Second Language Trajectories in Immigrant Children: Latent Class Growth Analysis
<p>The means of the language measures at the four measurement points (T1–T4) for the four classes (C1–C4). The language tests at T1 and T2: <span class="html-italic">Sprachentwicklungstest für zweijährige Kinder</span> (<a href="#B19-education-15-00163" class="html-bibr">Grimm, 2000</a>); the language tests at T3 and T4: <span class="html-italic">Sprachstandserhebungstest für Kinder im Alter zwischen 5 und 10 Jahren</span> (<a href="#B54-education-15-00163" class="html-bibr">Petermann, 2010</a>). Due to the change in instruments between T2 and T3, discontinuous piecewise LCGA with two growth models for each German test, but one common class parameter, was conducted. The language mean scores represent the proportion of attained scores relative to the possible score (values ranged from 0 = no points attained to 1 = all points attained) for each language test separately.</p> "> Figure A1
<p>Illustration of estimated means for class C1 (improvement to low-level proficiency) comparing NMAR (stayers and dropouts) and MAR solutions.</p> "> Figure A2
<p>Illustration of estimated means for class C2 (improvement to medium-level proficiency) comparing NMAR (stayers and dropouts) and MAR solutions.</p> "> Figure A3
<p>Illustration of estimated means for class C3 (improvement to high-level proficiency) comparing NMAR (stayers and dropouts) and MAR solutions.</p> "> Figure A4
<p>Illustration of estimated means for class C3 (permanent high-level proficiency) comparing NMAR (stayers and dropouts) and MAR solutions.</p> ">
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Patterns of Second Language Acquisition
1.2. Predictors of Second Language Trajectories
1.3. The Current Study
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Language Measures
2.2.2. Predictors
2.3. Analytic Strategy
3. Results
3.1. Research Question 1: Latent Classes of Growth Trajectories
3.2. Research Question 2: Predicting Class Membership
4. Discussion
4.1. Patterns of Second Language Acquisition
4.2. Predictors of Second Language Patterns
4.3. Implications
4.4. Strengths, Limitations, and Future Research
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Predictor | M (SD)/n (%) T2 Participated (n = 295–325) | M (SD)/n (%) T2 Dropped Out (n = 96–118) | M (SD)/n (%) T3 Participated (n = 250–278) | M (SD)/n (%) T3 Dropped Out (n = 140–165) | M (SD)/n (%) T4 Participated (n = 202–222) | M (SD)/n (%) T4 Dropped Out (n = 195–221) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
T1 Age (in months) | 42.03 (4.26) | 41.47 (4.17) | 41.90 (4.16) | 41.87 (4.37) | 41.76 (4.14) | 42.02 (4.33) |
SES | 0.02 (0.87) | −0.13 (0.82) | 0.05 (0.86) | −0.15 (0.84) | 0.09 (0.85) | −0.13 (0.85) |
T1 Duration (years) of parents’ residence in Switzerland | 13.86 (9.03) | 13.69 (7.96) | 14.01 (9.05) | 13.49 (8.24) | 14.45 (9.07) | 13.17 (8.40) |
T1 Contact with German speaking persons | 2.25 (1.02) | 2.09 (1.09) | 2.30 (1.01) | 2.05 (1.07) | 2.37 (1.01) | 2.05 (1.04) |
T1 Abstract thinking | 4.92 (2.41) | 4.50 (2.46) | 4.96 (2.43) | 4.54 (2.41) | 4.93 (2.40) | 4.68 (2.40) |
