Nothing Special   »   [go: up one dir, main page]

You seem to have javascript disabled. Please note that many of the page functionalities won't work as expected without javascript enabled.
 
 
Sign in to use this feature.

Years

Between: -

Subjects

remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline

Journals

remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline

Article Types

Countries / Regions

remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline

Search Results (1,134)

Search Parameters:
Keywords = spatial cognition

Order results
Result details
Results per page
Select all
Export citation of selected articles as:
13 pages, 1750 KiB  
Article
Dysbindin-1 Mutation Alters Prefrontal Cortex Extracellular Glutamate and Dopamine In Vivo
by Karen K. Szumlinski, Michael C. Datko, Kevin D. Lominac and J. David Jentsch
Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2024, 25(23), 12732; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms252312732 - 27 Nov 2024
Viewed by 100
Abstract
Elevated risk for schizophrenia is associated with a variation in the DTNBP1 gene encoding dysbindin-1, which may underpin cognitive impairments in this prevalent neuropsychiatric disorder. The cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia involve anomalies in glutamate and dopamine signaling, particularly within the prefrontal cortex (PFC). [...] Read more.
Elevated risk for schizophrenia is associated with a variation in the DTNBP1 gene encoding dysbindin-1, which may underpin cognitive impairments in this prevalent neuropsychiatric disorder. The cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia involve anomalies in glutamate and dopamine signaling, particularly within the prefrontal cortex (PFC). Indeed, mice with Dtnbp1 mutations exhibit spatial and working memory deficits that are associated with deficits in glutamate release and NMDA receptor function as determined by slice electrophysiology. The present study extended the results from ex vivo approaches by examining how the Dtnbp1 mutation impacts high K+- and NMDA receptor-evoked glutamate release within the PFC using in vivo microdialysis procedures. Dntbp1 mutant mice are also reported to exhibit blunted K+-evoked dopamine release within the PFC. Thus, we examined also K+- and NMDA-evoked dopamine release within this region. Perfusion of high-concentration K+ or NMDA solutions increased the PFC levels of both dopamine and glutamate in wild-type (WT) but not in Dtnbp1 mutants (MUT), whereas mice heterozygous for the Dtnbp1 mutation (HET) exhibited blunted K+-evoked dopamine release. No net-flux microdialysis procedures confirmed elevated basal extracellular content of both glutamate and dopamine within the PFC of HET and MUT mice. These in vivo microdialysis results corroborate prior indications that Dtnbp1 mutations perturb evoked dopamine and glutamate release within the PFC, provide in vivo evidence for impaired NMDA receptor function within the PFC, and suggest that these neurochemical anomalies may be related to abnormally elevated basal neurotransmitter content. Full article
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Glutamatergic Signaling in the Nervous System)
Show Figures

Figure 1

Figure 1
<p>Cartoon of the location of the active membranes of the microdialysis probes of wild-type (WT), heterozygous mutant (HET), and homozygous mutant (MUT) mice in the study of (<b>A</b>) K+-evoked neurotransmitter release and glutamate no net-flux and (<b>B</b>) NMDA-evoked neurotransmitter release and dopamine no net-flux.</p>
Full article ">Figure 2
<p>Comparison of the effects of perfusion of K+ solutions (0, 50, and 100 mM) into the mPFC of wild-type (WT), heterozygous mutant (HET), and homozygous mutant (MUT) mice on (<b>A</b>) extracellular levels of glutamate and (<b>B</b>) extracellular levels of dopamine. The data represent the means +/− SEMs of the number of mice indicated in parentheses. * <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.05 vs. WT (Genotype effect; SNK post-hoc tests); # <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.05 vs. 0 mM (K+ effect; corrected <span class="html-italic">t</span>-tests).</p>
Full article ">Figure 3
<p>Comparison of the effects of perfusion of NMDA-containing solutions (0, 100, and 500 μM) into the mPFC of wild-type (WT), heterozygous mutant (HET), and homozygous mutant (MUT) mice on (<b>A</b>) extracellular levels of glutamate and (<b>B</b>) extracellular levels of dopamine. The data represent the means +/− SEMs of the number of mice indicated in parentheses. * <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.05 vs. WT (Genotype effect; SNK post-hoc tests); # <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.05 vs. 0 mM (NMDA effect; corrected <span class="html-italic">t</span>-tests).</p>
Full article ">Figure 4
<p>Summary of the results of the no net-flux microdialysis experiments in which (<b>A</b>) glutamate and (<b>B</b>) dopamine were perfused into the mPFC of wild-type (WT), heterozygous mutant (HET) and homozygous mutant (MUT) mice. (<b>C</b>) Comparison of the x-intercept (y = 0; estimate of basal neurotransmitter content) for glutamate (left) and the slopes of the linear regression for the glutamate study (reflection of clearance/reuptake; right) between WT, HET, and MUT mice. The data in panel C is derived from panel A. (<b>D</b>) Comparison of y = 0 (left) and the slopes of the linear regressions (right) from the dopamine study. The data in panel D are derived from panel B. The data represent the means +/− SEMs of the number of mice indicated in parentheses. * <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.05 vs. WT (Genotype effect; SNK post-hoc tests).</p>
Full article ">
11 pages, 384 KiB  
Article
The Effect of Rearing and Adult Environment on HPA Axis Responsivity and Plumage Condition in Laying Hens
by Janicke Nordgreen, Lucille Dumontier, Tom V. Smulders, Judit Vas, Rupert Palme and Andrew M. Janczak
Animals 2024, 14(23), 3422; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani14233422 - 26 Nov 2024
Viewed by 224
Abstract
The hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis responsivity is influenced by early life experience and also modified by the environment an individual experiences as an adult. Because laying hens are transferred from rearing to laying farms at 16–18 weeks of age, they are well suited to [...] Read more.
The hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis responsivity is influenced by early life experience and also modified by the environment an individual experiences as an adult. Because laying hens are transferred from rearing to laying farms at 16–18 weeks of age, they are well suited to study the interaction effect between early (rearing) and adult (laying) environments on physiology and behaviour. In the European Union, there is a move towards cage-free systems for laying hens, but globally, the majority of layers are kept in conventional or furnished cages. Conventional cages have no enrichment, whereas furnished cages have little enrichment. Both cage types limit the movement of birds and the expression of highly motivated behaviours. Lack of enrichment may lead to under-stimulated birds that are more sensitive to stress and fear-inducing challenges later in life. Fearfulness has been linked to feather pecking, which has negative consequences for animal welfare and productivity. Previous research has shown that birds reared in cages are more fearful and perform more poorly in a test of spatial cognition than birds reared in aviaries when tested within five weeks after transition to adult housing. However, recent results suggest that these effects might not be long-lasting. We, therefore, tested the effect of the early (rearing) (aviary vs. cage) and adult (laying) environments (standard vs. additionally enriched furnished cages) on the corticosterone response to restraint in birds that were 35 weeks into lay (i.e., 52 weeks of age) and assessed their plumage condition. We hypothesised that a lower level of enrichment, both during rearing and adulthood, would represent a lower level of stimulation, resulting in a stronger corticosterone response to restraint and poorer feather cover. Both stressed (restrained) and control birds increased their corticosterone levels from the first (baseline) to second sample (p < 0.01 for both), but the increase in the restrained group was significantly higher than in the control group (F1, 111 = 9.51; p = 0.003). There was no effect of the early environment, but birds housed in standard furnished cages as adults had overall higher corticosterone levels than birds housed in enriched furnished cages regardless of their early life environment (F1, 51 = 4.12; p = 0.048). Neither early nor adult housing influenced the feather score except for on the belly, where birds housed in enriched cages as adults had a poorer feather score, contrary to our prediction. In conclusion, no effect of the early environment on HPA axis responsivity to an acute stressor could be detected at 52 weeks of age, but adult enrichment had a favourable effect on overall corticosterone levels. Full article
(This article belongs to the Section Animal Welfare)
27 pages, 1795 KiB  
Article
Housing Environmental Enrichment, Lifestyles, and Public Health Indicators of Neurogenesis in Humans: A Pilot Study
by Mohamed Hesham Khalil and Koen Steemers
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2024, 21(12), 1553; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph21121553 - 25 Nov 2024
Viewed by 419
Abstract
Background: In response to the rising mental health concerns and cognitive decline associated with the human brain’s neurogenesis, which continues until the tenth decade of life but declines with age and is suppressed by poor environments, this pilot study investigates how physical environments [...] Read more.
Background: In response to the rising mental health concerns and cognitive decline associated with the human brain’s neurogenesis, which continues until the tenth decade of life but declines with age and is suppressed by poor environments, this pilot study investigates how physical environments may influence public health proxy measures of neurogenesis in humans. This pilot study focuses on the residential environment where people spend most of their time and age in place, exploring the dependency of depression, anxiety, and cognitive impairment variations on spatial and lifestyle variables. Methods: A total of 142 healthy adults in England completed a survey consisting of PHQ-8, GAD-7, and CFI questionnaires and other questions developed to capture the variance in spatial and lifestyle factors such as time spent at home, house type layout complexity, spaciousness, physical activity, routine and spatial novelty, and perceived loneliness. Results: Extensive time spent at home has adverse effects on all measures, while multi-storey houses perform better than single-story houses with positive correlations with physical activity and spatial novelty. Separate regression models on the variance in depression, as the most salient dependent variable and reliably associated with neurogenesis, reveal that getting out of the house explains 20.5% of the variance in depression symptoms. At the scale of the house, multi-storey houses explain 16.5% of the variance. Both percentages are closer to the effect of loneliness, which we found to explain 26.6% of the variance in depression. Conclusions: The built environment appears to be significantly associated with changes in cognitive function and mental health symptoms associated with neurogenesis. This pilot study shows the equally important effect of physical and social enrichment, offering critically needed insights for neuroarchitecture and brain health research that is interested in public health. Full article
Show Figures

