JOURNAL OF
PSYCHIATRIC
RESEARCH
Journal of Psychiatric Research 40 (2006) 1–21
www.elsevier.com/locate/jpsychires
Review
Posttraumatic stress disorder: A state-of-the-science review
Charles B. Nemeroff a,*, J. Douglas Bremner b, Edna B. Foa c,
Helen S. Mayberg d, Carol S. North e, Murray B. Stein f
a
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, 1639 Pierce Drive, Atlanta, GA 30322-4990, USA
b
Department of Psychiatry and Radiology, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA
c
Department of Psychiatry, University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine, Philadelphia, PA, USA
d
Department of Psychiatry and Neurology, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA
e
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
f
Department of Psychiatry and Family & Preventive Medicine,
University of California San Diego and VA San Diego Healthcare System, La Jolla, CA, USA
Received 9 December 2004; received in revised form 30 June 2005; accepted 11 July 2005
Abstract
This article reviews the state-of-the-art research in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) from several perspectives: (1) Sex differences: PTSD is more frequent among women, who tend to have different types of precipitating traumas and higher rates of comorbid
panic disorder and agoraphobia than do men. (2) Risk and resilience: The presence of Group C symptoms after exposure to a disaster
or act of terrorism may predict the development of PTSD as well as comorbid diagnoses. (3) Impact of trauma in early life: Persistent
increases in CRF concentration are associated with early life trauma and PTSD, and may be reversed with paroxetine treatment. (4)
Imaging studies: Intriguing findings in treated and untreated depressed patients may serve as a paradigm of failed brain adaptation to
chronic emotional stress and anxiety disorders. (5) Neural circuits and memory: Hippocampal volume appears to be selectively
decreased and hippocampal function impaired among PTSD patients. (6) Cognitive behavioral approaches: Prolonged exposure therapy, a readily disseminated treatment modality, is effective in modifying the negative cognitions that are frequent among PTSD
patients. In the future, it would be useful to assess the validity of the PTSD construct, elucidate genetic and experiential contributing
factors (and their complex interrelationships), clarify the mechanisms of action for different treatments used in PTSD, discover ways
to predict which treatments (or treatment combinations) will be successful for a given individual, develop an operational definition of
remission in PTSD, and explore ways to disseminate effective evidence-based treatments for this condition.
Ó 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Posttraumatic stress disorder; Early life trauma; Hippocampus; Risk factors; Cognitive therapy; Antidepressants
Contents
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
*
Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Sex, trauma, and PTSD: what are the differences? . . . . . . . . . .
Risk and resilience factors after disasters and terrorism . . . . . .
Early life trauma and PTSD . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
What can imaging studies of depression teach us about
anxiety disorders? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 404 727 8382; fax: +1 404 727 3233.
E-mail address: cnemero@emory.edu (C.B. Nemeroff).
0022-3956/$ - see front matter Ó 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2005.07.005
.........................................
.........................................
.........................................
.........................................
failed adaptation to chronic emotional stress and
........................................
.
.
.
.
2
2
4
7
10
2
C.B. Nemeroff et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research 40 (2006) 1–21
6.
7.
8.
9.
Neural circuits, memory, and PTSD. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Cognitive behavioral approaches to the treatment of PTSD: what works? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Future directions for research . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Disclosure statement . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Acknowledgments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
1. Introduction
It has long been known that people sometimes develop
maladaptive symptoms after exposure to extreme stress.
Jacob Mendez Da Costa, an eminent Philadelphia physician, described an eponymous condition resembling
posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) among veterans
of the American Civil War (Vaisrub, 1975). The relatively high prevalence of this condition among veterans
of the Vietnam War (Card, 1987; Long et al., 1996;
Beals et al., 2002) was one important impetus for the
burgeoning of PTSD research over the last several decades. The diagnosis of PTSD was first included in the
third edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
of Mental Disorders (DSM) in 1980; (APA, 1980), since
then, considerable research effort has been directed towards the etiology, phenomenology, clinical and neurobiological characteristics, and treatment of PTSD and
related and common comorbid disorders.
In this article, we review the state of the art in
PTSD from six different perspectives: (1) sex differences
in trauma and PTSD; (2) risk and resilience factors in
the mass traumas of disasters and terrorism; (3) the impact of early life trauma and its relationship to psychiatric sequelae including PTSD; (4) imaging studies of
depression which can serve as a paradigm of failed
adaptation to chronic emotional stress; (5) alterations
in neural circuits and memory in PTSD; and (6) cognitive therapy approaches to the treatment of PTSD. We
conclude with suggestions for key directions in future
PTSD research.
.
.
.
.
.
.
13
15
17
18
18
18
Combat exposure accounts for a large proportion of
the PTSD seen among men in the United States: the
population-attributable fraction is reported to be
27.8% of 12-month PTSD (Prigerson et al., 2002). The
main burden of PTSD in the United States, however,
stems not from war or terrorism but from far more common events such as criminal victimization, motor vehicle
accidents, and childhood maltreatment (physical, sexual, and emotional) (Kessler et al., 1995; Stein, 2002;
Kessler, 2000). Men and women differ in the types of
trauma most frequently encountered; molestation and
sexual abuse are more frequent in women, while fights,
accidents, and threats involving a weapon (and combat)
are more frequent in men. Despite this, even when subjected to the same type of trauma as men, women still
have approximately twice the risk of developing PTSD
symptoms, and their symptoms are more likely to persist
than symptoms among men (Fig. 1) (Kessler et al., 1995;
Kessler, 2000; Breslau et al., 1997; Breslau and Davis,
1992; Van Loey et al., 2003; Holbrook et al., 2002; Stein
et al., 2000; DeLisi et al., 2003). However, it should be
noted that not all studies find this increased susceptibility in women. In a nested case-control analysis of a population-based survey of 30,000 Gulf War era veterans,
exposure to either of two kinds of severe trauma—sexual assault and combat—was associated with comparable risk for PTSD in men and women (Kang et al.,
2005). Furthermore, there is preliminary evidence from
studies of recent US veterans of war in Iraq and Afghanistan that men and women are at equal risk for PTSD
