Key Points
-
The prefrontal cortex seems to be essential for the control and organization of behaviour. In particular, it has been linked to the relative cognitive sophistication that has been reached by higher primates, especially humans. However, the functions of one of its constituent regions, the orbitofrontal cortex, have remained enigmatic.
-
In terms of neuroanatomical connectivity, the primate orbitofrontal cortex is uniquely placed to integrate sensory and autonomic information to modulate behaviour through both visceral and motor systems.
-
Recent neuroimaging studies in humans have confirmed the role of the human orbitofrontal cortex as a nexus for sensory integration, modulation of visceral reactions, and participation in learning, prediction and decision making for emotional and reward-related behaviours. But these studies have also shown that the human orbitofrontal cortex is a highly heterogeneous brain region that encompasses many different functions.
-
In particular, the human orbitofrontal cortex has been found to represent not only the reward value and expected reward value of foods and other reinforcers, but also their subjective pleasantness. This link to subjective hedonic processing could provide a basis for further exploration of the brain systems involved in the conscious experience of pleasure and reward, and, as such, offer a unique method for studying the hedonic quality of human experience.
-
Based on the available evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology, a tentative new model of the functional neuroanatomy of the orbitofrontal cortex is offered with medial–lateral and posterior–anterior distinctions, in which the implicit reward value is assigned early on in the hierarchy for each type of reinforcer, with a further progression up the processing hierarchy (reflecting the effects of combinations of stimuli) towards areas that are connected to brain regions necessary for conscious hedonic processing.
-
At present, little is known about the functional and structural development of the human orbitofrontal cortex in children and adolescents. However, further investigation of the link to hedonic processing could potentially lead to a better understanding of and novel treatments for disorders linked to anhedonia, such as depression, obesity and eating disorders.
Abstract
Hedonic experience is arguably at the heart of what makes us human. In recent neuroimaging studies of the cortical networks that mediate hedonic experience in the human brain, the orbitofrontal cortex has emerged as the strongest candidate for linking food and other types of reward to hedonic experience. The orbitofrontal cortex is among the least understood regions of the human brain, but has been proposed to be involved in sensory integration, in representing the affective value of reinforcers, and in decision making and expectation. Here, the functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex is described and a new integrated model of its functions proposed, including a possible role in the mediation of hedonic experience.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Access options
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 12 print issues and online access
$189.00 per year
only $15.75 per issue
Buy this article
- Purchase on SpringerLink
- Instant access to full article PDF
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Holland, P. C. & Gallagher, M. Amygdala–frontal interactions and reward expectancy. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 14, 148–155 (2004). An excellent recent review of the converging evidence from mammals indicating that reinforcer expectancy is encoded through the interconnections between the basolateral complex of the amygdala and the orbitofrontal cortex.
Cardinal, R. N., Parkinson, J. A., Hall, J. & Everitt, B. J. Emotion and motivation: the role of the amygdala, ventral striatum, and prefrontal cortex. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 26, 321–352 (2002).
O'Doherty, J. et al. Sensory-specific satiety-related olfactory activation of the human orbitofrontal cortex. Neuroreport 11, 893–897 (2000).
Gottfried, J. A., O'Doherty, J. & Dolan, R. J. Encoding predictive reward value in human amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. Science 301, 1104–1107 (2003). A demonstration of how predictive reward value is encoded in the human brain in the orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala.
Kringelbach, M. L., O'Doherty, J., Rolls, E. T. & Andrews, C. Activation of the human orbitofrontal cortex to a liquid food stimulus is correlated with its subjective pleasantness. Cereb. Cortex 13, 1064–1071 (2003). A clear demonstration that neural activity in the orbitofrontal cortex is correlated with hedonic experience.
Fuster, J. M. The Prefrontal Cortex (Raven, New York, USA, 1997).
Pandya, D. N. & Yeterian, E. H. Comparison of prefrontal architecture and connections. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 351, 1423–1432 (1996).
Brodmann, K. Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Grosshirnrinde in ihren Prinzipien dargestellt auf Grund des Zellenbaues (Barth, Leipzig, Germany, 1909).
Walker, A. E. A cytoarchitectural study of the prefrontal area of the macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 73, 59–86 (1940).
Petrides, M. & Pandya, D. N. in Handbook of Neuropsychology Vol. 9 (eds Boller, F. & Grafman, J.) 17–58 (Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1994).