Figure 1

Figure 1
<p>Pilot study design. Depression, anxiety, and cognitive impairments are public health proxies representing changes in adult hippocampal neurogenesis (AHN), while the environmental enrichment of housing is explained through the built environment, lifestyles, and the social environment with an emphasis on perceived loneliness.</p>
Full article ">Figure 2
<p>Significant mean difference between house types.</p>
Full article ">Figure 3
<p>Variance in depression (proxy of adult hippocampal neurogenesis) explained by the variability of housing structural enrichment. The different house types combined with the effect of physical activity through the house and the effect of spatial novelty (changing space used for each activity) together explain 16.5% of the variance in depression. Within this percentage, physical activity throughout the house explains 7.3%, while spatial novelty explains 3.1%, and the independent variable explaining the remaining percentage requires further research.</p>
Full article ">Figure 4
<p>Percentage of variance in depression through physical enrichment and social environments potentially regulating AHN.</p>
Full article ">
15 pages, 2423 KiB  
Article
The Neurophysiological Effects of Virtual Reality Application and Perspectives of Using for Multitasking Training in Cardiac Surgery Patients: Pilot Study
by Irina Tarasova, Olga Trubnikova, Irina Kukhareva, Darya Kupriyanova and Anastasia Sosnina
Appl. Sci. 2024, 14(23), 10893; https://doi.org/10.3390/app142310893 - 25 Nov 2024
Viewed by 391
Abstract
Background: The use of virtual reality (VR) has great potential for medical research and practice, which can help expand rehabilitation activities. This study aimed to evaluate the acceptability and feasibility of the original protocol VR multitasking cognitive training (CT) in both a healthy [...] Read more.
Background: The use of virtual reality (VR) has great potential for medical research and practice, which can help expand rehabilitation activities. This study aimed to evaluate the acceptability and feasibility of the original protocol VR multitasking cognitive training (CT) in both a healthy group and cardiac surgery patients. Methods: A specialized hardware and software complex was developed and tested on both a group of 25 practically healthy individuals, mean age 22.9 years (SD 2.57), and 25 cardiac surgery patients, mean age 62.2 years (SD 7.35). The participants were instructed to complete one session of multitasking CT within the VR complex. Psychometric testing and EEG studies were conducted. Results: All participants were highly accepting of the intended future use, attitude, and pleasure of the training. The healthy subjects demonstrated a statistically significant improvement in attention and spatial skills after VR (p ˂ 0.05). The EEG data revealed a significant increase in alpha power compared to pre-experiment levels (p ˂ 0.0001). The theta/alpha ratio significantly decreased after the VR multitasking CT as compared to the baseline (p ˂ 0.0001). Conclusions: The data obtained indicate that the original training protocol for multitasking CT using VR is acceptable and easy to use for both healthy individuals and cardiac surgery patients. Full article
(This article belongs to the Section Applied Biosciences and Bioengineering)
Show Figures

