Nothing Special   »   [go: up one dir, main page]

Academia.eduAcademia.edu

Gamma Rhythms in the Brain

2011, PLoS Biology

Primer Gamma Rhythms in the Brain Xiaoxuan Jia1*, Adam Kohn1,2 1 Dominick P. Purpura Department of Neuroscience, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Bronx, New York, United States of America, 2 Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Bronx, New York, United States of America Brain rhythms are activity fluctuations shared in populations of neurons. They are evident in extracellular electric fields and detectable through recordings performed within the brain or on the scalp. The gamma rhythm, a relatively high frequency (30– 80 Hz) component of these fluctuations, has received a great deal of attention. Gamma is modulated by sensory input and internal processes such as working memory and attention. Numerous theories have proposed that gamma contributes directly to brain function, but others argue that gamma is better viewed as a simple byproduct of network activity. Here we provide a basic introduction to this enigmatic signal, the mechanisms that generate it, and an accompanying paper in PLoS Biology attempting to elucidate its potential function. Hans Berger first successfully measured the brain waves of humans in 1924 using the electroencephalogram (EEG) [1]. His goal was to demonstrate that the electromagnetic fields of the human brain could be used for telepathy. Although the signals he detected were unsuccessful for this purpose, the EEG was widely adopted by clinicians and scientists. This is because the recordings are easy to perform and the rhythms detected are informative of brain state. For example, when we are in a deep sleep, the EEG consists of low-frequency, large-amplitude oscillations; when we are awake and attentive, it consists primarily of fast, small amplitude rhythms. Brain rhythms are evident as extracellular voltage fluctuations. These arise from summed electrical activity (primarily, but not exclusively, inputs) in populations of neurons, and are shaped by the geometry and alignment of those neurons [2]. The resultant fluctuations can be measured on the scalp by EEG or magnetoencephalography (MEG), and intracranially with subdural electrodes (electrocorticography). They can also be measured, on a more local basis, with a high impedance electrode placed in the brain (Figure 1A). The voltage fluctuations detected are then low-pass filtered (,250 Hz) to capture the slower fluctuations of brain rhythms (Figure 1B). The resultant signal—termed the local field potential (LFP)—was frequently used to study brain function, until it fell in popularity with the advent of single-cell electrophysiology in the late 1950s. Over the last decade, however, LFPs have attracted renewed interest as a potentially useful signal for studying the behavior of ensembles of neurons. The LFP is a continuous voltage signal that can vary in amplitude and frequency content. Like the EEG, it can be decomposed into different frequency components—delta (,4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–12 Hz), beta (12–30 Hz), gamma (30–80 Hz), and high-gamma or high-frequency activity (.80 Hz)—although the precise frequency ranges associated with these terms vary across studies. The relative contribution of these different components to the measured signal is quantified by their relative power (Figure 2). In quiescent networks, most of the power in the LFP is found at low frequencies, indicating that rhythms like delta and theta contribute more significantly than high frequency ones. This is still the case when networks are activated, but less so: the power in higher frequencies increases, whereas that in lower frequencies is suppressed. The enhancement of gamma power in this driven state is particularly striking and is evident as a distinct ‘‘bump’’ in the power spectrum (Figure 2; right panel, solid line). A prominent gamma rhythm provides a signature of engaged networks. Gamma has been observed in a number of cortical areas, as well as subcortical structures, in numerous species. In sensory cortex, gamma power increases with sensory drive [3,4], and with a broad range of cognitive phenomena, including perceptual grouping [5] and attention [6]. At a given recording site, gamma is stronger for some stimuli than others, generally displaying selectivity and a preference similar to that of nearby neuronal spiking activity [7,8]. In higher cortex, gamma power is elevated during working memory [9] and learning [10]. Interestingly, irregular gamma activity has been observed in neurological disorders such as Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson’s disease, schizophrenia, and epilepsy [11]. To interpret the meaning of changes in gamma requires an understanding of the cellular and network mechanisms that generate it. Fast-spiking GABAergic inhibitory interneurons are known