40
2. Sex, trauma, and PTSD: what are the differences?
Prevalence of Trauma
%
30
Male
Female
20
10
0
%
While much of the early research on PTSD involved
men with combat-related disorders, population studies
have revealed that PTSD is more prevalent in women.
In the National Comorbidity Survey, Kessler et al.
(1995) found an overall lifetime prevalence of PTSD of
7.8%, but women were over twice as likely as men to
have suffered from the condition (10.4% vs 5.0%;
p < 0.05). Community surveys consistently reveal elevated rates of PTSD among women [e.g., a Swedish
study by Frans et al. (2005)]. Why are most PTSD sufferers female?
.
.
.
.
.
.
70
60
50
40
30
20
10
0
Probability of PTSD
Trauma Accident Threat /
witness
weapon
Physical Molestation Combat
attack
Rape
Fig. 1. Prevalence of trauma and probability of PTSD (Kessler RC
et al. J Clin Psychiatry 2000;61(Suppl 5):4–12. Kessler RC et al. Arch
Gen Psychiatry 1995;52:1048–60.)
3
C.B. Nemeroff et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research 40 (2006) 1–21
and 31.8% of PTSD related to violence by intimate partners. Most of the participants in this study had experienced childhood maltreatment, and it was therefore
not possible to explore the impact of this factor on
PTSD risk. However, other studies have shown that
adults with histories of childhood trauma have increased
rates of PTSD. Although childhood sexual trauma has
been the most intensively studied in this regard, other
types of maltreatment—emotional abuse in particular—may be at least as important in explaining gender
differences in PTSD. Studying 8667 adult members of
an HMO, Edwards et al. (2003) found that lower scores
on the mental health scale of the SF-36 were associated
with more categories of abuse (sexual, physical and
emotional abuse, and witnessing of maternal battering),
and that both an emotionally abusive family environment and the interaction of such an environment with
the different types of maltreatment significantly lowered
mental health scores. While men reported significantly
more physical abuse, women reported a significantly
higher prevalence of sexual abuse as well as moderate
or severe emotional abuse in the family environment
(Fig. 2). Thus, differences in types of trauma exposure—possibly relating to sexual trauma (in childhood
and adulthood) and intimate partner violence—may explain some of the gender differences in rates of PTSD.
Psychosocial factors such as social support and stigma are also likely to influence gender differences in
PTSD following trauma exposure. Andrews et al.
(2003) assessed 118 male and 39 female victims of violent crime for PTSD symptoms as well as for levels of
positive support and negative reactions from others.
They found that women reported significantly more negative responses from family and friends, and that this
factor mediated the relationship between trauma exposure and subsequent PTSD symptoms at 6 months
post-exposure. This study serves to remind us that it
may be unwarranted to assume that men and womenÕs
experience of the same type of trauma will be similar.
For example, motor vehicle collisions—even those that
80
% in category
symptoms (Kang et al., 2005). It may be that gender differences are negligible under circumstances of extreme
trauma exposure: this possibility would be compatible
with findings from a population-based twin study in
which there was an effect of gender on major depression
in the presence of lower, but not higher, levels of psychosocial stress (Kendler et al., 2004). The implications
of this observation for mental health prevention efforts
remain to be determined.
In one of the very few prospective studies of PTSD,
Perkonigg et al. (2000) followed a German sample of
3021 individuals aged 14–24 and found that predictors
of trauma exposure included a pre-existing anxiety disorder (odds ratio (OR) 1.3–3.0) or substance use disorder (OR 3–7); once trauma had occurred, the
strongest predictors of PTSD symptoms were female
sex (OR 2–3) and assault or sexual trauma
(OR 2–4).
Many studies have now demonstrated that female
gender is a strong risk factor for the development of
PTSD (Stein, 2002). Though this gender difference is
fairly clear from an epidemiologic perspective, the mechanisms for this disparity are uncertain; they may involve
both differences in types of trauma exposure and differences in response to trauma. Conceivably, there may be
a biological basis to womenÕs apparent vulnerable to
certain kinds of traumatic stressors. Barr et al. (2004)
have shown that the serotonin transporter promoter
polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) modulates the effect of
early adversity in female—but not male—macaque monkeys. They speculate that women with the short (‘‘s’’) allele may be more susceptible to the effects of early
adversity, a mechanism that explains the increased risk
among women for certain stress-related syndromes such
as PTSD This work remains to be replicated and extended to humans, but it does pose a testable hypothesis
for gender differences in PTSD.