Gender (female) | 157 (48.31) | 64 (54.24) | 134 (48.20) | 87 (52.73) | 116 (52.25) | 105 (47.51) |
Attendance of early childcare before T1 (yes) | 195 (60.37) | 64 (54.71) | 171 (61.96) | 88 (53.66) | 139 (63.18) | 120 (54.55) |
Familial language: mainly other than German (yes) | 275 (85.14) | 99 (84.62) | 235 (85.15) | 139 (84.76) | 183 (83.18) | 191 (86.82) |
Appendix B
Appendix C
Predictor Variable | Profile C1 (23%) | Profile C3 (27%) | Profile C4 (11%) | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
b | SE | OR | p | b | SE | OR | p | b | SE | OR | p | |
Intercept | 0.74 | 3.11 | 0.81 | −4.48 | 1.72 | <0.01 | −16.91 | 4.13 | <0.001 | |||
T1 Age | 0.08 | 0.07 | 1.08 | 0.27 | 0.06 | 0.04 | 1.06 | 0.18 | 0.26 | 0.09 | 1.29 | <0.01 |
Gender | 0.21 | 0.54 | 1.23 | 0.70 | 0.23 | 0.32 | 1.26 | 0.47 | 0.30 | 0.50 | 1.35 | 0.55 |
T1 Abstract thinking | −0.30 | 0.19 | 0.74 | 0.12 | 0.09 | 0.08 | 1.09 | 0.25 | 0.33 | 0.12 | 1.39 | <0.01 |
T1 duration of parents’ residence in Switzerland | −0.15 | 0.06 | 0.86 | <0.01 | 0.01 | 0.02 | 1.01 | 0.81 | 0.05 | 0.03 | 1.05 | 0.07 |
T1 Contact with German speakers | 0.00 | 0.25 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 0.52 | 0.16 | 1.63 | <0.01 | 0.72 | 0.29 | 2.05 | <0.05 |
Attendance of early childcare before T1 | −2.00 | 0.65 | 0.14 | <0.01 | 0.78 | 0.39 | 2.18 | <0.05 | 1.86 | 0.78 | 6.45 | <0.05 |
Familial language: mainly other than German | −0.74 | 1.08 | 0.48 | 0.49 | −0.60 | 0.53 | 0.55 | 0.25 | −2.15 | 0.73 | 0.12 | <0.01 |
SES | −0.14 | 0.36 | 0.87 | 0.70 | 0.26 | 0.22 | 1.30 | 0.23 | 1.13 | 0.33 | 3.09 | <0.01 |
Predictor Variable | Profile C1 (23%) | Profile C2 (38%) | Profile C4 (11%) | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
b | SE | OR | p | b | SE | OR | p | b | SE | OR | p | |
Intercept | 5.23 | 2.63 | <0.05 | 4.48 | 1.72 | <0.01 | −12.43 | 4.14 | <0.01 | |||
T1 Age | −0.02 | 0.06 | 1.02 | 0.73 | −0.06 | 0.04 | 0.95 | 0.18 | 0.20 | 0.09 | 1.22 | <0.05 |
Gender | −0.02 | 0.49 | 0.98 | 0.96 | −0.23 | 0.32 | 0.79 | 0.47 | 0.07 | 0.51 | 1.07 | 0.90 |
T1 Abstract thinking | −0.39 | 0.16 | 0.68 | <0.05 | −0.09 | 0.08 | 0.92 | 0.25 | 0.24 | 0.12 | 1.27 | 0.05 |
T1 duration of parents’ residence in Switzerland | −0.16 | 0.05 | 0.86 | <0.01 | −0.01 | 0.02 | 1.00 | 0.81 | 0.05 | 0.03 | 1.05 | 0.11 |
T1 Contact with German speakers | −0.52 | 0.24 | 0.59 | <0.05 | −0.52 | 0.16 | 0.59 | <0.01 | 0.20 | 0.29 | 1.22 | 0.50 |
Attendance of early childcare before T1 | −2.78 | 0.59 | 0.06 | <0.001 | −0.78 | 0.39 | 0.46 | <0.05 | 1.09 | 0.80 | 2.97 | 0.17 |
Familial language: mainly other than German | −0.14 | 0.84 | 0.87 | 0.87 | 0.60 | 0.53 | 1.83 | 0.25 | −1.55 | 0.70 | 0.21 | <0.05 |
SES | −0.40 | 0.32 | 0.67 | 0.21 | −0.26 | 0.22 | 0.77 | 0.23 | 0.87 | 0.33 | 2.38 | <0.01 |
Predictor Variable | Profile C1 (23%) | Profile C2 (38%) | Profile C3 (27%) | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
b | SE | OR | p | b | SE | OR | p | b | SE | OR | p | |
Intercept | 17.66 | 4.77 | <0.001 | 16.91 | 4.13 | <0.001 | 12.43 | 4.13 | <0.01 | |||
T1 Age | −0.18 | 0.10 | 0.84 | 0.08 | −0.26 | 0.09 | 0.77 | <0.01 | −0.20 | 0.09 | 0.82 | <0.05 |