Carmichael, S. T. & Price, J. L. Architectonic subdivision of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in the macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 346, 366–402 (1994).
Chiavaras, M. M. & Petrides, M. Orbitofrontal sulci of the human and macaque monkey brain. J. Comp. Neurol. 422, 35–54 (2000).
Chiavaras, M. M. & Petrides, M. Three-dimensional probabilistic atlas of the human orbitofrontal sulci in standardized stereotaxic space. Neuroimage 13, 479–496 (2001).
Carmichael, S. T. & Price, J. L. Sensory and premotor connections of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of macaque monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 363, 642–664 (1995).
Barbas, H. Anatomic organization of basoventral and mediodorsal visual recipient prefrontal regions in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 276, 313–342 (1988).
Amaral, D. G. & Price, J. L. Amygdalo-cortical projections in the monkey (Macaca fascicularis). J. Comp. Neurol. 230, 465–496 (1984).
Carmichael, S. T. & Price, J. L. Limbic connections of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in macaque monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 363, 615–641 (1995).
Van Hoesen, G. W., Morecraft, R. J. & Vogt, B. A. in The Neurobiology of the Cingulate Cortex and Limbic Thalamus: A Comprehensive Handbook (eds Vogt, B. A. & Gabriel, M.) 249–284 (Birkhäuser, Boston, USA, 1993).
Öngür, D. & Price, J. L. The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. Cereb. Cortex 10, 206–219 (2000).
Mesulam, M. -M. & Mufson, E. J. Insula of the old world monkey. III Efferent cortical output and comments on function. J. Comp. Neurol. 212, 38–52 (1982).
Rempel-Clower, N. L. & Barbas, H. Topographic organization of connections between the hypothalamus and prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 398, 393–419 (1998).
Cavada, C., Company, T., Tejedor, J., Cruz Rizzolo, R. J. & Reinoso Suarez, F. The anatomical connections of the macaque monkey orbitofrontal cortex. A review. Cereb. Cortex 10, 220–242 (2000).
Eblen, F. & Graybiel, A. M. Highly restricted origin of prefrontal cortical inputs to striosomes in the macaque monkey. J. Neurosci. 15, 5999–6013 (1995).
Barbas, H. & Pandya, D. N. Architecture and intrinsic connections of the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 286, 353–375 (1989).
Nauta, W. J. The problem of the frontal lobe: a reinterpretation. J. Psychiatr. Res. 8, 167–187 (1971).
Rolls, E. T. The Brain and Emotion (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 1999).
Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H. & Anderson, S. W. Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50, 7–15 (1994). A classic paper showing that patients with lesions to the orbitofrontal and medial prefrontal cortices are impaired at real-life decision making, although they retain otherwise normal intellectual functions.
Kringelbach, M. L. & Rolls, E. T. The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. Prog. Neurobiol. 72, 341–372 (2004). A review of the functions of the human orbitofrontal cortex, including a large meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies showing that different subregions of the orbitofrontal cortex have different functions.
Elliott, R., Dolan, R. J. & Frith, C. D. Dissociable functions in the medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from human neuroimaging studies. Cereb. Cortex 10, 308–317 (2000).
Dias, R., Robbins, T. & Roberts, A. Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts. Nature 380, 69–72 (1996).
Rolls, E. T., Hornak, J., Wade, D. & McGrath, J. Emotion-related learning in patients with social and emotional changes associated with frontal lobe damage. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 57, 1518–1524 (1994).
Iversen, S. D. & Mishkin, M. Perseverative interference in monkeys following selective lesions of the inferior prefrontal convexity. Exp. Brain Res. 11, 376–386 (1970). A classic lesioning study that showed the importance of the lateral orbitofrontal cortex in reversal learning in monkeys.
Hornak, J. et al. Reward-related reversal learning after surgical excisions in orbitofrontal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 463–478 (2004).
Damasio, A. R. The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 351, 1413–1420 (1996).
James, W. The Principles of Psychology (Henry Holt, New York, USA, 1890).
Lange, C. G. Über Gemüstbewegungen. (Org. Om Sindsbevægelser) (Theodor Thomas, Leipzig, Germany, 1887).
Cannon, W. B. The James–Lange theory of emotion. Am. J. Psychol. 39, 106–124 (1927).