Regarding explanations that focus on differences in
type of exposure, violence at the hands of an intimate
partner is an epidemic problem that predominantly affects women; it contributes significantly to the burden
of PTSD in the female population. Plichta and Falik
(2001) used a nationally representative sample of 2850
American women from the Commonwealth FundÕs
1998 Survey of WomenÕs Health to estimate that over
40% of women had experienced some form of violence;
34.6% had experienced intimate partner violence, and
8% had been physically abused by their partners within
the previous 12 months. Victims of intimate partner violence often develop psychiatric disorders. In this context, Bennice et al. (2003) suggested that sexual
violence was a greater risk factor than physical violence
for the development of PTSD symptoms. In a sample of
44 women who had been victims of intimate partner violence within the last 2 years, Stein and Kennedy (2001)
found that PTSD had a lifetime prevalence of 50%,
70
60
None
Low
Moderate
Severe
50
40
30
20
10
0
Men (n = 3,905)
Women (n = 4,558)
Intensity of emotional abuse
Fig. 2. Emotional abuse and gender in an HMO sample of adults
(Edwards VJ et al. Am J Psychiatry 2003;160:1453–60.)
4
C.B. Nemeroff et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research 40 (2006) 1–21
are similar in extent of personal injury or vehicular damage—may be experienced very differently by men and
women, perhaps in large part because of psychosocial
factors (e.g., spousal and/or community support) that
mediate the response to trauma. The existence of such
factors does not negate the epidemiological fact that
women are more at risk for PTSD after many types of
trauma exposure, but it may provide another mechanistic explanation for these differences.
The presence of PTSD adversely affects womenÕs
health and functioning in many domains (Dobie et al.,
2004; Ouimette et al., 2004). Walker et al. found that
in a sample of 1225 women belonging to an urban
HMO, health care costs were doubled among those with
high PTSD scores (P45 on the PTSD Symptom Checklist) (Walker et al., 2003). Better identification of PTSD
in primary care settings might well lead not only to
appropriate treatment but also to cost savings.
Do women differ from men in the extent to which
they have conditions comorbid with PTSD? In the National Comorbidity Survey, Kessler et al. (1995) found
that, compared with men with PTSD, women with
PTSD had lower rates of comorbid alcohol abuse or
dependence (27.9% vs 51.9%), drug abuse or dependence
(26.9% vs 34.5%), but higher rates of panic disorder
(12.6% vs 7.3%) and agoraphobia (22.4% vs 16.1%).
However, other data suggest that prior substance abuse
may increase the risks of trauma exposure and PTSD,
and that childhood maltreatment may increase the risk
of substance abuse (McCauley et al., 1997). Rates of social phobia, major depression, and dysthymia were similar, but a more recent study by Oquendo et al. (2003)
showed that among 156 inpatients with major depression, those with comorbid lifetime PTSD were more
likely to have attempted suicide than those without
comorbid PTSD (75% vs 54%; p 6 0.01), and among
the subgroup with both conditions, the risk of a suicide
attempt was higher among women than among men.
Finally, it is possible that there may be gender differences in response to drug treatment for PTSD. However,
an early study by Brady et al. (2000) showing that men
responded poorly to treatment with sertraline was not
confirmed by later studies using larger sample sizes.
The metaphor of flexibility and elasticity is also apparent in mental health definitions, which involve the
capacity to bounce back, withstand hardship and repair
oneself (Wolin, 1993), or to master cycles of disruption
and reintegration (Flach, 1980). Psychiatric resilience
can be defined as resistance to and rapid recovery from
psychiatric illness. A wealth of elements that comprise
resilience has been proposed in the literature—including
active problem-solving, responsibility, self-esteem, independence, well-being, initiative, humor, insight, creativity, and many others. Measuring these concepts and
understanding their respective roles presents a formidable challenge.
Disasters tend to be random events that expose unselected populations to trauma. Thus, they offer unique
opportunities for researchers interested in studying risk
and resilience to disentangle the confounding issue of
pre-existing risk for exposure to traumatic events. Within a given community, individuals who are highly exposed to a traumatic experience—who are directly in
harmÕs way—will be distressed and challenged by their
experience, but only some of them will develop PTSD.
A second group of individuals are indirectly exposed
to the trauma—they may have lost jobs or had electrical
and water utilities cut off, or sustained minor property
damage. Finally, if the traumatic event is massive, many
individuals outside the particular community may be
remotely affected, as much of the American population
was affected after September 11, 2001 (Fig. 3). Each of
these groups deserves separate formal intervention.
PTSD and other psychiatric disorders are the domain
of a medical model that deals with disease and subsumes
wellness models that focus on restoring homeostasis.
These models need not be in conflict with each other;
COMMUNITY
INDIVIDUALS
directly
exposed
3. Risk and resilience factors after disasters and terrorism
Resilience has been addressed since ancient times by
authors as diverse as Confucius (‘‘Our greatest glory is
not in never falling, but in rising every time we fall’’)
and Nietzsche (‘‘That which does not kill us can only
make us stronger’’). Merriam-WebsterÕs Collegiate Dictionary (M-W Collegiate Dictionary, 1993) defines resilience as: (1) the capability of a strained body to recover
its size and shape after deformation and (2) an ability to
recover from or adjust easily to misfortune or change.
indirectly
exposed
SOCIETY
remotely
affected
PTSD
Medical
model
distress
challenge
Wellness
model
Fig. 3. Approaches to the mental health effects of disasters and
terrorism (Reprinted from Psychiatric Clinics of North America (in
press), North CS. Approaching disaster mental health research after
the 9/11 World Trade Center terrorist attacks. Copyright Ó 2004, with
permission from Elsevier. Also in Neria Y, Gross R, Marshall R,
Susser E (editors): September 11, 2001: Treatment, Research and
Public Mental Health in the Wake of a Terrorist Attack. New York:
Cambridge University Press, 2004; in press.)