Figure 1

Figure 1
<p>The hardware devices composing the VR training system. (<b>a</b>) VR headmounted display. (<b>b</b>) The wheel.</p>
Full article ">Figure 2
<p>The virtual multitasking cognitive training. (<b>a</b>) A cognitive task; (<b>b</b>) A cognitive task’s answer. In this case, a participant should press the key on the wheel to indicate an incorrect answer to a cognitive task; (<b>c</b>) End of the session; (<b>d</b>) General view of experiment.</p>
Full article ">Figure 2 Cont.
<p>The virtual multitasking cognitive training. (<b>a</b>) A cognitive task; (<b>b</b>) A cognitive task’s answer. In this case, a participant should press the key on the wheel to indicate an incorrect answer to a cognitive task; (<b>c</b>) End of the session; (<b>d</b>) General view of experiment.</p>
Full article ">Figure 3
<p>The average power ratio between the theta and alpha rhythms: dark columns—the indicators before multitasking CT within the VR complex, light columns—the indicators after completing one session of multitasking CT within the VR complex, error bars denote SE.</p>
Full article ">Figure 4
<p>The results of the SUS (<b>a</b>) and SMEQ (<b>b</b>) scales in the healthy group: in the subfigures, the mean scores and minimum-maximum of appropriate scales are shown.</p>
Full article ">Figure 5
<p>The results of the SUS (<b>a</b>) and SMEQ (<b>b</b>) scales in the cardiac surgery patients: in the subfigures, the mean scores and minimum-maximum of appropriate scales are shown.</p>
Full article ">
27 pages, 3910 KiB  
Article
Exploring the Application of Neurostructural Principles to the Design of Public Spaces on University Campuses
by Qihang Zhou and Xingxing Fang
Land 2024, 13(12), 1978; https://doi.org/10.3390/land13121978 - 21 Nov 2024
Viewed by 233
Abstract
In this study, we examined the application of neurostructural principles to the design of public spaces on university campuses to optimize students’ learning efficiency, social interactions, and psychological well-being. Using Hainan University in China as a case study, a descriptive analysis was used [...] Read more.
In this study, we examined the application of neurostructural principles to the design of public spaces on university campuses to optimize students’ learning efficiency, social interactions, and psychological well-being. Using Hainan University in China as a case study, a descriptive analysis was used to evaluate the case study design of the data. Data on students’ preferences for and satisfaction with public learning spaces (libraries, student centers, and open learning areas) were also collected through a questionnaire. The questionnaire was based on the four stages of the AIDA (Attention, Interest, Desire, and Action) model and covered basic information about the participants and their first impressions of the learning spaces, design element preferences, emotional and cognitive influences, and willingness to participate in improving the design of campus spaces. Data were analyzed using quantitative methods, including frequency analysis and score aggregation, to assess the students’ satisfaction with the existing design elements of the learning space and their suggestions for potential improvements. A random sample of students enrolled at Hainan University was used to ensure that the data were representative. The results of the study indicate that the rational allocation of natural light, the optimization of the acoustic environment, the adoption of soothing color schemes, and flexible spatial layouts are effective at relieving students’ psychological stress, enhancing their academic performance, and facilitating social interactions. Some of the existing designs are already in line with neurostructural principles, but there is still room for improvement, especially in terms of color schemes and spatial configurations. Students have positive attitudes towards participating in campus space improvement, with especially high interest in light optimization, spatial layout, and the use of natural materials. This study verifies the effectiveness of using neural structure principles in campus public spaces by establishing an empirical model, proves its positive effect on the quality of the campus environment and students’ well-being, and provides empirical evidence and theoretical support for future campus design. Full article
Show Figures

Figure 1

Figure 1
<p>Neuroarchitecture conceptual relationship diagram (source: the author of this study).</p>
Full article ">Figure 2
<p>Research hypothesis model (source: the author of this study).</p>
Full article ">Figure 3
<p>Diagram of AIDA model (source: the Internet).</p>
Full article ">Figure 4
<p>Library and classrooms at Hainan University’s Haidian Campus (source: Google Maps).</p>
Full article ">Figure 5
<p>Emotional impact diagram of users at the Desire stage (Q14–Q16).</p>
Full article ">Figure 6
<p>Q17 data chart.</p>
Full article ">Figure 7
<p>Q18: The application of natural elements in campus public learning spaces and their impact on personal emotions.</p>
Full article ">Figure 8
<p>Q19: Proportions of user choices regarding neuro-architectural elements.</p>
Full article ">Figure 9
<p>Q20: Would you like to participate in the design and feedback process regarding common learning spaces on campus to help improve them?</p>
Full article ">Figure 10
<p>Q21–Q22 results.</p>
Full article ">
17 pages, 3196 KiB  
Article
Comparative Study of Injected Alzheimer’s Disease Models in Rats: Insights from Experimental Research
by Hanane Doumar, Hicham El Mostafi, Aboubaker Elhessni, Abderrahim Laaziz and Abdelhalem Mesfioui
Pathophysiology 2024, 31(4), 643-659; https://doi.org/10.3390/pathophysiology31040047 - 20 Nov 2024
Viewed by 404
Abstract
Background/Objectives: Alzheimer’s disease (AD) remains incurable, highlighting the need for new and diverse animal models to better understand its complex mechanisms. This study compares various injected animal models of AD, focusing on the main theories that explain the disease; Methods: Female Wistar rats [...] Read more.
Background/Objectives: Alzheimer’s disease (AD) remains incurable, highlighting the need for new and diverse animal models to better understand its complex mechanisms. This study compares various injected animal models of AD, focusing on the main theories that explain the disease; Methods: Female Wistar rats (10-months old) were administered intracebroventricularly by artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF) (Control), beta amyloid Aβ1-42 (BA), okadaic acid (OKA), lipopolysaccharides (LPS), buthionine sulfoximine (BSO) or by a mixture of these different molecules (MLG). Cognitive performance was assessed one week or one month after stereotaxic surgery; Results: Our results, show that only the Aβ and the MLG induced a persistence and progressive deficits in the working memory, recognition memory and spatial memory in rats. As the hippocampus (HIP) and the prefrontal cortex (PFC) are particularly involved in memory behavior, we analyzed long-term neuroadaptations in these brain subregions using spectrophotometric and histological methods to assess oxidative stress changes and neuronal loss, respectively. We found that the behavioral impairments in memory and learning were accompanied by irreversible oxidative stress changes and neurodegenerescence, particularly in the HIP; Conclusions: This study provides promising data on the modeling of AD in order to develop an effective therapeutic approach. Full article
Show Figures