to be crucial, with their activity being both necessary and sufficient to generate gamma [12–14]. Network models suggest that this process may be enhanced by interactions with excitatory neurons [15] and that local gamma-generating networks can be coupled by long-range horizontal connections [16] or gap junctions among inhibitory interneurons [17]. Such coupling would seem necessary, as gamma has been shown to be coherent across millimeters of cortex [18–20]. It is well established that gamma correlates with engaged or driven networks, but it is less clear whether it is a simple byproduct of network activity or has an important functional role. This is not for lack of proposals: numerous functions have been attributed to this rhythm. Most of these hinge on a relationship between gamma and the timing of spiking activity in nearby neurons. Spikes are actively generated signals in individual neurons and relay information between neural networks. Gamma activity is not actively propagated. It is a component of an extracellular field Citation: Jia X, Kohn A (2011) Gamma Rhythms in the Brain. PLoS Biol 9(4): e1001045. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1001045 Published April 12, 2011 Copyright: ß 2011 Jia, Kohn. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Funding: This work was supported in part by a grant from the National Institutes of Health to AK (EY016774). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist. Primers provide a concise introduction into an important aspect of biology highlighted by a current PLoS Biology research article. Abbreviations: EEG, electroencephalogram; LFP, local field potential * E-mail: jxiaoxuan@gmail.com PLoS Biology | www.plosbiology.org 1 April 2011 | Volume 9 | Issue 4 | e1001045 Figure 1. Illustration of LFP recordings. (A) A high impedance electrode detects extracellular electrical activity of nearby neurons. (B) This raw signal is low-pass filtered (e.g., ,250 Hz) to provide the local field potential (LFP), and high-pass filtered (e.g., 0.5–10 kHz) to isolate spiking activity. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1001045.g001 One purports that gamma acts as a temporal reference frame, with the gamma phase at which spikes occur encoding stimulus strength [22]. Consistent with this suggestion, neurons in visual cortex can encode stimulus orientation in ‘‘phase-of-firing’’ relative to gamma [23]. Another theory proposes that gamma may influence the communication between neuronal populations [24,25]. Here the suggestion is that when field potentials and spiking activity in two groups of neurons are phase coherent, the communication between them will be maximal. A third hypothesis, ‘‘binding by synchrony’’, suggests gamma can link the representation of a single sensory input (e.g., a visual object) whose features are processed by potential that reflects primarily the synaptic input to a collection of neurons. Because of this, gamma can only play a role in processing if it is linked to spiking activity in a meaningful way. A coupling between gamma and spike timing could arise because local inhibitory neurons—which contribute strongly to gamma—fire preferentially at the trough of the gamma cycle [21]. This makes the spiking of excitatory projection neurons more likely to occur at an offset phase, when inhibition is weaker. Based on this mechanism—in some cases, predating its discovery—numerous theories have suggested that the gamma coordination of spiking activity is central to cortical processing. 10 Spontaneous Stimulus driven Power (A.U.) 0.2 mV 10 10 10 10 4 3 2 1 0 Spontaneous Stimulus driven 10 -1 0 0.2 second 40 80 120 Frequency (Hz) 160 200 Figure 2. LFPs for spontaneous and stimulus-driven activity. (Left) Example traces of the LFP during spontaneous activity and visually driven activity in primary visual cortex. (Right) The corresponding power spectra for the two conditions, with the frequency ranges of different rhythms indicated. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1001045.g002 PLoS Biology | www.plosbiology.org 2 April 2011 | Volume 9 | Issue 4 | e1001045 related transients. They show this contamination can contribute to the high frequency components of the LFP, and has a measurable effect on frequencies extending down to 50 Hz. For studies that have focused on spike–gamma interactions in the lower gamma rhythm range (30–50 Hz), contamination is thus less likely to be an issue, although the precise frequency range over which contamination occurs will depend on the specific properties of the filters used to separate spikes from LFPs. However, for frequencies above this range (.50 Hz), which include the higher frequencies of gamma and the full range of high-gamma, spike–LFP correlations might be inflated by spike contamination. More generally, the findings of Ray and Maunsell suggest the need for a re-evaluation of the spike–LFP