Gender | −0.09 | 0.64 | 0.92 | 0.89 | −0.30 | 0.50 | 0.74 | 0.55 | −0.07 | 0.51 | 0.94 | 0.90 |
T1 Abstract thinking | −0.63 | 0.20 | 0. 53 | <0.01 | −0.33 | 0.12 | 0.72 | <0.01 | −0.24 | 0.12 | 0.79 | 0.05 |
T1 duration of parents’ residence in Switzerland | −0.20 | 0.06 | 0.82 | <0.001 | −0.05 | 0.03 | 0.95 | 0.07 | −0.05 | 0.03 | 0.95 | 0.11 |
T1 Contact with German speakers | −0.72 | 0.35 | 0.49 | <0.05 | −0.72 | 0.29 | 0.49 | <0.05 | −0.20 | 0.29 | 0.82 | 0.50 |
Attendance of early childcare before T1 | −3.86 | 0.91 | 0.02 | <0.001 | −2.15 | 0.73 | 0.16 | <0.01 | −1.09 | 0.80 | 0.34 | 0.17 |
Familial language: mainly other than German | 1.41 | 1.03 | 4.09 | 0.17 | 2.15 | 0.73 | 8.60 | <0.01 | 1.55 | 0.70 | 4.70 | <0.05 |
SES | −1.27 | 0.43 | 0.28 | <0.01 | −1.23 | 0.33 | 0.32 | <0.01 | −0.87 | 0.22 | 0.42 | <0.01 |
Appendix D
Class 1 (Improvement to Low-Level Proficiency) | Class 2 (Improvement to Medium-Level Proficiency) | Class 3 (Improvement to High-Level Proficiency) | Class 4 (Permanent High-Level Proficiency) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age T1 | Low | 0.32 | 0.52 | 0.17 | 0.00 |
Mean | 0.38 | 0.44 | 0.18 | 0.01 | |
High | 0.43 | 0.36 | 0.19 | 0.01 | |
Gender | Male | 0.38 | 0.44 | 0.18 | 0.01 |
Female | 0.41 | 0.39 | 0.20 | 0.01 | |
Abstract thinking T1 | Low | 0.57 | 0.32 | 0.11 | 0.00 |
Mean | 0.38 | 0.44 | 0.18 | 0.01 | |
High | 0.21 | 0.51 | 0.26 | 0.01 | |
Duration of parents’ residence in Switzerland T1 | Low | 0.70 | 0.22 | 0.08 | 0.00 |
Mean | 0.38 | 0.44 | 0.18 | 0.01 | |
High | 0.14 | 0.59 | 0.27 | 0.01 | |
Contact with German speakers T1 | Low | 0.41 | 0.48 | 0.12 | 0.00 |
Mean | 0.38 | 0.44 | 0.18 | 0.01 | |
High | 0.33 | 0.39 | 0.27 | 0.01 | |
Attendance of early childcare before T1 | No | 0.38 | 0.44 | 0.18 | 0.01 |
Yes | 0.06 | 0.48 | 0.43 | 0.04 | |
Familial language | Mainly German | 0.49 | 0.27 | 0.21 | 0.03 |
Mainly other than German | 0.38 | 0.44 | 0.18 | 0.01 | |
SES | Low | 0.42 | 0.43 | 0.14 | 0.00 |
Mean | 0.38 | 0.44 | 0.18 | 0.01 | |
High | 0.33 | 0.43 | 0.22 | 0.01 |
References
- Adesope, O. O., Lavin, T., Thompson, T., & Ungerleider, C. (2010). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Review of Educational Research, 80(2), 207–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. O. (2014). Auxiliary variables in mixture modeling: 3-step approaches using mplus. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 21, 329–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aukrust, V. G., & Rydland, V. (2011). Preschool classroom conversations as long-term resources for second language and literacy acquisition. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 32(4), 198–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Becker, R. (2010). Soziale ungleichheit im schweizer bildungssystem und was man dagegen tun könnte. In H.-U. Grunder, & M. Neuenschwander (Hrsg.) (Eds.), Schulübergang und selektion—Forschungsbefunde, praxisbeispiele, umsetzungsperspektiven (pp. 91–108). Rüegger. Available online: https://boris.unibe.ch/4235/ (accessed on 13 June 2024).