Figure 1

Figure 1
<p>Experimental timeline.</p>
Full article ">Figure 2
<p>Cognitive performance in the Y-Maze (Y-Mz), Novel Object Recognition (NOR) and Morris water maze (MWM) tests. These tests were performed 1 week (1W) or 1 month (1M) after the stereotaxic surgery in rats injected by aCSF (CTR), beta amyloid Aβ1-42 (BA), okadaic acid (OKA), lipopolysaccharides (LPS), buthionine sulfoximine (BSO) or by a mixture of these different molecules (MLG). Bars represent the mean ± SEM. of (<b>A</b>) the Y-maze spontaneous alternation (%), (<b>B</b>) the recognition index (%), (<b>C</b>,<b>D</b>) the latency to reach the hidden platform (s) in the MWM, one week and one month after stereotaxic surgery, respectively, (<b>E</b>) the time in the quadrant cible assessed during a probe trial of the MWM, (<b>F</b>) The Cognition <math display="inline"><semantics> <mrow> <mi>Z</mi> </mrow> </semantics></math>-score was calculated based on the animals’ performances in the three preceding behavioral tests. Symbols Signification: * <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.05, ** <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.01, *** <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.001 vs. CTR; <sup>k</sup> <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.05, <sup>kk</sup> <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.01 vs. AOK; <sup>p</sup> <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.05, <sup>pp</sup> <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.01 vs. LPS; <sup>b</sup> <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.05, <sup>bbb</sup> <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.001 vs. BTN according to two-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s test.</p>
Full article ">Figure 3
<p>Histologic quantification of neurons in rats’ brain. (<b>A</b>) Regions of interest and their anatomical positions: prelimbic cortex (PrL), medial coretx (M1), hippocampal pyramidal cell (CA3) and dental gyrus (DG) (2.70 and −3.80 mm relative to bregma; left hemisphere). (<b>B</b>) Number of neurons per mm2 assessed by cell counting in PrL, M1, CA3 and DG subregions. (<b>C</b>) Representative Nissl-stained forebrain sections showing potential inflammatory infiltration in the MLG group (green arrows) across the M1, PrL, CA1 and CA3 brain subregions. The data are illustrated as box-and-whisker plots for CTR, OKA, LPS, BSO, BA and MLG groups. Each value represents the mean ± SEM. from 5 animals per group. Symbols Signification: * <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.05, ** <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.01 vs. CTR according to Kruskal–Wallis ANOVA, and Dunn’s multiple comparison test. Scale bars: 200 µm. Drawings are adapted from Paxinos &amp; Watson, (2013). All coordinates for the studied subregions were obtained from the rat brain atlas [<a href="#B23-pathophysiology-31-00047" class="html-bibr">23</a>].</p>
Full article ">Figure 4
<p>Histologic semi-quantitative analysis of neurodegeneration in rats’ brain. (<b>A</b>) Score of Neurodegeneration in prelimbic cortex (PrL), medial coretx (M1), hippocampal pyramidal cell (CA3) and dental gyrus (DG) subregions. (<b>B</b>) Representative images of Nissl-stained forebrain sections (2.70 and −3.80 mm relative to bregma; left hemisphere; PrL, M1, CA3, and DG) for CTR, OKA, LPS, BSO, BA, and MLG groups. Cellular loss and areas of neurodegeneration are indicated by black and red arrows, respectively. Each value represents the mean ± SEM. from 5 animals per group. Symbols Signification: * <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.05, ** <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.01, *** <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.001 vs. CTR; <sup>k</sup> <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.05, <sup>kk</sup> <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.01, <sup>kkk</sup> <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.001 vs. AOK; <sup>p</sup> <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.05, <sup>pp</sup> <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.01, <sup>ppp</sup> <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.001 vs. LPS; <sup>bb</sup> <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.01, <sup>bbb</sup> <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.001 vs. BTN; <sup>δδδ</sup> <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.001: 1W vs. 1M (time effect) according to Kruskal–Wallis ANOVA, and Dunn’s multiple comparison test. Scale bars: 50 µm or 150 µm. Drawings are adapted from the rat brain atlas [<a href="#B23-pathophysiology-31-00047" class="html-bibr">23</a>].</p>
Full article ">Figure 5
<p>Hypothetical percentages of involvement of pathophysiology triggering AD. The <math display="inline"><semantics> <mrow> <mi>Z</mi> </mrow> </semantics></math>-scores, expressed as percentages, were calculated for each experimental group after one week (short-term) or one month (long-term) post-stereotaxic surgery. These <math display="inline"><semantics> <mrow> <mi>Z</mi> </mrow> </semantics></math>-scores serve as indicators for the corresponding pathophysiological hypotheses related to AD. For instance, the <math display="inline"><semantics> <mrow> <mi>Z</mi> </mrow> </semantics></math>-score associated with the beta-amyloid accumulation hypothesis reflects the average of the <math display="inline"><semantics> <mrow> <mi>Z</mi> </mrow> </semantics></math>-scores across all behavioral parameters (Cognition <math display="inline"><semantics> <mrow> <mi>Z</mi> </mrow> </semantics></math>-score), biochemical parameters (Oxidative Stress <math display="inline"><semantics> <mrow> <mi>Z</mi> </mrow> </semantics></math>-score), and histological parameters (Neurodegeneration <math display="inline"><semantics> <mrow> <mi>Z</mi> </mrow> </semantics></math>-score).</p>
Full article ">
19 pages, 3204 KiB  
Article
ADAS Alarm Sound Design for Autonomous Vehicles Based on Local Optimization: A Case Study in Shanghai, China
by Jun Ma, Yuanyang Zuo, Octave Jolimoy, Zaiyan Gong and Wenxia Xu
Appl. Sci. 2024, 14(22), 10733; https://doi.org/10.3390/app142210733 - 20 Nov 2024
Viewed by 351
Abstract
Alarm sounds significantly influence a user’s sensory perception while driving, directly affecting driving judgement and safety. Personal experience and the environment play an important role in information cognition, but they are rarely considered in the current warning design. We propose a methodology enabling [...] Read more.
Alarm sounds significantly influence a user’s sensory perception while driving, directly affecting driving judgement and safety. Personal experience and the environment play an important role in information cognition, but they are rarely considered in the current warning design. We propose a methodology enabling engineers and designers to locally optimize the advanced driver-assistance system (ADAS) functions and applied it to the Shanghainese ecosystem to improve performance. The alarm sound content is studied and sorted out to conduct user research and spatial sound collection evaluation. Local optimization and the subdivision of data are carried out to generate a user perception set on which the experimental tests and evaluation analysis are implemented. The framework increases the overall efficiency of auditory warning systems and minimizes Human–Machine Interface misunderstandings, thus providing the optimal security scheme for users. Full article
Show Figures