C.B. Nemeroff et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research 40 (2006) 1–21
together they form a comprehensive whole that addresses the effects of disasters, both negative (distress,
symptoms, disease) and positive (personal challenge
and growth).
Epidemiological studies that attempt to estimate the
level of psychopathology in a population after a disaster
often neglect important aspects of the PTSD diagnosis.
Many popular questionnaires do not determine the sufficiency of exposure to the traumatic event, separate new
from pre-existing symptoms, or inquire into the duration of the symptoms or the duration or extent of the
resulting disability. Epidemiological estimates of prevalence of PTSD may be inflated as a result.
North et al. (1999) used structured diagnostic interviews to assess 182 adult survivors of the bombing of
the Alfred P. Murrah Federal Building in Oklahoma
City. Interviewees were selected randomly from the
Oklahoma City Health DepartmentÕs bombing registry.
Of these individuals, 87% reported injuries, and 77% had
required medical intervention for them. At 6 months
after the disaster, even in this highly exposed group,
55% had no psychiatric diagnosis. PTSD was seen in
36% of this population, and 2% of these survivors had
PTSD related to traumatic events other than the bombing. Development of PTSD was generally rapid—76% of
cases began on the day of the bombing, 94% within the
first week, and 98% within the first month. The strongest
predictors of PTSD were female sex (45% vs 23%;
p = 0.002) and a pre-disaster history of psychiatric illness (45% vs 26%; p = 0.009). Over half of those with
PTSD had a comorbid psychiatric diagnosis, usually
major depression. The second most common psychiatric
diagnosis was major depression, seen in 23% of the sample; 78% of those with prior history of major depression
had recurrent or persistent major depression at the time
of assessment. All cases of substance abuse seen in this
sample had developed prior to the bombing, and no
new cases were seen.
Individuals with PTSD reported widespread social
and occupational dysfunction. For example, 52% of
those with PTSD alone and 87% of those with PTSD
and a comorbid diagnosis reported some type of functional interference, compared with 27% of those with a
non-PTSD diagnosis and 16% of those with no psychiatric diagnosis.
Over the first 6 months after the disaster, 79% of the
study participants met the DSM-III-R criterion for
group B symptoms (intrusive memories, dreams or
nightmares of the event, flashbacks, and being upset
by reminders), and 82% met the criterion for group D
symptoms (insomnia, difficulty in concentrating, irritability, hypervigilance, and being jumpy or easily startled). However, only 36% met the criteria for group C
symptoms (avoidance, psychogenic amnesia, detachment or estrangement, loss of interest, restricted affect,
and a sense of shortened future). Of those who met
5
the group C symptom criteria, 94% had a diagnosis of
PTSD. The presence of group C symptoms also predicted other diagnoses, the presence of comorbid conditions, interference with activities, dissatisfaction with
work performance, and receiving mental health treatment (Fig. 4).
The above results suggest that a simplified algorithm
could be constructed for mental health assessment and
triage in the aftermath of a disaster. The first step would
be to assess for PTSD, the most common diagnosis; if
the population at risk is too large for full assessment
of everyone, this task may be facilitated by screening
for group C criteria, which constitute a marker for the
likelihood of PTSD. The second step would be to assess
for other diagnoses, whose presence may be critical to
selection and outcome of treatment. Finally, treatment
would be selected based on the diagnosis (Fig. 5).
A recent analysis of 160 samples of disaster victims
(Norris et al., 2002) indicated that impairment and adverse outcomes were associated with female gender,
ethnic minorities, youthfulness, prior psychiatric problems, secondary stressors, inadequate psychosocial resources, developing rather than developed countries,
severity of exposure, and mass violence (e.g., terrorism, shooting sprees) rather than natural or technological disasters.
What about the resilience of those in the larger community—the intended target of demoralization and
intimidation by terrorist groups—who are indirectly affected by disasters? While individuals directly exposed to
the September 11 disaster have not yet been studied as
were the Oklahoma City survivors, numerous studies
of indirectly exposed groups have found that 1–2
months after the attacks, 7.5% of a representative sample of the adult Manhattan population reported posttraumatic symptoms (Galea et al., 2002). A variety of
risk factors were identified, including prior stressors, residence near the World Trade Center, loss of possessions
or job due to the attacks, the death of a friend or relative
during the attack, and low levels of social support. At
least two national studies (Schlenger et al., 2002; Schuster et al., 2001) have reported an association between
extensive viewing of television coverage and substantial
stress reactions, although the causal directionalities of
this association have not been elucidated. A Pew poll
(Associated Press, 2001) found that a few days after
the attacks, 90% of a nationally representative sample
found that 7 of 10 reported feeling depressed, half had
trouble concentrating, and one third had disturbed
sleep. However, a longitudinal study by Silver et al.
(2002) found that the prevalence of posttraumatic stress
symptoms related to September 11 among the US population outside New York City declined from 17% at 2
months to 5.8% at 6 months. Not surprisingly, coping
strategies assessed shortly after the attacks were the
strongest predictors of posttraumatic stress symptoms.