Craig, A. D. How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 3, 655–666 (2002).
Wilson, J. et al. Fast, fully automated global and local magnetic field optimization for fMRI of the human brain. Neuroimage 17, 967–976 (2002).
Deichmann, R., Josephs, O., Hutton, C., Corfield, D. R. & Turner, R. Compensation of susceptibility-induced BOLD sensitivity losses in echo-planar fMRI imaging. Neuroimage 15, 120–135 (2002).
Frey, S., Kostopoulos, P. & Petrides, M. Orbitofrontal involvement in the processing of unpleasant auditory information. Eur. J. Neurosci. 12, 3709–3712 (2000).
Small, D. M. et al. Human cortical gustatory areas: a review of functional neuroimaging data. Neuroreport 10, 7–14 (1999).
Zatorre, R. J., Jones-Gotman, M., Evans, A. C. & Meyer, E. Functional localization and lateralization of human olfactory cortex. Nature 360, 339–340 (1992).
Rolls, E. T. et al. Representations of pleasant and painful touch in the human orbitofrontal and cingulate cortices. Cereb. Cortex 13, 308–317 (2003).
Aharon, I. et al. Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron 32, 537–551 (2001).
Critchley, H. D., Mathias, C. J. & Dolan, R. J. Fear conditioning in humans: the influence of awareness and autonomic arousal on functional neuroanatomy. Neuron 33, 653–663 (2002).
Thut, G. et al. Activation of the human brain by monetary reward. Neuroreport 8, 1225–1228 (1997).
O'Doherty, J., Kringelbach, M. L., Rolls, E. T., Hornak, J. & Andrews, C. Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nature Neurosci. 4, 95–102 (2001).
Small, D. M., Jones-Gotman, M., Zatorre, R. J., Petrides, M. & Evans, A. C. Flavor processing: more than the sum of its parts. Neuroreport 8, 3913–3917 (1997).
De Araujo, I. E. T., Rolls, E. T., Kringelbach, M. L., McGlone, F. & Phillips, N. Taste–olfactory convergence, and the representation of the pleasantness of flavour, in the human brain. Eur. J. Neurosci. 18, 2059–2068 (2003).
Rolls, B. J., Rolls, E. T., Rowe, E. A. & Sweeney, K. Sensory specific satiety in man. Physiol. Behav. 27, 137–142 (1981).
Butter, C. M., Mishkin, M. & Rosvold, H. E. Conditioning and extinction of a food-rewarded response after selective ablations of frontal cortex in rhesus monkeys. Exp. Neurol. 7, 65–75 (1963).
Baylis, L. L. & Gaffan, D. Amygdalectomy and ventromedial prefrontal ablation produce similar deficits in food choice and in simple object discrimination learning for an unseen reward. Exp. Brain Res. 86, 617–622 (1991).
Baxter, M. G., Parker, A., Lindner, C. C., Izquierdo, A. D. & Murray, E. A. Control of response selection by reinforcer value requires interaction of amygdala and orbital prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 20, 4311–4319 (2000).
Rahman, S., Sahakian, B. J., Hodges, J. R., Rogers, R. D. & Robbins, T. W. Specific cognitive deficits in mild frontal variant frontotemporal dementia. Brain 122, 1469–1493 (1999).
Farrow, T. F. et al. Investigating the functional anatomy of empathy and forgiveness. Neuroreport 12, 2433–2438 (2001).
Blood, A. J., Zatorre, R. J., Bermudez, P. & Evans, A. C. Emotional responses to pleasant and unpleasant music correlate with activity in paralimbic brain regions. Nature Neurosci. 2, 382–387 (1999).
Rozin, P. in International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences (eds Smelser, N. J. & Baltes, P. B.) 5719–5722 (Elsevier, Amsterdam, 2001).
Harlow, J. M. Passage of an iron rod through the head. Boston Med. Surg. J. 39, 389–393 (1848).
Macmillan, M. An Odd Kind of Fame: Stories of Phineas Gage (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 2000).
Blair, R. J. & Cipolotti, L. Impaired social response reversal. A case of 'acquired sociopathy'. Brain 123, 1122–1141 (2000).
Anderson, S. W., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D. & Damasio, A. R. Impairment of social and moral behavior related to early damage in human prefrontal cortex. Nature Neurosci. 2, 1032–1037 (1999).