Figure 1

Figure 1
<p>Sound map of Shanghai with different sounds represented (The symbols in the figure represent different sound type classifications).</p>
Full article ">Figure 2
<p>Graph of sound map and OEM (PSA) sound frequency composition.</p>
Full article ">Figure 3
<p>Oscillogram of study sounds showing 2 DoE parameters.</p>
Full article ">Figure 4
<p>The results of PCA: (<b>a</b>) PCA applied to the Franco-Chinese dataset showing groups and consensus (AN output); (<b>b</b>) biplot showing sounds and variables project on 2D PCA plan (3 outputs).</p>
Full article ">Figure 5
<p>(<b>a</b>) Outside view of the simulator; (<b>b</b>) picture of an ongoing simulation.</p>
Full article ">Figure 6
<p>Pictograms added to a simulated head-up display.</p>
Full article ">Figure 7
<p>Procedures.</p>
Full article ">
12 pages, 589 KiB  
Article
Distinguishing Doors and Floors on All Fours: Landmarks as Tools for Vertical Navigation Learning in Domestic Dogs (Canis familiaris)
by Lila Muscosky and Alexandra Horowitz
Animals 2024, 14(22), 3316; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani14223316 - 18 Nov 2024
Viewed by 637
Abstract
Spatial navigation allows animals to understand their environment position and is crucial to survival. An animal’s primary mode of spatial navigation (horizontal or vertical) is dependent on how they naturally move in space. Observations of the domestic dog (Canis familiaris) have [...] Read more.
Spatial navigation allows animals to understand their environment position and is crucial to survival. An animal’s primary mode of spatial navigation (horizontal or vertical) is dependent on how they naturally move in space. Observations of the domestic dog (Canis familiaris) have shown that they, like other terrestrial animals, navigate poorly in vertical space. This deficit is visible in their use of multi-story buildings. To date, no research has been conducted to determine if dogs can learn how to navigate in an anthropogenic vertical environment with the help of a landmark. As such, we herein investigate the effect of the addition of a visual or olfactory landmark on dogs’ ability to identify when they are on their home floor. Subject behaviors toward their home door and a contrasting floor door were compared before and after exposure to a landmark outside of their home door. While subjects initially showed no difference in latency to approach an apartment door on their home or a wrong floor, we found a significant difference in latency to approach the doors in the test trials for subjects who approached the doors in every trial. Other findings are equivocal, but this result is consistent with the hypothesis that dogs can learn to navigate in vertical space. Full article
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Second Edition: Research on the Human–Companion Animal Relationship)
Show Figures

Figure 1

Figure 1
<p>Example layout of an apartment building floor with the subject’s home floor (<b>A</b>) and a floor upstairs from the subject’s home floor, the wrong floor (<b>B</b>). In this example, the subject is presented with the visual landmark, the umbrella stand. The landmark is placed next to the subject’s home door. The camera is on a tripod and is situated to film the same door on both floors, out of the way from the subject. The experimenter (E) is hidden from view of the subject and owner.</p>
Full article ">Figure 2
<p>Examples of (<b>A</b>) approach, (<b>B</b>) look at owner, (<b>C</b>) sniff door, and (<b>D</b>) sniff landmark.</p>
Full article ">Figure 3
<p>Mean (+/− SD) approach latency to the apartment door on the home and wrong floors of the building by dogs before (baseline) and after (test) they have time to learn a landmark placed outside of their door (* <span class="html-italic">p</span> = 0.03, NS: not significant).</p>
Full article ">Figure 4
<p>Percentage of dogs to approach the home and wrong floors of their building (<span class="html-italic">n</span> = 29) before (baseline) and after (test) they have time to learn a landmark placed outside of their door (* <span class="html-italic">p</span> = 0.03, NS: not significant).</p>
Full article ">
20 pages, 1549 KiB  
Article
The Influence of Music Reading on Spatial Working Memory and Self-Assessment Accuracy
by Michel A. Cara
Brain Sci. 2024, 14(11), 1152; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14111152 - 17 Nov 2024
Viewed by 609
Abstract
Background/Objectives: Previous research has suggested that Western musicians, who generally demonstrate proficiency in reading musical scores, exhibit superior performance in visuospatial working memory tasks compared to non-musicians. Evidence indicates brain activation in regions such as the left inferior parietal lobe and the right [...] Read more.
Background/Objectives: Previous research has suggested that Western musicians, who generally demonstrate proficiency in reading musical scores, exhibit superior performance in visuospatial working memory tasks compared to non-musicians. Evidence indicates brain activation in regions such as the left inferior parietal lobe and the right posterior fusiform gyrus during music reading, which are associated with visuospatial processing. This study aimed to explore how musical training influences spatial working memory and to examine the relationship between self-assessment accuracy and cognitive performance. Methods: A visuospatial working memory test, the Corsi block-tapping test (CBT), was administered to 70 participants, including 35 musicians with experience in music reading and 35 non-musicians. CBT performances were compared between groups, controlling for sex and age differences using analysis of covariance. Participants were also asked to self-assess their visuospatial capabilities. Results: Musicians performed significantly better than non-musicians in the CBT and demonstrated greater metacognitive accuracy in evaluating their visuospatial memory capacities. A total of 46.34% of musicians who claimed good performance on the CBT did in fact perform well, in comparison with 14.63% of non-musicians. Sex influenced the outcomes of spatial working memory, while age did not significantly affect performance. Conclusions: This self-awareness of visuospatial capabilities reflects a form of metacompetence, encompassing reflective thinking and the ability to assess one’s cognitive skills. Furthermore, while differences in spatial working memory between musicians and non-musicians appear to be related to executive functions associated with general music practice, further investigation is needed to explore other potential influences beyond musical experience. Full article
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Advances in Spatial Vision and Visual Perception)
Show Figures