timing relationship, particularly in cases where this has been established using signals recorded from the same electrode. In a related analysis, Ray and Maunsell tested the relationship between high-gamma and gamma power. To do so, they manipulated stimulus size. It is well known that many neurons in primary visual cortex are less responsive to large stimuli than small ones [33]. Gamma power, in contrast, increases with stimulus size [34]. Building on this work, Ray and Maunsell show that high-gamma power is modulated similarly to spiking activity by stimulus size (i.e., suppressed by large stimuli), and thus differently from gamma. The authors also show that high-gamma power has similar temporal dynamics as spiking activity, whereas gamma does not. Together, this strongly suggests that the proposed functions of gamma do not apply to high-gamma, even in situations where both signals are similarly enhanced. Rather, high-gamma may best be viewed as a reliable and convenient signal to represent multi-unit activity (MUA). The findings of Ray and Maunsell help clarify the relationships among gamma, high-gamma, and spiking activity. But much remains unclear. It is certain that gamma, like other brain rhythms, can provide a signature of cognitive state, as well as network dysfunction. To move beyond the interesting correlation between these rhythms and brain state, like those first described by Berger, we need a better understanding of the underlying generative mechanisms, the way in which these signals modulate spiking activity, and the effect they have on the computations performed by neuronal networks. Only then will we know what role, if any, gamma plays in cortical function. different groups of neurons [26]. At the heart of these proposals is the concept that gamma influences spike timing and that this affects cortical computation and function. A number of recent studies have taken a more critical view of the role of gamma, testing whether it has the properties required for its purported functions. One study showed that the frequency of gamma can vary between nearby sets of neurons, limiting its ability to function as a global timing reference [27]. Another has shown that, at a single site, the gamma rhythm is not ‘‘autocoherent’’, meaning that its absolute phase changes with time, a pernicious feature for a reference clock or integrative signal [28]. In this vein, it is also worth noting that gamma fluctuations are small, roughly 10–20 microvolts on average, and account for only 0.5%–10% of the total power in the LFP. These observations raise the possibility that gamma is simply a resonant frequency that has no special function, a byproduct of a recurrently connected neuronal network. To test existing proposals, and to understand the function of gamma more generally, it is critical to analyze the temporal relationship between spikes and gamma. This is typically done using spike-field coherence analysis or by spike-triggered averaging of the LFP, both of which provide a measure of the temporal or phase relationship between spike trains and the LFP. These measures have revealed weak but measurable coupling, which increases when gamma power is elevated [6,9,29]. This coupling is only meaningful, however, if the two signals are measured independently. LFPs and spikes are often recorded from a single electrode. Because extracellular action potential waveforms have a broad frequency spectrum, including power below 250 Hz, their energy can leak into the LFP signal [30,31]. That is, the low-pass filtering of the extracellular voltage signal, which is used to isolate the LFP (Figure 1), may not entirely remove action potential waveforms. The resultant contamination would introduce spurious correlations: the timing of spikes will appear to be related to fluctuations in LFP power for the simple reason that a remnant of the spike waveform remains in that signal. The paper by Ray and Maunsell in this issue of PLoS Biology carefully examines the interaction between spikes and the LFP [32]. Using clever analysis, they provide rigorous quantification of the contamination of LFP signals by spike waveforms and spike- References 13. Cardin JA, Carlen M, Meletis K, Knoblich U, Zhang F, et al. (2009) Driving fast-spiking cells induces gamma rhythm and controls sensory responses. Nature 459: 663–667. 14. Bartos M, Vida I, Jonas P (2007) Synaptic mechanisms of synchronized gamma oscillations in inhibitory interneuron networks. Nat Rev Neurosci 8: 45–56. 15. Whittington MA, Cunningham MO, LeBeau FE, Racca C, Traub RD (2011) Multiple origins of the cortical gamma rhythm. Dev Neurobiol 71: 92–106. 16. Traub RD, Whittington MA, Stanford IM, Jefferys JG (1996) A mechanism for generation of long-range synchronous fast oscillations in the cortex. Nature 383: 621–624. 17. Gibson JR, Beierlein M, Connors BW (1999) Two networks of electrically coupled inhibitory neurons in neocortex. Nature 402: 75–79. 