the range of conditions employed by Nagare et al.25 the spectral sensitivity of the circadian system did not change. It should be true then that the form of the sigmoidal four-parameter logistic function in Eq. 2 would remain unchanged and the basic function simply would be shifted along the log CLA abscissa as a function of exposure duration. Guided by parsimony and the assumption that spectral sensitivity did not change over 3 h, it was assumed for modeling purposes that only the half-saturation constant in Eq. 2 (i.e., CLA = 355.7) would systematically change as a function of exposure duration. Thus, Eq. 2 was modified slightly (Eq. 3), whereby q was the only variable.
Results
Test dataset optimization
Best fitting logistic functions based upon Eq. 3 and developed using curve fitting software OriginPro 2020 (OriginLab Corporation, Northampton, MA) for the six different exposure durations from Nagare et al.25 are shown in Fig. 4. All the curves fit the melatonin suppression data significantly (p < 0.001) and the goodness of fit as assessed by the coefficient of determination (R2) was always greater than 0.90. The optimized half-saturation constant (q) values ranged from CLA = 168.3 for the 3.0-h test dataset to CLA = 758.0 for the 0.5-h test dataset.
The free parameter, q, in Eq. 3 can be considered as a light exposure (amount × duration) term, where
and t is the duration of light exposure, in hours, qm is the amount of measured light, in terms of CLA, producing half saturation and, to avoid any assumption of reciprocity between the amount of exposure and the duration of exposure, r is a free parameter. In the original model by Rea and colleagues, q = 355.7 for a 1-h exposure duration.
Subsequent modeling combining Eqs. 3 and 4 resulted in optimized values of qm = 411.3 and r = − 0.855 (Eq. 5). The left panel in Fig. 5 shows the functional relationship between exposure duration (in hours) and the optimized half-saturation values, q, from Fig. 4.
where,
CSt corresponds to the absolute response of the SCN as characterized by nocturnal melatonin suppression following a light exposure duration of t in hours.
The goal of the present study was to investigate whether the CS function can be simply supplemented by adding light exposure duration (t) as an independent stimulus parameter. Since there was no significant difference between the CS formulation based upon a 1-h exposure and the 1-h suppression data from Nagare, et al.25 (Table 1), the optimized function (CSt, Eq. 5) was simplified. In the original CS model (i.e., based on 1-h exposure), q = 355.7, so in the simplified Eq. 6, qm becomes a constant, qm = 355.7, and to obviate the exponent, r, entirely r = − 1.0. Thus,
Figure 5 illustrates the optimized half-saturation function, where qm = 411.3 and r = − 0.855, and the simplified half-saturation function, where qm = 355.7 and r = − 1.000. In the simplified CSt function (Eq. 6), CLA and t are the only unknowns.
Absolute predictions based upon the proposed CSt function (Eq. 6) are depicted in Fig. 6 for the six durations, from t = 0.5 h to t = 3.0 h.
Validation
To validate the simplified function, it was possible to compare the predicted CSt values with nocturnal melatonin suppression data from 11 published studies28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38 (Supplementary Tables S2 and S3).
Figure 7 shows the calculated melatonin suppression data from the selected studies along with a priori predictions from the simplified function (Eq. 6; see Fig. 6). All the curves fit the melatonin suppression data significantly (p < 0.001; Supplementary Table S4). Except for the 2.5-h exposure duration, the goodness of fits, R2, were greater than 0.80. For the 2.5-h dataset, there simply were not enough data to produce statistically reliable estimates of nocturnal melatonin suppression. Nevertheless, the CSt predictions for 2.5 h go through the center of the melatonin suppression data and, in addition, the fits for the flanking 2.0-h and 3.0-h data were well predicted by the simplified function. These two pieces of indirect supporting evidence suggest that the simplified function is reliable for predicting nocturnal melatonin for the 2.5-h duration. Figure 8 shows the relationship between CSt (now in the physical domain, Φ) and melatonin suppression for the data used in the validation exercise. Taken together, the validation exercise using independent data supports the utility of simplified function, CSt, for predicting melatonin suppression during the early biological night.