- Berlin, K. S., Parra, G. R., & Williams, N. A. (2014). An introduction to latent variable mixture modeling (part 2): Longitudinal latent class growth analysis and growth mixture models. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 39(2), 188–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, E. (2017). The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin, 143(3), 233–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bihler, L.-M., Agache, A., Kohl, K., Willard, J. A., & Leyendecker, B. (2018). Factor analysis of the classroom assessment scoring system replicates the three domain structure and reveals no support for the bifactor model in german preschools. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bockmann, A.-K., Sachse, S., & Buschmann, A. (2020). Sprachentwicklung im überblick. In S. Sachse, A.-K. Bockmann, & A. Buschmann (Hrsg.) (Eds.), Sprachentwicklung (pp. 3–44). Springer. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chomsky, N. (2007). New horizons in the study of language and mind (7. print). Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Collins, B. A., O’Connor, E. E., Suárez-Orozco, C., Nieto-Castañon, A., & Toppelberg, C. O. (2014). Dual language profiles of Latino children of immigrants: Stability and change over the early school years. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(3), 581–620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cummins, J. (1979). Linguistic interdependence and the educational development of bilingual children. Review of Educational Research, 49(2), 222–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cummins, J. (2016). Reflections on cummins (1980), “The cross-lingual dimensions of language proficiency: Implications for bilingual education and the optimal age issue”. TESOL Quarterly, 50(4), 940–944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., & Weisleder, A. (2013). SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science, 16(2), 234–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fernández, C., & Ortega, C. (2008). Labor market assimilation of immigrants in Spain: Employment at the expense of bad job-matches? Spanish Economic Review, 10(2), 83–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fibla, L., Kosie, J. E., Kircher, R., Lew-Williams, C., & Byers-Heinlein, K. (2022). Bilingual language development in infancy: What can we do to support bilingual families? Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 9(1), 35–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Francot, R., Blom, E., Broekhuizen, M., & Leseman, P. (2021). Profiles of bilingualism in early childhood: A person-centred latent profile transition approach. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 24(3), 569–582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gonzalez, A. M., Steele, J. R., & Baron, A. S. (2017). Reducing children’s implicit racial bias through exposure to positive out-group exemplars. Child Development, 88(1), 123–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goodrich, J. M., & Lonigan, C. J. (2017). Language-independent and language-specific aspects of early literacy: An evaluation of the common underlying proficiency model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(6), 782–793. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grimm, H. (2000). Sprachentwicklungstest für zweijährige Kinder (SETK-2). Hogrefe. [Google Scholar]
- Grob, A., Keller, K., & Trösch, L. M. (2014). ZweitSprache. Mit ausreichenden Deutschkenntnissen in den Kindergarten. Abschlussbericht. University of Basel. [Google Scholar]
- Grosjean, F. (2022). The mysteries of bilingualism. John Wiley & Sons, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Guan, S.-S. A., Greenfield, P. M., & Orellana, M. F. (2014). Translating into understanding: Language brokering and prosocial development in emerging adults from immigrant families. Journal of Adolescent Research, 29(3), 331–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gunnerud, H. L., Ten Braak, D., Reikerås, E. K. L., Donolato, E., & Melby-Lervåg, M. (2020). Is bilingualism related to a cognitive advantage in children? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 146(12), 1059–1083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Halle, T. G., Whittaker, J. V., Zepeda, M., Rothenberg, L., Anderson, R., Daneri, P., Wessel, J., & Buysse, V. (2014). The social–emotional development of dual language learners: Looking back at existing research and moving forward with purpose. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(4), 734–749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hammer, C. S., Burchinal, M., Hong, S. S., LaForett, D. R., Páez, M., Buysse, V., Espinosa, L., Castro, D., & López, L. M. (2020). Change in language and literacy knowledge for Spanish–English dual language learners at school-entry: Analyses from three studies. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 51, 81–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hammer, C. S., Hoff, E., Uchikoshi, Y., Gillanders, C., Castro, D. C., & Sandilos, L. E. (2014). The language and literacy development of young dual language learners: A critical review. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(4), 715–733. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hammer, C. S., Lawrence, F., Rodriguez, B., Davison, M. D., & Miccio, A. W. (2011). Changes in language usage of Puerto Rican mothers and their children: Do gender and timing of exposure to English matter? Applied Psycholinguistics, 32(2), 275–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hammer, K. (2017). Sociocultural integration and second language proficiency following migration. In J.-C. Beacco, H.-J. Krumm, D. Little, & P. Thalgott (Hrsg.) (Eds.), The linguistic integration of adult migrants/L’intégration linguistique des migrants adultes (pp. 91–96). De Gruyter Mouton. [Google Scholar]
- Hirosh, Z., & Degani, T. (2018). Direct and indirect effects of multilingualism on novel language learning: An integrative review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(3), 892–916. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hockema, S. A., & Smith, L. B. (2009). Learning your language, outside-in and inside-out. Linguistics, 47(2), 453–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoff, E. (2013). Interpreting the early language trajectories of children from low-SES and language minority homes: Implications for closing achievement gaps. Developmental Psychology, 49(1), 4–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoff, E. (2018). Bilingual development in children of immigrant families. Child Development Perspectives, 12(2), 80–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Homuth, C., Liebau, E., & Will, G. (2021). The role of socioeconomic, cultural, and structural factors in daycare attendance among refugee children. Journal for Educational Research Online, 2021(1), 16–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jung, T., & Wickrama, K. A. S. (2008). An introduction to latent class growth analysis and growth mixture modeling. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(1), 302–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keller, K. (2009). Relevance of child-care services for bilingual children in Basel [Unpublished master’s thesis]. University of Basel.