Figure 1

Figure 1
<p>Example of a 3-block sequence in the CBT. The blocks light up in red in the order (1), (2), (3), as shown in panels (<b>a</b>–<b>c</b>). The task is considered correctly completed if the participant clicks on the blocks in the same order. However, if the participant does not recall the full sequence but clicks at least one block in the correct position within the sequence, such as (2), (1), (3) (e.g., panels <b>b</b>,<b>c</b>,<b>a</b>), the response would be considered partially correct, with the third block correctly identified. To advance to the next level, which involves a 4-block sequence, the participant must correctly complete the full sequence in at least one of three attempts.</p>
Full article ">Figure 2
<p>Self-evaluation of metacompetences in musicians and non-musicians. Bars represent the self-assessment of participants: left—claimed to have good visuospatial capabilities and obtained good performance on the CBT (musicians 46.34% and non-musicians 14.63%); right—claimed to have good visuospatial capabilities and obtained poor performances on the CBT (musicians 9.76% and non-musicians 29.27%).</p>
Full article ">Figure 3
<p>Results on the Corsi block-tapping test as a function of years of music reading practice: 54.84% of participants scored under 7 points (<span class="html-italic">M</span> = 6.44, <span class="html-italic">SD</span> = 0.34); 32.26% scored between 7 and 8 points (<span class="html-italic">M</span> = 7.48, <span class="html-italic">SD</span> = 0.10); and 12.9% scored over 8 points (<span class="html-italic">M</span> = 8.36, <span class="html-italic">SD</span> = 0.06). The black circles represent individual observations from different participants in the visuospatial task.</p>
Full article ">Figure A1
<p>Corsi block-tapping test’s experimental display. The coordinates (in cm) are measured from the center of each figure.</p>
Full article ">
18 pages, 11109 KiB  
Article
Effects of Sarcosine (N-methylglycine) on NMDA (N-methyl-D-aspartate) Receptor Hypofunction Induced by MK801: In Vivo Calcium Imaging in the CA1 Region of the Dorsal Hippocampus
by Yi-Tse Hsiao, Ching-Yuan Chang, Ting-Yen Lee, Wan-Ting Liao, Wen-Sung Lai and Fang-Chia Chang
Brain Sci. 2024, 14(11), 1150; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14111150 - 16 Nov 2024
Viewed by 650
Abstract
Background: Hypofunction of the glutamate system in the brain is one of the pathophysiological hypotheses for schizophrenia. Accumulating animal and clinical studies show that sarcosine (N-methylglycine), a glycine transporter-1 inhibitor, is effective in ameliorating the negative and cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia. The aims [...] Read more.
Background: Hypofunction of the glutamate system in the brain is one of the pathophysiological hypotheses for schizophrenia. Accumulating animal and clinical studies show that sarcosine (N-methylglycine), a glycine transporter-1 inhibitor, is effective in ameliorating the negative and cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia. The aims of the present study were to observe the effects of sarcosine on neuronal activity in the dorsal CA1 (dCA1) hippocampal neurons within an NMDA receptor hypofunction model induced by MK801. Methods: We applied in vivo calcium imaging to observe the dynamics of fluorescence from the dCA1 hippocampal neurons when the mice were exploring in an open field. Using this tool, we directly measured and compared neuronal properties between sarcosine-treated and untreated mice. At the same time, the physiological function of the neurons was also quantified by measuring their place fields. Results: Our data demonstrated that MK-801 (0.2 mg/kg) diminished the fluorescence intensity of dCA1 neurons that had been genetically modified with a calcium indicator. MK-801 also significantly increased the correlation coefficient between the fluorescence dynamics of pairs of cells, a feature that may be linked to the symptom of disorganization in human patients with schizophrenia. The spatial correlations of place fields in the mice were impaired by MK-801 as well. Injected sarcosine (500 mg or 1000 mg/kg) significantly alleviated the abovementioned abnormalities. Conclusions: Our data provide evidence to support the use of sarcosine to alleviate symptoms of schizophrenia, especially hippocampus-related functions. Full article
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Advanced Clinical Diagnosis, Evaluation, and Treatment of Psychosis)
Show Figures

Figure 1

Figure 1
<p>Examples of fluorescence intensity of dorsal CA1 neurons from mouse #3 after administration of MK-801 and sarcosine. (<b>A</b>) Histology slice for the location of GRIN lens implantation. (<b>B</b>) Experimental schedule of a session. The animal was explored in an identical open field in trials A and B. The drugs were injected before trial B. (<b>C</b>) Stacked image that was background-subtracted and movement-corrected (MC) by MIN1PIPE [<a href="#B26-brainsci-14-01150" class="html-bibr">26</a>] of one recording session. The unit of the traces are normalized “processed calcium traces (PCT)”, which are normalized calcium signals after neural enhancement and denoising by MIN1PIPE (<b>D</b>) Examples of fluorescence traces. (<b>E</b>–<b>H</b>) Example of fluorescence intensities of the same animal under the treatment of PBS + PBS, MK-801 + PBS, MK-801 + sarcosine (SAR500, 500 mg/kg), and MK-801 + sarcosine (SAR1000, 1000 mg/kg). (<b>I</b>) Mean delta fluorescence intensities (trial B–A) of neurons of all animals (Mean ± SEM) (Each single cell was treated as a sample for bootstrapping before statistical analysis). **: vs. PBS + PBS; ##: vs MK-801 + PBS; %%: vs MK-801 + SAR500 indicates significant difference (<span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.01), as determined using the Bootstrap method followed by Tukey’s post hoc test for multiple comparisons.</p>
Full article ">Figure 2
<p>Fluorescence intensity ratio of trial B/trial A of grouped cell ensembles. (<b>A</b>–<b>C</b>) Cumulative distribution of the intensity ratio which is randomly sampled and averaged 20 intensity traces 1000 times in mice #1 to #3, respectively. The manipulations of PBS + PBS, MK-801 + PBS, MK-801 + SAR500, and MK-801 + SAR1000 are represented by black, gray, red, and blue lines. (<b>D</b>–<b>F</b>) Intensity ratio, which is randomly selected and averaged 30 intensity traces 1000 times. (<b>G</b>–<b>I</b>) Intensity ratio, which is randomly selected and averaged 50 intensity traces 1000 times.</p>
Full article ">Figure 3
<p>Examples of dorsal CA1 cell–cell correlation matrixes from mouse #3. (<b>A</b>–<b>D</b>) Cell–cell correlation matrixes after the administration of PBS + PBS, MK-801 + PBS, MK-801 + SAR500, and MK-801 + SAR1000, respectively. (<b>E</b>) Delta correlation coefficient (trial B–trial A) of pairwise neurons of all animals (Mean ± SEM) (Each single cell was treated as a sample for bootstrapping before statistical analysis). **: vs. PBS + PBS; ##: vs. MK-801 + PBS; %%: vs. MK-801 + SAR500 indicates significant difference (<span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.01), as determined using the Bootstrap method followed by Tukey’s post hoc test for multiple comparisons.</p>
Full article ">Figure 4
<p>Examples of maximum fluorescence intensity maps from a fluorescence-positive cell in dorsal CA1. (<b>A</b>) Maximum fluorescence intensity maps of an example dorsal CA1 fluorescence positive cell are depicted by colors, where warm colors represent strong fluorescence signals and white represents weak signals. The gray line represents the walking trajectory of the animal (mouse #1). (<b>B</b>) Spatial correlation between trial A and trial B in neurons from all animals (Mean ± SEM) (Each single cell was treated as a sample for bootstrapping before statistical analysis). **: vs. PBS + PBS; ##: vs. MK-801 + PBS; %%: vs. MK-801 + SAR500 indicates significant difference (<span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.01), as determined using the Bootstrap method followed by Tukey’s post hoc test for multiple comparisons.</p>
Full article ">
30 pages, 1866 KiB  
Article
Key Factors Influencing Design Learners’ Behavioral Intention in Human-AI Collaboration Within the Educational Metaverse
by Ronghui Wu, Lin Gao, Jiaxin Li, Qianghong Huang and Younghwan Pan
Sustainability 2024, 16(22), 9942; https://doi.org/10.3390/su16229942 - 14 Nov 2024
Viewed by 509
Abstract
This study investigates the key factors which influence design learners’ behavioral intention to collaborate with AI in the educational metaverse (EMH-AIc). Engaging design learners in EMH-AIc enhances learning efficiency, personalizes learning experiences, and supports equitable and sustainable design education. However, limited research has [...] Read more.
This study investigates the key factors which influence design learners’ behavioral intention to collaborate with AI in the educational metaverse (EMH-AIc). Engaging design learners in EMH-AIc enhances learning efficiency, personalizes learning experiences, and supports equitable and sustainable design education. However, limited research has focused on these influencing factors, leading to a lack of theoretical grounding for user behavior in this context. Drawing on social cognitive theory (SCT), this study constructs a three-dimensional theoretical model comprising the external environment, individual cognition, and behavior, validated within an EMH-AIc setting. By using Spatial.io’s Apache Art Studio as the experimental platform and analyzing data from 533 design learners with SPSS 27.0, SmartPLS 4.0, and partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM), this study identifies those rewards, teacher support, and facilitating conditions in the external environment, with self-efficacy, outcome expectation, and trust in cognition also significantly influencing behavioral intention. Additionally, individual cognition mediates the relationship between the external environment and behavioral intention. This study not only extends SCT application within the educational metaverse but also provides actionable insights for optimizing design learning experiences, contributing to the sustainable development of design education. Full article
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Artificial Intelligence in Education and Sustainable Development)
Show Figures