Hornak, J. et al. Changes in emotion after circumscribed surgical lesions of the orbitofrontal and cingulate cortices. Brain 126, 1671–1712 (2003). Strong evidence from surgically circumscribed lesions in humans showing that the orbitofrontal and medial prefrontal cortices are involved in emotion identification, social behaviour and subjective emotional state.
Hornak, J., Rolls, E. T. & Wade, D. Face and voice expression identification in patients with emotional and behavioural changes following ventral frontal lobe damage. Neuropsychologia 34, 247–261 (1996).
Schultz, W. & Dickinson, A. Neuronal coding of prediction errors. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 23, 473–500 (2000).
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D. & Anderson, S. W. Dissociation of working memory from decision making within the human prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 18, 428–437 (1998).
Maia, T. V. & McLelland, J. L. A reexamination of the evidence for the somatic marker hypothesis: what participants really know in the Iowa gambling task. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 101, 16075–16080 (2004).
Rogers, R. D. et al. Choosing between small, likely rewards and large, unlikely rewards activates inferior and orbital prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 19, 9029–9038 (1999).
Rolls, E. T., Kringelbach, M. L. & de Araujo, I. E. T. Different representations of pleasant and unpleasant odors in the human brain. Eur. J. Neurosci. 18, 695–703 (2003).
Anderson, A. K. et al. Dissociated neural representations of intensity and valence in human olfaction. Nature Neurosci. 6, 196–202 (2003).
Small, D. M. et al. Dissociation of neural representation of intensity and affective valuation in human gustation. Neuron 39, 701–711 (2003).
Schnider, A. & Ptak, R. Spontaneous confabulators fail to suppress currently irrelevant memory traces. Nature Neurosci. 2, 677–681 (1999).
Schnider, A. Spontaneous confabulation and the adaptation of thought to ongoing reality. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 4, 662–671 (2003). A fascinating review article that links the medial orbitofrontal cortex to spontaneous confabulation in patients and proposes that this region might serve to adapt to ongoing reality.
Schnider, A., Treyer, V. & Buck, A. The human orbitofrontal cortex monitors outcomes even when no reward is at stake. Neuropsychologia 43, 316–323 (2005).
Petrovic, P., Kalso, E., Petersson, K. M. & Ingvar, M. Placebo and opioid analgesia — imaging a shared neuronal network. Science 295, 1737–1740 (2002). An elegant demonstration of placebo mechanisms in the human brain in which the pain relief in placebo-responders is correlated with activity in lateral orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate cortices.
Petrovic, P. & Ingvar, M. Imaging cognitive modulation of pain processing. Pain 95, 1–5 (2002).
Nobre, A. C., Coull, J. T., Frith, C. D. & Mesulam, M. M. Orbitofrontal cortex is activated during breaches of expectation in tasks of visual attention. Nature Neurosci. 2, 11–12 (1999).
Kringelbach, M. L. & Rolls, E. T. Neural correlates of rapid context-dependent reversal learning in a simple model of human social interaction. Neuroimage 20, 1371–1383 (2003).
Kringelbach, M. L. Learning to change. PLoS Biol. 2, E140 (2004).
Blair, R. J., Morris, J. S., Frith, C. D., Perrett, D. I. & Dolan, R. J. Dissociable neural responses to facial expressions of sadness and anger. Brain 122, 883–893 (1999).
Walton, M. E., Devlin, J. T. & Rushworth, M. F. Interactions between decision making and performance monitoring within prefrontal cortex. Nature Neurosci. 7, 1259–1265 (2004). An important paper that demonstrates the central role played by the orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate cortices in decision making.
Ullsperger, M. & von Cramon, D. Y. Decision making, performance and outcome monitoring in frontal cortical areas. Nature Neurosci. 7, 1173–1174 (2004).
Chalmers, D. Facing up to the problem of consciousness. J. Conscious. Stud. 2, 200–219 (1995).
Hull, C. L. Essentials of Behavior (Yale Univ. Press, New Haven, Connecticut, USA, 1951).
Bindra, D. How adaptive behavior is produced: a perceptual–motivational alternative to response-reinforcement. Behav. Brain Sci. 1, 41–91 (1978).
Cabanac, M. Physiological role of pleasure. Science 173, 1103–1107 (1971).