- Keller, K., Troesch, L. M., & Grob, A. (2013). Shyness as a risk factor for second language acquisition of immigrant preschoolers. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 34(6), 328–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keller, K., Troesch, L. M., Loher, S., & Grob, A. (2015). Deutschkenntnisse von Kindern statusniedriger und statushoher Einwanderergruppen: Der Einfluss des familialen und extrafamilialen Sprachkontexts. Frühe Bildung, 4(3), 144–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, D.-H., Lambert, R. G., & Burts, D. C. (2018). Are young dual language learners homogeneous? Identifying subgroups using latent class analysis. The Journal of Educational Research, 111(1), 43–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, S.-Y. (2014). Determining the number of latent classes in single- and multiphase growth mixture models. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 21(2), 263–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kim, S.-Y., & Kim, J.-S. (2012). Investigating stage-sequential growth mixture models with multiphase longitudinal data. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 19(2), 293–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kingsbury, M., Findlay, L., Arim, R., & Wei, L. (2021). Differences in child care participation between immigrant and nonimmigrant families. Journal of Childhood Studies, 46(4), 46–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lauro, J., Core, C., & Hoff, E. (2020). Explaining individual differences in trajectories of simultaneous bilingual development: Contributions of child and environmental factors. Child Development, 91(6), 2063–2082. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- López, L. M., & Foster, M. E. (2021). Examining heterogeneity among Latino dual language learners’ school readiness profiles of English and Spanish at the end of Head Start. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 73, 101239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marx, A. E., & Stanat, P. (2012). Reading comprehension of immigrant students in Germany: Research evidence on determinants and target points for intervention. Reading and Writing, 25(8), 1929–1945. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Millán-Franco, M., Gómez-Jacinto, L., Hombrados-Mendieta, I., González-Castro, F., & García-Cid, A. (2019). The effect of length of residence and geographical origin on the social inclusion of immigrants. Psychosocial Intervention, 28(3), 119–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Montanari, S. (2019). 14. Facilitated language learning in multilinguals. In S. Montanari, & S. Quay (Hrsg.) (Eds.), Multidisciplinary perspectives on multilingualism: The fundamentals (pp. 299–324). De Gruyter Mouton. [Google Scholar]
- Muthén, B., Asparouhov, T., Hunter, A. M., & Leuchter, A. F. (2011). Growth modeling with nonignorable dropout: Alternative analyses of the STAR*D antidepressant trial. Psychological Methods, 16(1), 17–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998). Mplus User’s Guide (8. Aufl.). Muthén & Muthén. [Google Scholar]
- Newman, D. A. (2014). Missing Data: Five Practical Guidelines. Organizational Research Methods, 17(4), 372–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nylund, K. L., Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. O. (2007). Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A monte carlo simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 14(4), 535–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pace, A., Luo, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2017). Identifying pathways between socioeconomic status and language development. Annual Review of Linguistics, 3(1), 285–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paradis, J. (2011). Individual differences in child English second language acquisition: Comparing child-internal and child-external factors. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1(3), 213–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paradis, J., Soto-Corominas, A., Chen, X., & Gottardo, A. (2020). How language environment, age, and cognitive capacity support the bilingual development of Syrian refugee children recently arrived in Canada. Applied Psycholinguistics, 41(6), 1255–1281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Petermann, F. (2010). Sprachstandserhebungstest für Fünf- bis Zehnjährige (SET 5-10). Hogrefe. [Google Scholar]
- Schmerse, D. (2021). Peer effects on early language development in dual language learners. Child Development, 92(5), 2153–2169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schöler, H. (2020). Spracherwerbstheorien. In S. Sachse, A.-K. Bockmann, & A. Buschmann (Hrsg.) (Eds.), Sprachentwicklung (pp. 65–87). Springer. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spurk, D., Hirschi, A., Wang, M., Valero, D., & Kauffeld, S. (2020). Latent profile analysis: A review and “how to” guide of its application within vocational behavior research. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 120, 103445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Staatssekretariat für Bildung, Forschung und Innovation SBFI. (2023). Bildungsbericht Schweiz 2023. Schweizerische Koordinationsstelle für Bildungsforschung. Available online: https://www.skbf-csre.ch/fileadmin/files/pdf/bildungsberichte/2023/BiBer_2023_D.pdf (accessed on 12 October 2024).