6
C.B. Nemeroff et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research 40 (2006) 1–21
80
80
70
b
70
PTSD Criterion Group
*
Does not meet criteria
60
Meets criteria
50
40
30
20
% with predisaster disorder
% with comorbid postdisaster diagnosis
a
10
†
60
50
40
30
20
10
Group B
Group D
Group C
Group B
Group C Criteria Not Met
*
c
70
60
*
50
40
30
20
10
Group B
Group D
70
% reporting interference with activities
% receiving mental health treatment
80
Group D
Group C
Group C Criteria Not Met
*
d
60
*
50
40
30
†
20
10
Group C
Group B
Group C Criteria Not Met
Group D
Group C
Group C Criteria Not Met
70
% reporting dissatisfaction with
own work performance
e
*
60
*
50
40
30
20
10
Group B
Group D
Group C
Group C Criteria Not Met
Fig. 4. Diagnosis, treatment and functional indicators associated with PTSD criterion groups. (a) Comorbid postdisaster diagnosis. (b) Predisaster
disorder. (c) Receiving mental health treatment. (d) Interference with activities. (e) Dissatisfaction with work performance. Criterion group
categories are not mutually exclusive, except for categories group B and group D, which exclude those who met group C criteria. An asterisk
indicates p < 0.001; a dagger, p < 0.01 (North CS et al. JAMA 1999;282(8):755–62. Copyright Ó 1999 by the American Medical Association,
reprinted with permission.)
Avoidance and numbing symptoms, reflecting inability
to cope effectively, were associated with several indicators of problem functioning; these findings were consis-
tent with those of a study of a mass shooting episode in
which early coping problems were found to predict
PTSD 3 years later (North et al., 2001).
C.B. Nemeroff et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research 40 (2006) 1–21
STEP 1
STEP 2
STEP 3
ASSESS
for PTSD
ASSESS
OTHER
DIAGNOSES
SELECT
TREATMENT
BASED ON
DIAGNOSES
SCREEN for
C symptoms
DSM-IV
diagnosis
DSM-IV
diagnosis
PTSD “plus”
PTSD
PTSD only
No PTSD
Other diagnosis
No diagnosis
Medication: SSRI
Rx other disorders
Psychotherapy
Education/reassurance
Fig. 5. Flow chart for mental health assessment and triage after
disasters (North CS, Pfefferbaum B, Hong BA. P-FLASH. Disaster
mental health training program presented for the Mental Health
Associations of New York City and New Jersey, 2003–2004.
How can a communityÕs resilience to disasters be increased? Policies and practices that address this question need to be empirically driven, tailoring the
interventions to the needs of the individuals being
served, rather than the ideologies of service providers.
It is essential to recognize and treat the subset of community members with psychiatric illnesses—facilitated
by screening for group C symptoms—and to find ways
to assist people who have group B and D symptoms
(which are nearly ubiquitous among high-exposure
groups) without pathologizing them. Mass cognitive
reframing could help to correct widespread cognitive
distortions and substitute more rational and adaptive
thoughts. In this regard, the potential for positive effects of disasters should not be overlooked. Amazingly,
many participants in disaster studies have reported
favorable outcomes; a review by McMillen (1999) reported strengthened spiritual or religious beliefs in
15–40% and improvements in interpersonal relationships with family or friends in 10–40%. Finally, a focus
on community cohesiveness may also be useful to help
people pull together their individual resilience for the
good of the greater community.
4. Early life trauma and PTSD
Until the last decade, the hypothesis that early life
trauma is associated with an increased risk of adult
mood and anxiety disorders was supported largely by
anecdotal reports inspired by psychoanalytic concepts
of early critical periods of development. Research on
the biology of depression and some anxiety disorders
has commonly been plagued by the confounding factor
of early life stress—the hypercortisolemia and structural
brain changes attributed solely to the disorder is likely
due, at least in part, to the biological sequelae of early
life trauma.
7
If early trauma is indeed a principal risk factor for later depression and anxiety disorders, one would expect
the baseline prevalence of such trauma histories to be
very high. However, prevalence data are surprisingly
sparse, being derived mainly from small samples or
spontaneous reports of trauma from social service
departments or hospital emergency rooms. We do know
that reported cases constitute a relatively small fraction
of all cases, and that although prevalence estimates of
childhood abuse and other traumas such as early loss
of parents are extremely approximate, they are indeed
sufficient to account in part for the high prevalence of
depression and anxiety disorders among the general
adult population.
In a retrospective study involving 17,337 adult HMO
members, Dube et al. (2001) examined the relationship
between adverse childhood experiences and later suicide
attempts. In this population, whose mean age was 57
years and which was 54% female, the lifetime prevalence
of any suicide attempt was 3.8%, but adverse childhood
experiences of any type (emotional, physical, or sexual
abuse, substance abuse, mental illness or incarceration
of a family member, or parental domestic violence, separation or divorce) increased this risk 2- to 5-fold. Moreover, the greater the number of adverse experiences, the
greater the risk of suicide attempts; those with seven or
more such experiences had an adjusted odds ratio of
31.1 for a suicide attempt compared with those who
had none of these adverse experiences. McHolm et al.
(2003) found consistent results in a community sample
of 347 Canadian women aged 15–64 with major depressive disorder: suicidal ideation among them was most
strongly associated with a self-reported history of childhood physical abuse (OR 2.77; 95% CI = 1.26–6.12).
The striking results of a study by McCauley et al.
(1997) have also received a great deal of attention,
though it has been criticized for the potential confound
of retrospective reporting. This cross-sectional survey of
1,931 women from all socioeconomic classes, drawn
from four community-based primary care internal medicine practices, found a 22% prevalence of reported
childhood or adolescent physical or sexual abuse. Compared with the remainder of the sample, those with
childhood, but not adult, abuse histories reported significantly more physical symptoms (mean 6.2 vs 4.0;
p < 0.001), as well as significantly higher Symptom
Checklist-22 scores for depression, anxiety, somatization, and interpersonal sensitivity, a 4.7-fold higher
prevalence of drug abuse, a 2.2-fold higher alcohol
abuse, a 3.7-fold higher history of suicide attempts,
and a 3.2-fold higher risk of psychiatric admission to
hospital.