Discussion
In the present study we showed that the original CS formulation proposed by Rea et al.17,18,24 could be used to predict the amount of nocturnal melatonin suppression during the early biological night for different durations of exposure by adding just one additional duration-dependent parameter, t. These findings indirectly support the inference that CLA and CS are robust specifications of the instantaneous luminous stimulus for the circadian system in terms of both spectrum and amount. To predict the amount of nocturnal melatonin suppression, however, CLA and CS are only part of the stimulus specification. As is well known, and shown here, the duration of exposure must also be specified. The simplified CSt formulation was not only useful for predicting the data from Nagare et al.25 but provided excellent explanatory power for several other published studies. Thus, specification of the luminous stimulus for suppressing nocturnal melatonin during the early biological night can be described in terms of CSt which itself is defined in terms of the spectrum (CLA), the amount (CS), and the exposure duration (t) of the luminous stimulus.
The implications of the simplified function, CSt, are perhaps most relevant to light exposures in residences prior to sleeping. It is important that evening light not disrupt the circadian system, both in terms of delaying circadian phase and attenuating melatonin synthesis. In that regard, it had been suggested by Rea and Figueiro39 that most residential lighting would not produce sufficient light exposures (for typical spectra, amounts, and durations in residences) to significantly suppress melatonin synthesis. Specifically, Rea and Figueiro suggested, as a stated conservative threshold, that people at home in the evening should limit their light exposures to “white” light to 30 lx at the eyes for 30 min. The laboratory study by Nagare et al.25 provided a more precise estimate of exposure threshold, suggesting that light exposures to white light in residences should be limited to 50 lx at the eye for 2 h. These suggested exposure limits rely, first, on an assumption about the threshold for light-induced nocturnal melatonin suppression (≈ 10%) and, second, on empirical measurements and observations of lighting in residences in North America and Europe.
A nocturnal melatonin suppression exposure threshold of 10% was chosen for two reasons. First, the 10% value appears to be a good indicator of the “toe” of the logistic function relating log CLA to nocturnal melatonin suppression (e.g., Fig. 4) and, second, because the uncertainty is approximately 10% in melatonin measurements using radioimmune assay methods39.
Several studies have reported the amount of light, usually in photopic illuminance (lx), that were or might be incident on the corneas of occupants in their residents. A study by Burgess and Eastman40 reported a mean light exposure of 33.0 lx (SD 13.8) over 4 h prior to bedtime, as measured using pendant actiwatches. Scheuermaier et al.41 reported a mean light exposure of 34.8 lx (SD 24.1) prior to bedtime (19:00–00:00) for healthy young and older adults, measured using wrist worn actiwatches. In an extensive study involving 72 female school teachers, Rea, et al.42 reported mean evening residential vertical light levels of 28 lx, recorded using headband-Daysimeters27 between civil twilight and bedtime.
Warm incandescent, CFL, or LED sources of approximately 2,700 K dominate the residential lighting market43. According to the simplified function, CSt, a photopic illuminance of 34 lx from both the Burgess and Eastman and the Scheuermaier et al. studies from “warm” sources translates into CS1.0 = 0.04 for a 1-h exposure and CS3.0 = 0.10 for a 3-h exposure (Fig. 9). A photopic illuminance of 28 lx from the Rea et al. study translates into CS1.0 = 0.03 and CS3.0 = 0.09.
In modern homes, however, it is probably common today for occupants to experience prolonged light exposures from self-luminous displays. For instance, Gringras et al.44 reported that smartphones (iPhone 5S) operated from a typical reading distance (22.5 cm) can deliver a light level of 51 lx at the eye. For typical self-luminous spectra, this would translate into CS1.0 = 0.12 and CS3.0 = 0.29 (Fig. 9). Chang et al.45 recorded an average photopic light level of 32 lx at the eye (n = 12) from eReaders, which translates into CS1.0 = 0.06 and CS3.0 = 0.17 (Fig. 9). In a more extensive study of self-luminous tablets following up on the earlier work by Wood et al.46, Nagare et al.47 reported that iPads deliver around 70 lx at the eye for an average viewing distance of 30.5 cm; this translates into CS1.0 = 0.13 and CS3.0 = 0.30. Using the “Night-shift” setting for the same tablets, CS1.0 = 0.08 and CS3.0 = 0.21, respectively. In general, Fig. 9 shows that evening ambient light exposures in residences are typically below the proposed threshold of CSt = 0.10, even after 3 h. For self-luminous displays that might be used in the home, however, predicted CSt levels are well above the proposed threshold, even when using the “Night-shift” setting47.