- Staatssekretariat für Migration SEM. (2018). Ausländerstatistik 2014. Staatssekretariat für Migration SEM. Available online: https://www.admin.ch/gov/de/start/dokumentation/medienmitteilungen.msg-id-56988.html (accessed on 12 October 2024).
- Statistisches Amt des Kantons Basel-Stadt. (2018). Bevölkerungsstatistik. Available online: https://statistik.bs.ch/ (accessed on 12 October 2024).
- Subban, P., Bradford, B., Sharma, U., Loreman, T., Avramidis, E., Kullmann, H., Sahli Lozano, C., Romano, A., & Woodcock, S. (2022). Does it really take a village to raise a child? Reflections on the need for collective responsibility in inclusive education. European Journal of Special Needs Education, 38(2), 291–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tannenbaum, M., & Howie, P. (2002). The Association between Language Maintenance and Family Relations: Chinese Immigrant Children in Australia. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 23(5), 408–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tellegen, P., Laros, J., & Petermann, F. (2007). Snijders-Oomen non-verbaler Intelligenztest von 2, 5 bis 7 Jahre (SON-R 2, 5–7): Testbatterie zur sprachfreien Messung der allgemeinen Intelligent. Hogrefe. [Google Scholar]
- Troesch, L. M., Keller, K., Loher, S., & Grob, A. (2017). Umgebungs- und Herkunftssprache: Der Einfluss des elterlichen Sprachengebrauchs auf den Zweitspracherwerb der Kinder. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 31(2), 149–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Troesch, L. M., Segerer, R., Claus-Pröstler, N., & Grob, A. (2021). Parental acculturation attitudes: Direct and indirect impacts on children’s second language acquisition. Early Education and Development, 32(2), 272–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Druten-Frietman, L., Denessen, E., Gijsel, M., & Verhoeven, L. (2015). Child, home and institutional predictors of preschool vocabulary growth. Learning and Individual Differences, 43, 92–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and word. In L. S. Vygotsky, E. Hanfmann, & G. Vakar (Hrsg.) (Eds.), Thought and Language (pp. 119–153). MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Walther, B. (2023). Reliabilität von Skalen mit nur zwei Items—Spearman-Brown-Koeffizient. Available online: https://bjoernwalther.com/reliabilitaet-von-skalen-mit-nur-zwei-items-spearman-brown-koeffizient/ (accessed on 12 October 2024).