More recently, Safren et al. (2002) found that patients
with panic disorder were significantly more likely to
have histories of childhood physical or sexual abuse
than those with social phobia, while the prevalence of
8
C.B. Nemeroff et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research 40 (2006) 1–21
such histories was intermediate among patients with
generalized anxiety disorders; these findings held regardless of the presence or absence of comorbid anxiety or
depression. Such results suggest that, consistent with
other studies (Engel et al., 1993; Bremner et al.,
1993a,b), early abuse is a factor in the later development
of anxiety disorders as well as depression.
A suggested model of vulnerability to major depressive episodes, based on both animal and clinical studies,
posits that genetic factors, temperament, and trauma
early and later in life markedly increase the risk of
depression. When superimposed on this background of
risk, stressful life events and other recent difficulties trigger a major depressive episode. Many of these effects are
mediated by corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF),
which plays a key role in modulating the autonomic, immune, and behavioral effects of stress; increases in CRF
are associated with increased symptoms of depression
and anxiety. Indeed, these findings are consistent with
the repeated demonstration that mood and anxiety disorders are frequently comorbid—if not syndromally,
then certainly in terms of dimensional measures of
symptom severity. In fact, depression and anxiety share
common effective treatments, such as SSRIs, dual serotonin-norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors, and certain
forms of psychotherapy.
Is this general model consistent with preclinical
findings linking early trauma with hypothalamic-pituitaryadrenal (HPA) axis functioning? In a rat model of neglect, rat pups were removed from their mothers for 3
h daily between the ages of 2–14 days, and then returned
to their mothers in the animal colony for a week before
weaning; this naturalistic stressor is thought to be analogous to neglect in human childhood up to the age of
4–5 years. Pihoker et al. (1993) found that rat pups subjected to this type of maternal deprivation at the age of
10 days had significant reductions in median eminence
CRF concentrations after 24 h, interpreted as an increase in hypothalamic CRF release. However, the effect
was seen only during a critical time window—maternal
deprivation did not reduce CRF concentrations in older,
18-day old rat pups.
The effects of early maternal deprivation appear to be
remarkably persistent (Heim et al., 2004). Ladd et al.
(1996) found that adult male rats isolated from their
mothers for 6 h daily from postnatal days 2–20 (before
weaning) showed increases in ACTH concentrations,
both basal and induced by a mild foot shock. This effect
appeared to be mediated by large increases in CRF concentrations in the median eminence of the hypothalamus, where CRF and mRNA expression were greatly
increased compared to unstressed control animals. The
increase in CRF mRNA expression was also seen extrahypothalamically—for example, in the central nucleus
of the amygdala and in the parabrachial region of the locus coeruleus. Moreover, direct measurements of CRF
concentrations in cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) also demonstrated increases over levels in control animals.
Other intriguing changes in maternally deprived rats
have also been noted. For example, Heim et al. (2004)
demonstrated that rats with histories of early maternal
deprivation displayed anhedonia, as indicated by lack
of reinforcement for choosing saccharin-sweetened
water over plain water. Neumaier et al. (2002) found increased 5HT1B mRNA expression in the dorsal raphe
nucleus; other pilot studies have demonstrated persistent
changes including reduced hippocampal neurogenesis
(unpublished observations), hippocampal mossy fiber
development (Huot et al., 2002) and GABAA receptor
binding (Caldji et al., 2000). Gene chips are currently
being used to further characterize changes in gene
expression in maternally deprived animals.
What about behavioral alterations in maternally deprived rats? A hypersensitive startle to an acoustic stimulus is a well-validated marker of hyperarousal,
analogous to the exaggerated startle response seen in human adults with PTSD. Plotsky et al found that maternally deprived animals showed increased acoustic startle
responses compared to non-deprived and minimally separated animals (unpublished observations). Maternally
deprived animals have also been shown to prefer alcohol–sucrose to water–sucrose solutions (Huot et al.,
2001), though total fluid intake was unchanged.
Interestingly, drug treatment of maternally deprived
rats with paroxetine (via implantable mini-pump) has
been shown to normalize—at least partially—several
of the trauma-associated perturbations, both biochemical and behavioral (Plotsky et al., unpublished observations). For example, paroxetine reduced CRF mRNA
expression in the paraventricular nucleus, bed nucleus
of the stria terminalis, and central nucleus of the amygdala, reduced CRF concentrations in cerebrospinal
fluid, normalized pituitary CRF receptor binding, attenuated the exaggerated ACTH response to startle, reversed the blunted ACTH response to exogenous CRF
challenge, and decreased peripheral corticosterone levels. In contrast, paroxetine had no significant effects
on these parameters in nondeprived rats. Drug treatment also normalized behaviors such as the reduced
preference for sucrose and increased preference for ethanol (Huot et al., 2001), as well as anxiety-like behavior.
The time course of some of these effects is instructive: 3–
4 weeks were required for the full effect of paroxetine on
CRF mRNA levels in the paraventricular nucleus. However, CRF mRNA expression returned to its abnormally
high level 14 days after discontination of the drug. Pilot
studies with reboxetine and mirtazepine have revealed
similar effects of these antidepressants (unpublished
observations).
Non-human primate models of maternal deprivation
have shown a similar pattern of results. Coplan et al.