Figure 1

Figure 1
<p>Apache Art Studio user experience and practical scenarios.</p>
Full article ">Figure 2
<p>Results of PLS structural model.</p>
Full article ">Figure 3
<p>Results of research model (*** <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.001; ** <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.01; * <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.05; n.s. = not significant).</p>
Full article ">
18 pages, 5314 KiB  
Article
The Role of Spatial Ability in Academic Success: The Impact of the Integrated Hybrid Training Program in Architecture and Engineering Higher Education
by Ronen Porat and Ciprian Ceobanu
Educ. Sci. 2024, 14(11), 1237; https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14111237 - 11 Nov 2024
Viewed by 824
Abstract
Spatial ability (SA) is a critical cognitive skill across various disciplines, particularly in architecture and engineering. This research, comprising two main studies, investigates the relationship between SA and academic performance among first-year students. The first study examines the impact of the targeted Integrated [...] Read more.
Spatial ability (SA) is a critical cognitive skill across various disciplines, particularly in architecture and engineering. This research, comprising two main studies, investigates the relationship between SA and academic performance among first-year students. The first study examines the impact of the targeted Integrated Hybrid Training (IHT) SA intervention on the achievements of two main groups: engineering (electrical, software, and chemical) students and architecture students. The results indicate that, while the intervention significantly improved SA, the impact on academic performance varied. Architecture students, whose curriculum relies heavily on spatial reasoning, showed significant gains in courses such as studio design, with higher SA scores correlating with improved grades. Engineering students exhibited SA improvement without corresponding gains in the first-year core courses like calculus and physics. The second study focuses on architecture students who did not receive the intervention, revealing a strong positive correlation between inherent SA and success in architecture-specific courses. Correlation coefficients (r) ranged from 0.46 to 0.67, with adjusted p-values between 0.007 and 0.024, underscoring SA’s importance in architecture. These findings suggest that integrating SA training into architectural education can enhance academic performance, while disciplines might benefit from specialized SA training introduced or expanded in later years. This research contributes to educational theory by demonstrating SA’s impact across disciplines and supports the development of customized SA training programs. Future studies should explore long-term benefits, advanced SA training technologies, and individual differences in response to spatial interventions, providing insights for curriculum development in spatially demanding fields. Full article
Show Figures

Graphical abstract

Graphical abstract
Full article ">Figure 1
<p>Top row: Example of contour and surface visualization in multivariable functions in calculus. Bottom row: Example of the intersection of geometric shapes with planes in calculus: elliptic paraboloid, ellipsoid, hyperbola, and general two variables function [<a href="#B26-education-14-01237" class="html-bibr">26</a>,<a href="#B27-education-14-01237" class="html-bibr">27</a>,<a href="#B31-education-14-01237" class="html-bibr">31</a>].</p>
Full article ">Figure 2
<p>Examples of architectural spatial layouts: top-down plan, exterior perspective, and sectional view of a building, illustrating both indoor and outdoor environments.</p>
Full article ">Figure 3
<p>Course scores and GPA among the intervention and control groups of architecture students.</p>
Full article ">Figure 4
<p>Course scores and GPA among the intervention and control groups of engineering students.</p>
Full article ">Figure 5
<p>Correlations between SA in the pre-assessment and courses scores/GPA in Architecture control group.</p>
Full article ">
20 pages, 1754 KiB  
Hypothesis
Environmental Affordance for Physical Activity, Neurosustainability, and Brain Health: Quantifying the Built Environment’s Ability to Sustain BDNF Release by Reaching Metabolic Equivalents (METs)
by Mohamed Hesham Khalil
Brain Sci. 2024, 14(11), 1133; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14111133 - 10 Nov 2024
Viewed by 768
Abstract
Background/Objectives: Unlike enriched environments for rodents, human-built environments often hinder neuroplasticity through sedentary lifestyles, to which exercise can merely overcome its adverse effects. This paper introduces "environmental affordance for physical activity" to quantify the potential of spatial layout designs to stimulate activity and [...] Read more.
Background/Objectives: Unlike enriched environments for rodents, human-built environments often hinder neuroplasticity through sedentary lifestyles, to which exercise can merely overcome its adverse effects. This paper introduces "environmental affordance for physical activity" to quantify the potential of spatial layout designs to stimulate activity and sustain neuroplasticity, mainly hippocampal neurogenesis. Methods: A novel framework links metabolic equivalents (METs) that can be afforded by the spatial layout of the built environment to its role in increasing the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF)—a biomarker that promotes and sustains adult hippocampal neurogenesis and synaptic plasticity. Equations are developed to assess the built environment’s affordance for physical activity through BDNF changes measurable after brief exposure to the built environment for 20–35 min. Results: The developed equations are evidenced to be feasible to cause BDNF release through low- to moderate-intensity physical activity. This model provides a feasible assessment tool to test the built environment’s effectiveness towards neurosustainability. Conclusions: By sustaining neurogenesis, the environmental affordance for physical activity holds promise for improving mental health and preventing cognitive decline. Full article
(This article belongs to the Section Environmental Neuroscience)
Show Figures