Cautions associated with the simplified CSt function deserve mention. Although the present study supports the assumption that the spectral and absolute sensitivities of the SCN are well characterized by CLA and CS, respectively, it should not be assumed that the duration term, t, is applicable to melatonin suppression at every time of night. The dataset from Nagare et al.25 from which the simplified function was developed, were collected at clock times when control night melatonin levels were increasing. Thus, until further research is completed, the simplified function should only be applicable for predicting nocturnal melatonin suppression on the rising part of the melatonin curve. In this regard, Phillips et al.48 have shown that prolonged exposures to light prior to predicted DLMO attenuate the impact of light-induced melatonin suppression at night and, moreover, their particular protocol adds significant variance among subjects to the threshold for melatonin suppression. Perhaps a more obvious caution, the duration term in the simplified function should not be used to predict light-induced phase shifts, during the night or during the day. Even though CSt may be a good representation of the light stimulus to the SCN, light-induced phase shifting responses may not have the same functional relationship to the light stimulus as light-induced nocturnal melatonin suppressions. For example, recent studies have shown that light-induced nocturnal melatonin suppression is different than light-induced phase response49,50. Therefore, more research is needed to model the potentially interactive phase response characteristics of the SCN to combinations of spectrum, amount, and duration.
Conclusions
The present study extended the Rea et al. model by introducing a duration-dependent parameter and a proposed simplified formulation, CSt, to predict nocturnal melatonin suppression during the early biological night. Data from a variety of published studies supported quantitative CSt model predictions of nocturnal melatonin suppression. Overall the simplified CSt formulation should be helpful in setting guidelines to limit melatonin suppression for residential applications where people are exposed to light prior to sleeping.
Data availability
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
References
Mure, L. S., Vinberg, F., Hanneken, A. & Panda, S. Functional diversity of human intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells. Science 366, 1251–1255. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaz0898 (2019).
Foster, R. et al. Circadian photoreception in the retinally degenerate mouse (rd/rd). J. Comp. Physiol. A 169, 39–50 (1991).
Ruby, N. F. et al. Role of Melanopsin in Circadian Responses to Light. Science 298, 2211–2213. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1076701 (2002).
Hattar, S. et al. Melanopsin and rod-cone photoreceptive systems account for all major accessory visual functions in mice. Nature 424, 75–81. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature01761 (2003).
Panda, S. et al. Melanopsin is required for non-image-forming photic responses in blind mice. Science 301, 525–527. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1086179 (2003).
Dacey, D. et al. Melanopsin-expressing ganglion cells in primate retina signal colour and irradiance and project to the LGN. Nature 433, 749–754 (2005).
Fechner, G. T. Elements of Psychophysics (Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York, 1966).
He, Y., Rea, M. S., Bierman, A. & Bullough, J. D. Evaluating light source efficacy at mesopic conditions using reaction times. JIES 26, 125–138 (1997).
Rea, M. S., Radetsky, L. C. & Bullough, J. D. Toward a model of outdoor lighting scene brightness. Light. Res. Technol. 43, 7–30 (2011).
Rea, M. S. & Ouellette, M. J. Relative visual performance: a basis for application. Light. Res. Technol. 23, 135–144 (1991).
Goodspeed, C. H. & Rea, M. S. The significance of surround conditions for roadway signs. JIES 28, 164–173 (1999).
Rea, M. S., Bullough, J. D. & Zhou, Y. A method for assessing the visibility benefits of roadway lighting. Light. Res. Technol. 42, 215–241 (2010).
Bullough, J. D. & Radetsky, L. C. Illuminating Engineering Society Annual Conference 203–207 (Illuminating Engineering Society, Pittsburgh, PA, 2014).
Bullough, J. D., Besenecker, U. C., Snyder, J. D. & Skinner, N. P. Work zone lighting and visual performance: analysis and demonstration. Transp. Res. Rec. 2337, 25–34. https://doi.org/10.3141/2337-04 (2013).
Brainard, G. C. et al. Action spectrum for melatonin regulation in humans: evidence for a novel circadian photoreceptor. J. Neurosci. 21, 6405–6412. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-16-06405.2001 (2001).
Thapan, K., Arendt, J. & Skene, D. J. An action spectrum for melatonin suppression: evidence for a novel non-rod, non-cone photoreceptor system in humans. J. Physiol 535, 261–267. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7793.2001.t01-1-00261.x (2001).
Rea, M. S., Figueiro, M. G., Bullough, J. D. & Bierman, A. A model of phototransduction by the human circadian system. Brain Res. Rev. 50, 213–228. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresrev.2005.07.002 (2005).