(1996) found that infant bonnet macaques reared by
9
C.B. Nemeroff et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research 40 (2006) 1–21
mothers who faced variable and unpredictable foraging
conditions had later persistent and significant elevations
in CSF concentrations of CRF (Fig. 6), as well as decreases in CSF concentrations of cortisol. The effects
were not noted among offspring of mothers who faced
high or low foraging demands, likely because these demands were predictable.
Can these preclinical findings on HPA axis responses
to early life trauma be extended to the clinical arena? We
believe that they can, but one must do so cautiously. In a
prospective study of 49 women, Heim et al. (2000) scrutinized four groups aged 18–45: those with no early trauma history or current psychiatric illness (controls); those
with a trauma history only; those with a current episode
of major depression only; and those with both major
depression and early life trauma. Approximately 85%
of the last group also fulfilled the diagnostic criteria
for PTSD, while the rate of PTSD was 36% among those
with early trauma without major depression. These women were exposed to a standard laboratory stressor, the
Trier Social Stress Test, in which participants are assigned an oral presentation and mental arithmetic tasks.
This stressor reproducibly raised plasma cortisol concentrations in all groups including controls, but the increases were much more dramatic among the group
with both early life trauma and major depression
(Fig. 7). Whether or not they were depressed, women
with early life trauma also had a much greater ACTH
response to stress than controls or those with major
depression only (Fig. 7). In addition, heart rates increased modestly in controls but dramatically in subjects
with early life trauma and major depression; the magniGroup effect: p < 0.0001
VFD > LFD
VFD > HFD
140
130
*
CSF CR ,F pg/ml
120
110
100
90
80
70
60
*
50
VFD
LFD
HFD
Fig. 6. Levels of CSF CRF among infant bonnet macaques reared
under different foraging conditions. Scatterplot showing CSF CRF
concentrations in grown bonnet macaques whose mothers were
exposed to low, high, and variable foraging demands when subjects
were infants. Pooled data are expressed as means + SD; *not used for
the determination of mean group concentrations. VFD, variable
foraging demand; LFD, low foraging demand; HFD, high foraging
demand (Coplan JD et al. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1996;93:1619–23.
Copyright Ó 1996 National Academy of Sciences, USA.)
ACTH (pg/ml)
70
60
50
40
30
20
10
STRESS
0
-15
a
0 15 30 45 60 75 90
Time in minutes
CON (n=12)
ELS/non-MDD (n=14)
ELS/MDD (n=13)
non-ELS/MDD (n=10)
CORT (g/dl)
20
18
16
14
12
10
8
6
4
2
STRESS
0
-15 0 15 30 45 60 75 90
b
Time in minutes
Fig. 7. Trier Social Stress Test: Plasma ACTH and cortisol (a). (b)
Mean plasma adrenocorticotropin (ACTH) (a) and cortisol (b)
concentrations (±SEMs) before, during (shaded area), and after
psychosocial stress induction in women without a history of significant
early-life stress and no psychiatric disorder (controls, n = 12), women
with a history of childhood sexual or physical abuse without major
depression (early-life stress and no major depression [ELS/non-MDD],
n = 14), women with a history of childhood sexual or physical abuse
and current major depression (early-life stress and major depression
[ELS/MDD], n = 13), and women without a history of significant
early-life stress and a current major depression (no early-life stress and
major depression [non-ELS/MDD], n = 10). To convert ACTH from
pg/mL to pmol/L, multiply by 0.22. To convert cortisol from mg/dL to
nmol/L, multiply by 27.59. Asterisk indicates p < 0.05 for controls vs
ELS/non-MDD; p < 0.05 for the controls vs ELS/MDD; àp < 0.05 for
EKS/non-MDD vs non-ELS/MDD; §p < 0.01 for controls vs EKS/
MDD; ip < 0.01 for ELS/MDD vs nonELS/MDD; –p < 0.01 for ELS/
non-MDD vs ELS/MDD; #p < 0.05 for EKS/non-MDD vs ELS/
MDD; and **p < 0.05 for ELS/MDD vs non-ELS/MDD. (Heim C
et al. JAMA 2000;284:592–7. Copyright Ó 2000 by the American
Medical Association; reprinted with permission.)
tude of the heart rate response was intermediate in the
other two groups. These results are consistent with a
model in which the stress system is sensitized, and subsequent stresses in adult life lead to markedly hyperactive responses. In a case-control study of 27
medication-free adults with major depression and 25
matched controls, Carpenter et al. (2004) also found
that perceived early life stress was a significant predictor
of CSF CRF concentrations in both normal volunteers
and depressed subjects.
Recently, Caspi et al. (2003) reported that individuals
either homozygous (s/s) or heterozygous (s/l) for the
short arm of the serotonin transporter (SERT) promoter
are at considerably greater risk than individuals with the
l/l genotype for the development of major depression if
exposed to trauma early in life. Moreover, there was a
dose–response relationship for risk, such that individuals with the s/s genotype were at greater risk than those
with the s/I genotype.
Several clinical studies have linked elevated CSF
CRF concentrations not only with depression (Raadsheer et al., 1994; Banki et al., 1987; Nemeroff, 1989),
but also with PTSD. For example, Bremner et al.
(1997a,b) studied 11 Vietnam veterans with PTSD and
10
C.B. Nemeroff et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research 40 (2006) 1–21
found significant elevations in CSF CRF compared to
controls; these elevations were seen in veterans with or
without comorbid major depression. Baker et al.