Berridge, K. C. Food reward: brain substrates of wanting and liking. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 20, 1–25 (1996).
Berridge, K. C. & Robinson, T. E. What is the role of dopamine in reward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 28, 309–369 (1998).
Kringelbach, M. L. Food for thought: hedonic experience beyond homeostasis in the human brain. Neuroscience 126, 807–819 (2004).
Saper, C. B., Chou, T. C. & Elmquist, J. K. The need to feed: homeostatic and hedonic control of eating. Neuron 36, 199–211 (2002).
Kringelbach, M. L., O'Doherty, J., Rolls, E. T. & Andrews, C. Activation of the human orbitofrontal cortex to a liquid food stimulus is correlated with its subjective pleasantness. Cereb. Cortex 13, 1064–1071 (2003).
Hinton, E. C. et al. Neural contributions to the motivational control of appetite in humans. Eur. J. Neurosci. 20, 1411–1418 (2004). Demonstrates that the extrinsic incentive value of foods is located in mid-anterior parts of the orbitofrontal cortex.
De Araujo, I. E. T., Kringelbach, M. L., Rolls, E. T. & Hobden, P. The representation of umami taste in the human brain. J. Neurophysiol. 90, 313–319 (2003). Shows that the subjective synergistic enhancement of umami taste is represented in mid-anterior parts of the orbitofrontal cortex.
De Araujo, I. E. T., Kringelbach, M. L., Rolls, E. T. & McGlone, F. Human cortical responses to water in the mouth, and the effects of thirst. J. Neurophysiol. 90, 1865–1876 (2003).
Small, D. M., Zatorre, R. J., Dagher, A., Evans, A. C. & Jones-Gotman, M. Changes in brain activity related to eating chocolate: from pleasure to aversion. Brain 124, 1720–1733 (2001).
De Araujo, I. E. & Rolls, E. T. Representation in the human brain of food texture and oral fat. J. Neurosci. 24, 3086–3093 (2004).
Craig, A. D., Chen, K., Bandy, D. & Reiman, E. M. Thermosensory activation of insular cortex. Nature Neurosci. 3, 184–190 (2000).
Völlm, B. A. et al. Methamphetamine activates reward circuitry in drug naïve human subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology 29, 1715–1722 (2004).
Blood, A. J. & Zatorre, R. J. Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, 11818–11823 (2001).
Hornak, J. et al. Changes in emotion after circumscribed surgical lesions of the orbitofrontal and cingulate cortices. Brain 126, 1671–1712 (2003).
Drevets, W. C. Neuroimaging and neuropathological studies of depression: implications for the cognitive-emotional features of mood disorders. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 11, 240–249 (2001).
Volkow, N. D. & Li, T. K. Drug addiction: the neurobiology of behaviour gone awry. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 5, 963–970 (2004).
Dehaene, S., Kerszberg, M. & Changeux, J. P. A neuronal model of a global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 95, 14529–14534 (1998).
Gusnard, D. A. & Raichle, M. E. Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 2, 685–694 (2001).
Granger, C. W. J. Investigating causal relations by econometric models and cross-spectral methods. Econometrica 37, 424–438 (1969).
Dehaene, S. et al. Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming. Nature Neurosci. 4, 752–758 (2001).
Eslinger, P. J., Flaherty-Craig, C. V. & Benton, A. L. Developmental outcomes after early prefrontal cortex damage. Brain Cogn. 55, 84–103 (2004).
Uylings, H. B. & van Eden, C. G. Qualitative and quantitative comparison of the prefrontal cortex in rat and in primates, including humans. Prog. Brain Res. 85, 31–62 (1990).
Öngür, D. & Price, J. L. The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. Cereb. Cortex 10, 206–219 (2000). An important paper proposing that, on neuroanatomical grounds, the orbitofrontal cortex should be considered along with the medial prefrontal cortex as a crucial sensory–visceromotor link for consummatory behaviours.
Papez, J. Comparative Neurology (Crowell, New York, 1929).
Brodmann, K. Neue Ergebnisse ueber die Vergleichende histologische Localisation der Grosshirnfinde mit besonderer Berucksichtigung des Stirnhirns. Anat. Anz. Suppl. 41, 157–216 (1912).
Passingham, R. E. The Human Primate (W. H. Freeman, Oxford, 1982).