Rea, M. S., Figueiro, M. G., Bierman, A. & Hamner, R. Modelling the spectral sensitivity of the human circadian system. Light. Res. Technol. 44, 386–396. https://doi.org/10.1177/1477153511430474 (2012).
Macchi, M. M. & Bruce, J. N. Human pineal physiology and functional significance of melatonin. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 25, 177–195 (2004).
Moore, R. Neural control of the pineal gland. Behav. Brain Res. 73, 125–130 (1996).
Figueiro, M. G., Bullough, J. D., Parsons, R. H. & Rea, M. S. Preliminary evidence for spectral opponency in the suppression of melatonin by light in humans. NeuroReport 15, 313–316 (2004).
Figueiro, M. G., Bullough, J. D., Bierman, A. & Rea, M. S. Demonstration of additivity failure in human circadian phototransduction. Neuro Endocrinol. Lett. 26, 493–498 (2005).
Figueiro, M. G., Bierman, A. & Rea, M. S. Retinal mechanisms determine the subadditive response to polychromatic light by the human circadian system. Neurosci. Lett. 438, 242–245. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008.04.055 (2008).
Rea, M. S. & Figueiro, M. G. Light as a circadian stimulus for architectural lighting. Light. Res. Technol. 50, 497–510. https://doi.org/10.1177/1477153516682368 (2018).
Nagare, R., Rea, M. S., Plitnick, B. & Figueiro, M. G. Nocturnal melatonin suppression by adolescents and adults for different levels, spectra, and durations of light exposure. J. Biol. Rhythms 34, 178–194. https://doi.org/10.1177/0748730419828056 (2019).
Commission Internationale de l’Éclairage. Technical Note: Report on the First International Workshop on Circadian and Neurophysiological Photometry, 2013 (Commission Internationale de l’Éclairage, Vienna, 2015).
Bierman, A., Klein, T. R. & Rea, M. S. The daysimeter: a device for measuring optical radiation as a stimulus for the human circadian system. Meas. Sci. Technol. 16, 2292–2299. https://doi.org/10.1088/0957-0233/16/11/023 (2005).
Cajochen, C. et al. High sensitivity of human melatonin, alertness, thermoregulation, and heart rate to short wavelength light. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 90, 1311–1316. https://doi.org/10.1210/jc.2004-0957 (2005).
Chang, A. M. et al. Human responses to bright light of different durations. J. Physiol. 590, 3103–3112. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2011.226555 (2012).
Figueiro, M. G. et al. The impact of self-luminous electronic devices on melatonin suppression. Dig. Tech. Pap. 42, 408–411. https://doi.org/10.1889/1.3621337 (2011).
Gooley, J. J. et al. Spectral responses of the human circadian system depend on the irradiance and duration of exposure to light. Sci. Transl. Med. 2, 31–33. https://doi.org/10.1126/scitranslmed.3000741 (2010).
Kozaki, T., Hidaka, Y., Takakura, J. Y. & Kusano, Y. Salivary melatonin suppression under 100-Hz flickering blue light and non-flickering blue light conditions. Neurosci. Lett. 722, 134857. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2020.134857 (2020).
Kraneburg, A., Franke, S., Methling, R. & Griefahn, B. Effect of color temperature on melatonin production for illumination of working environments. Appl. Ergon. 58, 446–453. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2016.08.006 (2017).
Nagare, R., Plitnick, B. & Figueiro, M. G. Effect of exposure duration and light spectra on nighttime melatonin suppression in adolescents and adults. Light. Res. Technol. 51, 530–540. https://doi.org/10.1177/1477153518763003 (2019).
Rahman, S. A., Marcu, S., Shapiro, C. M., Brown, T. J. & Casper, R. F. Spectral modulation attenuates molecular, endocrine, and neurobehavioral disruption induced by nocturnal light exposure. Am. J. Physiol. Endocrinol. Metab. 300, E518-527. https://doi.org/10.1152/ajpendo.00597.2010 (2011).
Souman, J. L. et al. Spectral tuning of white light allows for strong reduction in melatonin suppression without changing illumination level or color temperature. J. Biol. Rhythms 33, 420–431. https://doi.org/10.1177/0748730418784041 (2018).
Van de Werken, M., Gimenez, M. C., de Vries, B., Beersma, D. G. & Gordijn, M. C. Short-wavelength attenuated polychromatic white light during work at night: limited melatonin suppression without substantial decline of alertness. Chronobiol. Int. 30, 843–854. https://doi.org/10.3109/07420528.2013.773440 (2013).