- White, I. R., Royston, P., & Wood, A. M. (2011). Multiple imputation using chained equations: Issues and guidance for practice. Statistics in Medicine, 30(4), 377–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Whiteside, K. E., Gooch, D., & Norbury, C. F. (2017). English language proficiency and early school attainment among children learning english as an additional language. Child Development, 88(3), 812–827. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wickrama, K. A. S., Lee, T. K., O’Neal, C. W., & Lorenz, F. O. (2016). Higher-order growth curves and mixture modeling with mplus: A practical guide (1. Auflage). Routledge. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Variable | N | Missing (%) | M | SD | Min. | Max. |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Language Measures: | ||||||
T1 Word Comprehension | 443 | 0 | 4.53 | 3.15 | 0 | 9 |
T1 Sentence Comprehension | 443 | 0 | 2.95 | 2.74 | 0 | 8 |
T1 Word Production | 443 | 0 | 5.56 | 6.92 | 0 | 24 |
T1 Sentence Production | 443 | 0 | 3.19 | 5.36 | 0 | 28 |
T2 Word Comprehension | 303 | 31.6 | 7.51 | 2.17 | 0 | 9 |
T2 Sentence Comprehension | 304 | 31.4 | 5.84 | 2.78 | 0 | 8 |
T2 Word Production | 303 | 31.6 | 13.50 | 7.00 | 0 | 24 |
T2 Sentence Production | 302 | 31.8 | 11.11 | 8.35 | 0 | 28 |
T3 Language Comprehension | 257 | 58.0 | 4.98 | 2.84 | 0 | 12 |
T3 Picture Naming | 254 | 42.7 | 18.81 | 8.61 | 0 | 40 |
T4 Language Comprehension | 213 | 51.9 | 7.44 | 2.66 | 0 | 12 |
T4 Picture Naming | 213 | 51.9 | 26.09 | 7.56 | 0 | 40 |
Predictors: | ||||||
T1 Age (in months) | 443 | 0.0 | 41.88 | 4.24 | 34 | 52 |
SES 1 | 411 | 7.2 | −0.02 | 0.86 | −2.0 | 1.72 |
T1 Duration (years) of residence in Switzerland (mothers) | 432 | 2.5 | 11.63 | 10.22 | 0 | 40 |
T1 Duration (years)of residence in Switzerland (fathers) | 413 | 6.8 | 16.08 | 11.59 | 0 | 40 |
T1 Contact (frequency) with German speaking adults | 379 | 14.4 | 2.17 | 1.18 | 1 | 4 |
T1 Contact (frequency) with German speaking children | 388 | 12.4 | 2.16 | 1.12 | 1 | 4 |
T1 Abstract thinking | 427 | 2.9 | 4.81 | 2.43 | 0 | 15 |
Yes (n) | Proportion in % | |||||
Gender (female) | 443 | 0.0 | 221 | 49.9 | ||
Attendance of early childcare before T1 | 440 | 0.7 | 259 | 58.9 | ||
Familial language: mainly other than German | 440 | 0.7 | 374 | 85.0 |
Predictor Variables | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| - | ||||||||||
| 0.03 | - | |||||||||
| 0.09 | 0.09 | - | ||||||||
| 0.42 *** | 0.05 | 0.03 | - | |||||||
| 0.00 | −0.06 | 0.13 ** | 0.09 | - | ||||||
| 0.13 ** | −0.05 | 0.12 * | −0.08 | 0.24 *** | - | |||||
| 0.05 | −0.00 | 0.02 | −0.23 *** | −0.18 *** | −0.02 | - | ||||
| −0.03 | −0.07 | 0.19 *** | −0.17 ** | 0.10 | 0.31 *** | −0.05 | - | |||
| 0.26 *** | 0.05 | 0.36*** | 0.17 *** | 0.38 *** | 0.42 *** | −0.20 *** | 0.28 *** | - | ||
| 0.13 * | −0.02 | 0.29 *** | 0.22 *** | 0.38 *** | 0.39 *** | −0.16 ** | 0.20 *** | 0.70 *** | - | |
| 0.08 | 0.01 | 0.28 *** | 0.26 *** | 0.27 *** | 0.20 *** | −0.22 *** | 0.22 *** | 0.60 *** | 0.70 *** | - |
| 0.05 | −0.05 | 0.29 *** | 0.19 *** | 0.24 *** | 0.29 *** | −0.24 *** | 0.29 *** | 0.58 *** | 0.67 *** | 0.73 |
Fit Statistics | 1 Class | 2 Classes | 3 Classes | 4 Classes | 5 Classes | 6 Classes |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