Figure 1

Figure 1
<p>Environmental affordance for physical activity as a sustainable model for neuroplasticity.</p>
Full article ">Figure 2
<p>Molecular mechanisms through which BDNF regulates neurogenesis and plasticity by physical activity. Environmental affordance for physical activity increases BDNF levels, which in turn activates the necessary pathways responsible for adult hippocampal neurogenesis and neuroplasticity processes.</p>
Full article ">Figure 3
<p>Mechanisms and methods of testing environmental affordance for physical activity. Environmental affordance for physical activity increases growth factors and BDNF (saturated through the blood-brain-barrier) that regulate adult hippocampal neurogenesis and synaptic integration, which leads to a long-term increase in hippocampal volume.</p>
Full article ">Figure 4
<p>Framework and potential mixed methods for exploring visual environmental agents.</p>
Full article ">Figure 5
<p>Summary of environmental affordance for physical activity mechanisms for adult hippocampal neurosustainability.</p>
Full article ">
18 pages, 982 KiB  
Review
Remote Sensing and GIS in Natural Resource Management: Comparing Tools and Emphasizing the Importance of In-Situ Data
by Sanjeev Sharma, Justin O. Beslity, Lindsey Rustad, Lacy J. Shelby, Peter T. Manos, Puskar Khanal, Andrew B. Reinmann and Churamani Khanal
Remote Sens. 2024, 16(22), 4161; https://doi.org/10.3390/rs16224161 - 8 Nov 2024
Viewed by 1206
Abstract
Remote sensing (RS) and Geographic Information Systems (GISs) provide significant opportunities for monitoring and managing natural resources across various temporal, spectral, and spatial resolutions. There is a critical need for natural resource managers to understand the expanding capabilities of image sources, analysis techniques, [...] Read more.
Remote sensing (RS) and Geographic Information Systems (GISs) provide significant opportunities for monitoring and managing natural resources across various temporal, spectral, and spatial resolutions. There is a critical need for natural resource managers to understand the expanding capabilities of image sources, analysis techniques, and in situ validation methods. This article reviews key image analysis tools in natural resource management, highlighting their unique strengths across diverse applications such as agriculture, forestry, water resources, soil management, and natural hazard monitoring. Google Earth Engine (GEE), a cloud-based platform introduced in 2010, stands out for its vast geospatial data catalog and scalability, making it ideal for global-scale analysis and algorithm development. ENVI, known for advanced multi- and hyperspectral image processing, excels in vegetation monitoring, environmental analysis, and feature extraction. ERDAS IMAGINE specializes in radar data analysis and LiDAR processing, offering robust classification and terrain analysis capabilities. Global Mapper is recognized for its versatility, supporting over 300 data formats and excelling in 3D visualization and point cloud processing, especially in UAV applications. eCognition leverages object-based image analysis (OBIA) to enhance classification accuracy by grouping pixels into meaningful objects, making it effective in environmental monitoring and urban planning. Lastly, QGIS integrates these remote sensing tools with powerful spatial analysis functions, supporting decision-making in sustainable resource management. Together, these tools when paired with in situ data provide comprehensive solutions for managing and analyzing natural resources across scales. Full article
Show Figures

Figure 1

Figure 1
<p>Articles published using different image analysis tools in different time intervals.</p>
Full article ">Figure 2
<p>Map of sites identified and included in database.</p>
Full article ">
19 pages, 38208 KiB  
Article
Cognitive Recognition of Space and Social Connections of Traditional Villages in Shanxi Province: A Case Study of Ding, Shijiagou, and Yanjing Villages
by Cong Sun and Inhee Lee
Sustainability 2024, 16(22), 9695; https://doi.org/10.3390/su16229695 - 7 Nov 2024
Viewed by 436
Abstract
The traditional clan-based structure of village society is gradually disintegrating due to rural population outflow and excessive tourism development. The construction of a diversified development model oriented toward the needs of local villagers is key to sustainable development and the preservation of cultural [...] Read more.
The traditional clan-based structure of village society is gradually disintegrating due to rural population outflow and excessive tourism development. The construction of a diversified development model oriented toward the needs of local villagers is key to sustainable development and the preservation of cultural heritage. In this study, a case analysis of the Ding, Shijiagou, and Yanjing Villages in Shanxi Province was conducted. The research approach of “cognitive recognition–spatial structure–social connections” was used to integrate cognitive maps and space syntax with social network analysis to quantitatively analyze the cognitive elements in these villages, with the establishment of a collective cognitive map, global integration value maps, and villagers’ intention element cognitive networks based on functional types. The spatial cognition patterns and social network structures of these villages were explored and analyzed in relation to the complex interactions between villages from the perspectives of spatial cognitive preference, spatial accessibility, and social interaction to compensate for the limitations of using a single analytical approach. Differences in village cognition patterns were found—the efficient use of different types of space is influenced by spatial cognitive preferences; the degree of spatial integration affects the distribution of functional spaces, with high-integration areas tending to overlap with high-cognition spaces and featuring strong accessibility and connectivity; the density and stability of social networks significantly influence spatial cognition and the interaction patterns in villages; high-density networks enhance the frequency of interactions between villagers and the effectiveness of space utilization; and differentiation in spatial cognition among different groups may reduce village space integration, thereby influencing the overall social functions and cultural heritage of the village. Therefore, it is crucial to enhance the connectivity and influence of key nodes, optimize spatial layouts and social network structures, and introduce modern elements while preserving traditional culture, an approach that can promote social interaction and spatial vitality in the villages. This study provides a scientific basis and serves as a reference for the construction of sustainable development models for villages. Full article
Show Figures

Figure 1

Figure 1
<p>Conceptual framework.</p>
Full article ">Figure 2
<p>Map of the study area. In all figures in this study, the map of China is drawn according to the standard map of GS (2023) 2627 downloaded from the Standard Map Service website of the National Administration of Surveying, Mapping and Geoinformation.</p>
Full article ">
Back to TopTop