Schoenbaum, G. & Setlow, B. Integrating orbitofrontal cortex into prefrontal theory: common processing themes across species and subdivisions. Learn. Mem. 8, 134–147 (2001).
Darwin, C. The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals (Univ. Chicago Press, Chicago, 1872).
Kringelbach, M. L. in The Oxford Companion to the Mind 2nd edn (ed. Gregory, R. L.) 287–290 (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 2004).
Weiskrantz, L. in Analysis of Behavioural Change (ed. Weiskrantz, L.) 50–90 (Harper and Row, New York/London, 1968).
Gogtay, N. et al. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 101, 8174–8179 (2004).
Acknowledgements
This research is supported by the Wellcome Trust and the Medical Research Council (to the Oxford Centre for Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of the Brain (FMRIB)).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The author declares no competing financial interests.
Related links
Glossary
- REINFORCERS
-
Positive reinforcers (rewards) increase the frequency of behaviour that leads to their acquisition. Negative reinforcers (punishers) decrease the frequency of behaviour that leads to their encounter and increase the frequency of behaviour that leads to their avoidance.
- BRODMANN'S AREAS
-
(BA). Korbinian Brodmann (1868–1918) was an anatomist who divided the cerebral cortex into numbered subdivisions on the basis of cell arrangements, types and staining properties (for example, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex contains several subdivisions, including BA 46 and BA 9). Modern derivatives of Brodmann's maps are commonly used as the reference system for the discussion of brain-imaging findings.
- GOAL-DIRECTED BEHAVIOUR
-
Behaviour directed towards the attainment of a future state (for example, obtaining the next meal).
- VENTROMEDIAL PREFRONTAL CORTEX
-
An anatomical term that refers to most of the medial orbitofrontal cortex and areas on the medial wall, but not to more central and lateral regions of the orbitofrontal cortex.
- RESPONSE INHIBITION
-
Lack of control and general perseveration are symptoms that commonly follow damage to the frontal lobes, and have often been ascribed to a lack of inhibitory control over the appropriate responses.
- REVERSAL LEARNING
-
Describes a task in which participants are trained to respond differentially to two stimuli under conditions of reward and punishment (or non-reward), and subsequently have to learn to change their behaviour when the reward values are reversed (that is, when the previously rewarded stimulus is no longer rewarded, and vice versa).
- JAMES–LANGE THEORY
-
Two nineteenth-century scholars, William James and Carl Lange, independently proposed that emotions arise as a result of bodily physiological events, such as increases in heart rate, rather than being the cause of them
- SELECTIVE SATIETY
-
A form of reinforcer devaluation in which participants that have been fed to satiety on one food still find other foods rewarding, and will eat some of these other foods. Selective satiety is particularly useful for studying affective representation in the brain, because it provides a means of altering the affective value of a stimulus without modifying its physical attributes, allowing a change in reward value to be detected.
- CO-ACTIVATION
-
The presence, in a neuroimaging experiment, of significant activity in two brain structures in the same subtraction.
- TRUE TASTE SYNERGISM
-
The combined effect of two taste stimuli, when greater than the sum of the effects of each one present alone.
- MULTIMODAL INTEGRATION
-
The process of combining information from different sensory modalities.
- MAGNETOENCEPHALOGRAPHY
-
(MEG). A non-invasive technique that allows the detection of the changing magnetic fields that are associated with brain activity on the timescale of milliseconds.
- GRANGER CAUSALITY
-
A technique for determining whether one time series is useful in predicting another.
- ANHEDONIA
-
Loss of interest or pleasure in almost all activities.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Kringelbach, M. The human orbitofrontal cortex: linking reward to hedonic experience. Nat Rev Neurosci 6, 691–702 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1747
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1747
This article is cited by
-
Roles of the medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortex in major depression and its treatment
Molecular Psychiatry (2024)
-
Mapping brain functional networks topological characteristics in new daily persistent headache: a magnetoencephalography study
The Journal of Headache and Pain (2023)
-
Altered reward processing in patients with lifelong premature ejaculation
Scientific Reports (2023)
-
First-in-human prediction of chronic pain state using intracranial neural biomarkers
Nature Neuroscience (2023)
-
Disruptions of default mode network and precuneus connectivity associated with cognitive dysfunctions in tinnitus
Scientific Reports (2023)