LL (No. of parameters) | 62.15 (8) | 285.18 (13) | 387.23 (18) | 428.67 (23) | 452.74 (28) | 476.96 (33) |
BIC | −75.55 | −491.15 | −664.78 | −717.19 | −734.86 | −752.83 |
SSABIC | −100.94 | −532.41 | −721.90 | −790.19 | −823.721 | −857.56 |
Entropy | 1.00 | 0.85 | 0.76 | 0.77 | 0.72 | 0.72 |
Adj. LMR-LRT (p) | - | 431.89 (<0.001) | 197.61 (<0.001) | 80.25 (0.002) | 71.96 (0.147) | 85.90 (0.641) |
BLRT (p) | - | 446.07 (<0.001) | 204.09 (<0.001) | 82.89 (<0.001) | 74.32 (<0.001) | 88.71 (<0.001) |
Group n (%); APP C1 | 443 (100%); 1.00 | 282 (63.8%); 0.97 | 110 (24.8%); 0.82 | 104 (23.5%); 0.81 | 48 (11.0%); 0.87 | 111 (25.1%); 0.92 |
C2 | 161 (36.2%); 0.94 | 198 (44.7%); 0.86 | 170 (38.4%); 0.84 | 112 (25.4%); 0.89 | 49 (11.0%); 0.90 | |
C3 | 135 (30.5%); 0.94 | 120 (27.0%); 0.88 | 75 (17.0%); 0.84 | 79 (17.9%); 0.76 | ||
C4 | 49 (11.1%); 0.91 | 99 (22.4%); 0.62 | 46 (10.4%); 0.53 | |||
C5 | 107 (24.3%); 0.78 | 87 (19.6%); 0.76 | ||||
C6 | 71 (16.1%); 0.68 |
Predictor Variable | Profile C2 (38%) | Profile C3 (27%) | Profile C4 (11%) | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
b | SE | OR | p | b | SE | OR | p | b | SE | OR | p | |
Intercept | −0.74 | 3.11 | −0.24 | 0.81 | −5.23 | 2.63 | −1.99 | <0.05 | −17.66 | 4.77 | −3.71 | <0.001 |
T1 Age | −0.08 | 0.07 | 0.93 | 0.27 | −0.02 | 0.06 | 0.98 | 0.73 | 0.18 | 0.10 | 1.20 | 0.08 |
Gender | −0.21 | 0.54 | 0.81 | 0.70 | 0.02 | 0.49 | 1.02 | 0.96 | 0.09 | 0.65 | 1.09 | 0.89 |
T1 Abstract thinking | 0.30 | 0.19 | 0.35 | 0.12 | 0.39 | 0.16 | 1.47 | <0.05 | 0.63 | 0.20 | 1.87 | <0.01 |
T1 duration of parents’ residence in Switzerland | 0.15 | 0.06 | 1.16 | <0.01 | 0.16 | 0.05 | 1.17 | <0.01 | 0.20 | 0.06 | 1.23 | <0.001 |
T1 Contact with German speakers | 0.00 | 0.25 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 0.52 | 0.24 | 1.68 | <0.05 | 0.72 | 0.35 | 2.05 | <0.05 |
Attendance of early childcare before T1 | 2.00 | 0.65 | 7.38 | <0.01 | 2.78 | 0.59 | 16.06 | <0.001 | 3.86 | 0.91 | 47.64 | <0.001 |
Familial language: mainly other than German | 0.74 | 1.08 | 2.11 | 0.49 | 0.14 | 0.84 | 1.15 | 0.87 | −1.41 | 1.03 | 0.25 | 0.17 |
SES | 0.14 | 0.36 | 1.15 | 0.70 | 0.40 | 0.32 | 1.49 | 0.21 | 1.27 | 0.43 | 3.54 | <0.01 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Troesch, L.M.; Torchetti, L.; Hasler, S.; Grob, A. Second Language Trajectories in Immigrant Children: Latent Class Growth Analysis. Educ. Sci. 2025, 15, 163. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15020163
Troesch LM, Torchetti L, Hasler S, Grob A. Second Language Trajectories in Immigrant Children: Latent Class Growth Analysis. Education Sciences. 2025; 15(2):163. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15020163
Chicago/Turabian StyleTroesch, Larissa Maria, Loredana Torchetti, Sonja Hasler, and Alexander Grob. 2025. "Second Language Trajectories in Immigrant Children: Latent Class Growth Analysis" Education Sciences 15, no. 2: 163. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15020163
APA StyleTroesch, L. M., Torchetti, L., Hasler, S., & Grob, A. (2025). Second Language Trajectories in Immigrant Children: Latent Class Growth Analysis. Education Sciences, 15(2), 163. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15020163