Abstract
SEVERAL examples of experience-dependent perceptual improve-ment (perceptual learning) suggest that plasticity in specific neu-ronal loci could underlie the learning process1–6. For a basic visual discrimination task (using an optimal stimulus for ‘automatic’ pre-attentive texture segregation7–10), discrete retinal input-dependent changes within a very early stage in the stream of visual processing were indicated as the locus of a large and consistent learning effect5. When do these changes occur? Here we report that except for a fast, rapidly saturating improvement early in the first practice session, performance was very stable within sessions. Indeed, observers showed little or no improvement until up to 8 hours after their last training session (latent phase). But large improvements occurred thereafter. Finally, there was almost no forgetting; what was gained was retained for at least 2–3 years. We conjecture that some types of perceptual experience trigger permanent neural changes in early processing stages of the adult visual system. These may take many hours to become functional.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Access options
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 51 print issues and online access
$199.00 per year
only $3.90 per issue
Buy this article
- Purchase on SpringerLink
- Instant access to full article PDF
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Ramachandran, V. S. & Braddick, O. Perception 2, 371–376 (1973).
McKee, S. P. & Westheimer, G. Percept. Psychophys. 24, 258–262 (1978).
Fiorentini, A. & Berardi, N. Vision Res. 21, 1149–1158 (1981).
Ball, K. & Sekuler, R. Vision Res. 27, 935–965 (1987).
Karni, A. & Sagi, D. Proc. natn. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 88, 4966–4970 (1991).
Poggio, T., Fahle, M. & Edelman, S. Science 256, 1018–1021 (1992).
Julesz, B. in Dynamic Aspects of Neocorticai Function (eds Edelman, G. M., Cowan, W. M. & Gall, W. E.) 585–612 (Wiley, New York, 1984).
Beck, J. (ed.) in Organization and Representation in Perception 285–317 (Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey, 1982).
Nothdurft, H. C. Vision Res. 25, 551–560 (1985).
Sagi, D. & Julesz, B. Science 228, 1217–1219 (1985).
Gurnsey, R. & Browse, R. A. Percept. Psychophys. 41, 239–252 (1987).
Zeki, S. M. Nature 274, 423–428 (1978).
Hubel, D. Nature 299, 515–524 (1982).
Ullman, S. Cognition 18, 97–157 (1984).
Polat, U. & Sagi, D. Vision Res. 33, 993–999 (1993).
Polat, U. & Sagi, D. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 34 (suppl.), 776 (1993).
Buisseret, P., Gary-Bobo, E. & Imbert M. Nature 272, 816–817 (1978).
McGaugh, J. L. Science 153, 1351–1358 (1966).
Matthies, H. Progr. Neurobiol. 32, 277–349 (1989).
Horn, G. & McCabe, B. J. J. Physiol. 423, 92 (1990).
Bear, M. F., Gu, Q., Kleinschmidt, A. & Singer, W. J. Neurosci. 10, 909–925 (1990).
Karni, A. & Sagi, D. Neurosci. Abstr. 18, 387 (1992).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Karni, A., Sagi, D. The time course of learning a visual skill. Nature 365, 250–252 (1993). https://doi.org/10.1038/365250a0
Received:
Accepted:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/365250a0