West, K. E. et al. Blue light from light-emitting diodes elicits a dose-dependent suppression of melatonin in humans. J. Appl. Physiol. 110, 619–626. https://doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.01413.2009 (2011).
Rea, M. S. & Figueiro, M. G. A working threshold for acute nocturnal melatonin suppression from “white” light sources used in architectural applications. J. Carcinog. Mutagen 4, 1000150. https://doi.org/10.4172/2157-2518.1000150 (2013).
Burgess, H. J. & Eastman, C. I. Early versus late bedtimes phase shift the human dim light melatonin rhythm despite a fixed morning lights on time. Neurosci. Lett. 356, 115–118 (2004).
Scheuermaier, K., Laffan, A. M. & Duffy, J. F. Light exposure patterns in healthy older and young adults. J. Biol. Rhythms 25, 113–122. https://doi.org/10.1177/0748730410361916 (2010).
Rea, M. S., Brons, J. A. & Figueiro, M. G. Measurements of light at night (LAN) for a sample of female school teachers. Chronobiol. Int. 28, 673–680. https://doi.org/10.3109/07420528.2011.602198 (2011).
Buccitelli, N., Elliott, C., Schober, S. & Yamada, M. 2015 U.S. Lighting Market Characterization (Office of Energy Efficiency & Renewable Energy, Washington, DC, 2017).
Gringras, P., Middleton, B., Skene, D. J. & Revell, V. L. Bigger, brighter, bluer-better? Current light-emitting devices: adverse sleep properties and preventative strategies. Front. Public Health 3, 233. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2015.00233 (2015).
Chang, A.-M., Aeschbach, D., Duffy, J. F. & Czeisler, C. A. Evening use of light-emitting eReaders negatively affects sleep, circadian timing, and next-morning alertness. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 112, 1232–1237. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1418490112 (2015).
Wood, B., Rea, M. S., Plitnick, B. & Figueiro, M. G. Light level and duration of exposure determine the impact of self-luminous tablets on melatonin suppression. Appl. Ergon. 44, 237–240. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2012.07.008 (2013).
Nagare, R., Plitnick, B. & Figueiro, M. G. Does the iPad Night Shift mode reduce melatonin suppression?. Light. Res. Technol. 51, 373–383. https://doi.org/10.1177/1477153517748189 (2019).
Phillips, A. J. K. et al. High sensitivity and interindividual variability in the response of the human circadian system to evening light. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 116, 12019–12024. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1901824116 (2019).
Figueiro, M. G., Bierman, A. & Rea, M. S. A train of blue light pulses delivered through closed eyelids suppresses melatonin and phase shifts the human circadian system. Nat. Sci. Sleep 5, 133–141. https://doi.org/10.2147/NSS.S52203 (2013).
Rahman, S. A. et al. Functional decoupling of melatonin suppression and circadian phase resetting in humans. J. Physiol 596, 2147–2157. https://doi.org/10.1113/jp275501 (2018).
Acknowledgements
The present study was funded by the Light and Health Alliance (Armstrong Ceiling and Wall Solutions; AXIS; CREE lighting, GE current, a Daintree company; LEDVANCE; OSRAM; and USAI Lighting); the National Institutes of Health (Training Program in Alzheimer’s Disease Clinical and Translational Research (NIA 5T32AG057464); and the Jim H. McClung Lighting Research Foundation. The authors wish to acknowledge the assistance of Barbara Plitnick, Sharon LeSage, Charles Jarboe, Allison Thayer, and David Pedler at the Lighting Research Center, Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
M.S.R. performed modeling and served as the primary author of the manuscript. R.N. performed data collection and modeling, prepared the figures and tables, and served as secondary author of the manuscript. M.G.F. supervised the data collection and melatonin analysis, and provided leadership in preparation of the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Rea, M.S., Nagare, R. & Figueiro, M.G. Predictions of melatonin suppression during the early biological night and their implications for residential light exposures prior to sleeping. Sci Rep 10, 14114 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-70619-5
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-70619-5
This article is cited by
-
Photophobia is associated with lower sleep quality in individuals with migraine: results from the American Registry for Migraine Research (ARMR)
The Journal of Headache and Pain (2024)
-
Numerical correlation between non-visual metrics and brightness metrics—implications for the evaluation of indoor white lighting systems in the photopic range
Scientific Reports (2023)
-
Towards intelligent illumination systems: from the basics of light science to its application
Zeitschrift für Arbeitswissenschaft (2023)