18. Leopold DA, Murayama Y, Logothetis NK (2003) Very slow activity fluctuations in monkey visual cortex: implications for functional brain imaging. Cereb Cortex 13: 422–433. 19. Frien A, Eckhorn R (2000) Functional coupling shows stronger stimulus dependency for fast oscillations than for low-frequency components in striate cortex of awake monkey. Eur J Neurosci 12: 1466–1478. 20. Murthy VN, Fetz EE (1992) Coherent 25- to 35-Hz oscillations in the sensorimotor cortex of awake behaving monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 89: 5670–5674. 21. Hasenstaub A, Shu Y, Haider B, Kraushaar U, Duque A, et al. (2005) Inhibitory postsynaptic potentials carry synchronized frequency information in active cortical networks. Neuron 47: 423–435. 22. Masquelier T, Hugues E, Deco G, Thorpe SJ (2009) Oscillations, phase-of-firing coding, and spike timing-dependent plasticity: an efficient learning scheme. J Neurosci 29: 13484–13493. 1. Haas LF (2003) Hans Berger (1873–1941), Richard Caton (1842–1926), and electroencephalography. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 74: 9. 2. Buzsaki G (2006) Rhythms of the brain. New York: Oxford University Press. 3. Adrian ED (1942) Olfactory reactions in the brain of the hedgehog. J Physiol 100: 459–473. 4. Henrie JA, Shapley R (2005) LFP power spectra in V1 cortex: the graded effect of stimulus contrast. J Neurophysiol 94: 479–490. 5. Tallon-Baudry C, Bertrand O (1999) Oscillatory gamma activity in humans and its role in object representation. Trends Cogn Sci 3: 151–162. 6. Fries P, Reynolds JH, Rorie AE, Desimone R (2001) Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attention. Science 291: 1560– 1563. 7. Berens P, Keliris GA, Ecker AS, Logothetis NK, Tolias AS (2008) Feature selectivity of the gamma-band of the local field potential in primate primary visual cortex. Front Neurosci 2: 199–207. 8. Liu J, Newsome WT (2006) Local field potential in cortical area MT: stimulus tuning and behavioral correlations. J Neurosci 26: 7779–7790. 9. Pesaran B, Pezaris JS, Sahani M, Mitra PP, Andersen RA (2002) Temporal structure in neuronal activity during working memory in macaque parietal cortex. Nat Neurosci 5: 805–811. 10. Bauer EP, Paz R, Pare D (2007) Gamma oscillations coordinate amygdalorhinal interactions during learning. J Neurosci 27: 9369–9379. 11. Uhlhaas PJ, Singer W (2006) Neural synchrony in brain disorders: relevance for cognitive dysfunctions and pathophysiology. Neuron 52: 155–168. 12. Whittington MA, Traub RD, Jefferys JG (1995) Synchronized oscillations in interneuron networks driven by metabotropic glutamate receptor activation. Nature 373: 612–615. PLoS Biology | www.plosbiology.org 3 April 2011 | Volume 9 | Issue 4 | e1001045 29. Fries P, Womelsdorf T, Oostenveld R, Desimone R (2008) The effects of visual stimulation and selective visual attention on rhythmic neuronal synchronization in macaque area V4. J Neurosci 28: 4823–4835. 30. Zanos TP, Mineault PJ, Pack CC (2011) Removal of spurious correlations between spikes and local field potentials. J Neurophysiol 105: 474–486. 31. David SV, Malaval N, Shamma SA (2010) Decoupling action potential bias from cortical local field potentials. Comput Intell Neurosci: 393019. 32. Ray S, Maunsell JHR (2011) Different origins of gamma rhythm and highgamma activity in macaque visual cortex. PLoS Biol 9(4): e1000610. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1000610. 33. Angelucci A, Bressloff PC (2006) Contribution of feedforward, lateral and feedback connections to the classical receptive field center and extra-classical receptive field surround of primate V1 neurons. Prog Brain Res 154: 93–120. 34. Gieselmann MA, Thiele A (2008) Comparison of spatial integration and surround suppression characteristics in spiking activity and the local field potential in macaque V1. Eur J Neurosci 28: 447–459. 23. Vinck M, Lima B, Womelsdorf T, Oostenveld R, Singer W, et al. (2010) Gamma- phase shifting in awake monkey visual cortex. J Neurosci 30: 1250–1257. 24. Fries P (2009) Neuronal gamma-band synchronization as a fundamental process in cortical computation. Annu Rev Neurosci 32: 209–224. 25. Womelsdorf T, Schoffelen JM, Oostenveld R, Singer W, Desimone R, et al. (2007) Modulation of neuronal interactions through neuronal synchronization. Science 316: 1609–1612. 26. Singer W (1999) Neuronal synchrony: a versatile code for the definition of relations? Neuron 24: 49–65, 111–125. 27. Ray S, Maunsell JH (2010) Differences in gamma frequencies across visual cortex restrict their possible use in computation. Neuron 67: 885–896. 28. Burns SP, Xing D, Shelley MJ, Shapley RM (2010) Searching for autocoherence in the cortical network with a time-frequency analysis of the local field potential. J Neurosci 30: 4033–4047. PLoS Biology | www.plosbiology.org 4 April 2011 | Volume 9 | Issue 4 | e1001045