Are Horses (Equus caballus) Sensitive to Human Emotional Cues?
<p>Setup for the study. (<b>a</b>) The experimental setting of the study: When the trial began, the experimenter (E) stood in front of the subject horse. When the subject horse (H) faced the experimenter, the experimenter quickly exhibited a gaze cue by turning her head to the back space of either the right or the left barrier. The dotted line shows the line of the experimenter’s visual line during the gaze-emotional cue presentations. (<b>b</b>) The photo of the experimental setting of the study: The assistant (A) guided the horse by lead to the experimental area. After the horse calmed down, the assistant looked at the floor. The assistant held the subject horse on a long- enough rope and allowed the horse to move freely within the range of the rope during the trial.</p> "> Figure 2
<p>Emotional facial expressions.</p> "> Figure 3
<p>Total frequency of gaze following. Total frequency of gaze following during two gaze-emotional cue presentations per trial of each experimental condition. The maximum value was 2 in each experimental condition because the experimenter presented gaze shift two times per trial. The median in each condition was 0 in the Happy condition, 1 in the Neutral condition, and 0 in the Disgust condition (* <span class="html-italic">p</span> < 0.05).</p> "> Figure 4
<p>Total looking time of horses. Total looking time of the horses during two gaze-emotional cue presentations per trial of each experimental condition. The maximum total looking time was 180 frames because the experimenter presented each gaze-emotional cue for 90 frames (3 s). The median in each condition was 0 in the Happy condition, 19 in the Neutral condition, and 0 in the Disgust condition (* <span class="html-italic">p</span> < 0.05) (30 frames = 1 s).</p> ">
Abstract
:Simple Summary
Abstract
1. Introduction
2. Method
2.1. Subjects
2.2. Materials
2.3. Procedure
2.4. Experiment Design
2.5. Analysis
2.6. Ethical Approval
3. Results
3.1. Total Frequency of Gaze Following
3.2. Total Looking Time
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Mendl, M.; Burman, O.H.P.; Parker, R.M.A.; Paul, E.S. Cognitive bias as an indicator of animal emotion and welfare: Emerging evidence and underlying mechanisms. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2009, 118, 161–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spoor, J.R.; Kelly, J.R. The Evolutionary significance of affect in groups: Communication and group bonding. Gr. Process. Intergr. Relat. 2004, 7, 398–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Flack, J.C.; de Waal, F.B.M. ‘Any animal whatever’ Darwinian building blocks of morality in monkeys and apes. J. Conscious. Stud. 2000, 7, 1–29. [Google Scholar]
- Müller, C.A.; Schmitt, K.; Barber, A.L.A.; Huber, L. Dogs can discriminate emotional expressions of human faces. Curr. Biol. 2015, 25, 601–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Parr, L.A. Cognitive and physiological markers of emotional awareness in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Anim. Cogn. 2001, 4, 223–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Albuquerque, N.; Guo, K.; Wilkinson, A.; Savalli, C.; Otta, E.; Mills, D. Dogs recognize dog and human emotions. Biol. Lett. 2013, 12, 20150883. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wathan, J.; Proops, L.; Grounds, K.; McComb, K. Horses discriminate between facial expressions of conspecifics. Sci. Rep. 2016, 6, 38322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Russell, C.L.; Bard, K.A.; Adamson, L.B. Social referencing by young chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 1997, 111, 185–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nagasawa, M.; Murai, K.; Mogi, K.; Kikusui, T. Dogs can discriminate human smiling faces from blank expressions. Anim. Cogn. 2011, 14, 525–533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Merola, I.; Prato-Previde, E.; Marshall-Pescini, S. Social referencing in dog-owner dyads? Anim. Cogn. 2012, 15, 175–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buttelmann, D.; Tomasello, M. Can domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use referential emotional expressions to locate hidden food? Anim. Cogn. 2013, 16, 137–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hare, B.; Brown, M.; Williamson, C.; Tomasello, M. The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science 2002, 298, 1634–1636. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Freedman, A.H.; Gronau, I.; Schweizer, R.M.; Ortega-Del Vecchyo, D.; Han, E.; Silva, P.M.; Galaverni, M.; Fan, Z.; Marx, P.; Lorente-Galdos, B.; et al. Genome sequencing highlights the dynamic early history of dogs. PLoS Genet. 2014, 10, e1004016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Merola, I.; Prato-Previde, E.; Lazzaroni, M.; Marshall-Pescini, S. Dogs’ comprehension of referential emotional expressions: Familiar people and familiar emotions are easier. Anim. Cogn. 2014, 17, 373–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hare, B.; Tomasello, M. Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends Cogn. Sci. 2005, 9, 439–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Maros, K.; Gácsi, M.; Miklósi, Á. Comprehension of human pointing gestures in horses (Equus caballus). Anim. Cogn. 2008, 11, 457–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nawroth, C.; Ebersbach, M.; von Borell, E. Juvenile domestic pigs (Sus scrofa domestica) use human-given cues in an object choice task. Anim. Cogn. 2014, 17, 701–713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Outram, A.K.; Stear, N.A.; Bendrey, R.; Olsen, S.; Kasparov, A.; Zaibert, V.; Thorpe, N.; Evershed, R.P. The earliest horse harnessing and milking. Science 2009, 323, 1332–1335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Proops, L.; McComb, K. Attributing attention: The use of human-given cues by domestic horses (Equus caballus). Anim. Cogn. 2010, 13, 197–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Takimoto, A.; Hori, Y.; Fujita, K. Horses (Equus caballus) adaptively change the modality of their begging behavior as a function of human attentional states. Psychologia 2016, 59, 100–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, A.V.; Proops, L.; Grounds, K.; Wathan, J.; McComb, K. Functionally relevant responses to human facial expressions of emotion in the domestic horse (Equus caballus). Biol. Lett. 2016, 12, 20150907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Proops, L.; Grounds, K.; Smith, A.V.; McComb, K. Animals remember previous facial expressions that specific humans have exhibited. Curr. Biol. 2018, 28, 1428–1432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Smith, A.V.; Proops, L.; Grounds, K.; Wathan, J.; Scott, S.K.; McComb, K. Domestic horses (Equus caballus) discriminate between negative and positive human nonverbal vocalisations. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 13052. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nakamura, K.; Takimoto-Inose, A.; Hasegawa, T. Cross-modal perception of human emotion in domestic horses (Equus caballus). Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 8660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ekman, P.; Friesen, W.V. Unmasking the Face: A Guide to Recognizing Emotions from Facial Clues; Prentice-Hall: Oxford, UK, 1975. [Google Scholar]
- Flom, R.; Gartman, P. Does affective information influence domestic dogs’ (Canis lupus familiaris) point-following behavior? Anim. Cogn. 2016, 19, 317–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Goossens, B.M.A.; Dekleva, M.; Reader, S.M.; Sterck, E.H.M.; Bolhuis, J.J. Gaze following in monkeys is modulated by observed facial expressions. Anim. Behav. 2008, 75, 1673–1681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tomasello, M.; Call, J.; Hare, B. Five primate species follow the visual gaze of conspecifics. Anim. Behav. 1998, 55, 1063–1069. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Shepherd, S.V. Following gaze: Gaze-following behavior as a window into social cognition. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2010, 4, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Proops, L.; McComb, K.; Reby, D. Cross-modal individual recognition in domestic horses (Equus caballus). Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2009, 106, 947–951. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Proops, L.; McComb, K. Cross-modal individual recognition in domestic horses (Equus caballus) extends to familiar humans. Proc. R. Soc. B 2012, 279, 3131–3138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Met, A.; Miklósi, Á.; Lakatos, G. Gaze-following behind barriers in domestic dogs. Anim. Cogn. 2014, 17, 1401–1405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lampe, J.F.; Andre, J. Cross-modal recognition of human individuals in domestic horses (Equus caballus). Anim. Cogn. 2012, 15, 623–630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Subjects | Age | Gender | Breed |
---|---|---|---|
Okaaoi 99 | 17 | Gelding | Mixed breed of Criollo and Hokkaido |
Michiyuki 99 | 17 | Gelding | Mixed breed of Criollo and Hokkaido |
Naesaka 08 | 8 | Gelding | Hokkaido |
Hikarisora | 6 | Mare | Mixed breed of Criollo and Hokkaido |
Utaoka 10 | 6 | Gelding | Hokkaido |
Soramizu 10 | 6 | Gelding | Hokkaido |
Tachigiku | 3 | Gelding | Light half-breed horse |
Tachimizu | 3 | Gelding | Light half-breed horse |
Tachihime | 3 | Mare | Light half-breed horse |
Kazeshin | 24 | Gelding | Light half-breed horse |
Kurihime | 11 | Mare | Light breed horse |
Anya | 10 | Mare | Light breed horse |
Sky | 9 | Gelding | Appaloosa |
Kurara | 9 | Mare | Light breed horse |
© 2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Baba, C.; Kawai, M.; Takimoto-Inose, A. Are Horses (Equus caballus) Sensitive to Human Emotional Cues? Animals 2019, 9, 630. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9090630
Baba C, Kawai M, Takimoto-Inose A. Are Horses (Equus caballus) Sensitive to Human Emotional Cues? Animals. 2019; 9(9):630. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9090630
Chicago/Turabian StyleBaba, Chihiro, Masahito Kawai, and Ayaka Takimoto-Inose. 2019. "Are Horses (Equus caballus) Sensitive to Human Emotional Cues?" Animals 9, no. 9: 630. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9090630
APA StyleBaba, C., Kawai, M., & Takimoto-Inose, A. (2019). Are Horses (Equus caballus) Sensitive to Human Emotional Cues? Animals, 9(9), 630. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9090630