(1999) subsequently measured sequentially sampled
CSF cortisol concentrations and 24-h urinary free cortisol excretion in 11 combat veterans with PTSD and 12
matched control subjects, and found that CSF CRF
concentrations were significantly higher among the combat veterans, though CRF levels were not correlated
with PTSD symptomatology. In contrast, 24-h urinary
free cortisol excretion was similar in the two groups.
Interestingly, however, cortisol excretion was inversely
correlated with PTSD symptoms.
Finally, what implications do these results have for
treatment selection? Keller et al. (2000) were among
the first to compare directly the efficacy of antidepressant (nefazodone) treatment, psychotherapy, and the
combination of the two in the treatment of chronic
depression. This study involved 681 adults with nonpsychotic major depressive episodes at least two years long
(mean duration 7.5–8.0 years) and scores P20 on the 24item Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HAM-D).
Many also had long-standing dysthymia, anxiety disorders, substance abuse disorders or personality disorders,
and had undergone prior treatment with antidepressants, psychotherapy, or both. These patients were
randomized to receive either the cognitive-behavioralanalysis system of psychotherapy (CBASP) (McCullough, 1984), delivered in 16–20 sessions over 12 weeks,
or nefazodone (mean dose 460–466 mg daily), or both
treatments. After 12 weeks, remission rates (HAM-D
6 8) among study completers were 22% in the nefazodone group, 24% in the CBASP group, and 42% in the
combined group (p < 0.001 vs either single modality)
(Fig. 8). These findings were consistent with the wide-
Change from baseline in mean Ham-D scores
30
Nefazodone
Psychotherapy
Nefazodone and psychotherapy
25
Score
20
15
10
5
0
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10 11 12
Week of study
Fig. 8. Nefazodone Chronic Depression Study. For baseline through
week 4, p < 0.001 for combined treatment vs psychotherapy (CBASP),
p = 0.004 for nefazodone vs CBASP, p = 0.39 for combined treatment
vs nefazodone. For weeks 4 through 12, p < 0.001 for combined
treatment vs nefazodone and for CBASP vs nefazodone, and p = 0.06
for combined treatment vs CBASP. (Keller MB et al. N Engl J Med
2000;342:1462–70. Copyright Ó 2000 Massachusetts Medical Society.
All rights reserved.)
spread clinical impression that combination treatment
is superior to single modalities.
A secondary analysis (Nemeroff et al., 2003) of the
data set from the foregoing study revealed several novel
findings. First, there is an extremely high prevalence of
early life trauma in this chronically depressed population. Only about one-third of the patients had no trauma history. Overall, 32–35% had suffered parental loss
before age 15 years, 40–48% physical abuse, 15–19% sexual abuse and 7–12% neglect. Patients with histories of
trauma had dramatically poorer responses to antidepressant treatment than to psychotherapy or combined
treatment. The differences in remission rates were even
more striking—32% on antidepressant therapy, 48%
on psychotherapy, and approximately 52% on combined
therapy. Given that treatment of maternally deprived
animals with SSRIs can reverse many of the adverse biochemical and behavioral effects of trauma, it would be
desirable to conduct a similar study with an SSRI in humans; a trial involving fluoxetine is currently underway.
5. What can imaging studies of depression teach us about
failed adaptation to chronic emotional stress and anxiety
disorders?
Research on functional brain imaging in depression
(and on the biology of depression in general) has ‘‘matured’’ much more than similar research in PTSD. Not
only can we learn a great deal from the depression literature that is relevant to PTSD, but it is now well established that these syndromes are frequently comorbid,
and that certain antidepressants, the SSRIs in particular, are effective for both conditions.
Depression may be conceptualized as an end-product
of failed adaptation to chronic emotional stress. The
individual, who begins with a given set of biological
risks (e.g., female sex, certain gene polymorphisms, temperament, and reactivity of the HPA axis) is subjected to
a variety of exogenous stressors, which act to destabilize
the usual homeostatic state of the brain circuits that are
involved in mood regulation. The result is a depressive
episode, and the goal of treatment is to restabilize the
key brain circuits. Defining these brain circuits and their
perturbations as precisely as possible by separating their
putative components experimentally is a major research
challenge.
Do depressed patients differ from healthy individuals
in their acute adaptive responses to stress? Several studies have used provoked sadness and neuroimaging studies to determine changes in regional brain activity in
response to this sad mood challenge. Normal individuals, depressed patients, and those thought to be at risk
for depression may be studied in this way. Mayberg
et al. (1999) asked eight healthy right-handed euthymic
women to prepare scripts of sad personal experiences
C.B. Nemeroff et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research 40 (2006) 1–21
and then to allow themselves to feel sadness while
undergoing [15O]H2O positron emission tomography
(PET). Limbic-paralimbic blood flow was increased,
particularly in the subgenual cingulate (BrodmannÕs
area 25) and the anterior insula, while neocortical blood
flow (in the right dorsolateral prefrontal and inferior
parietal areas) was reciprocally decreased. This pattern
was consistent with the well-known clinical observation
that intense sadness is often accompanied by cognitive
difficulties.
A very different pattern was seen in depressed patients. Liotti et al. (2002a,b) compared regional blood
flow changes after the same sad mood challenge among
acutely depressed patients (mean HAM-D score 21.3),
euthymic patients who were in remission (mean HAM-D
score 4.8 without medications), and normal volunteers.
Decreases in blood flow in the medial orbitofrontal cortex (area 10/11) and a variety of other areas were virtually identical in both the remitted and symptomatic
depressed patients, and differed substantially from control subjects. Remitted patients (but not acutely depressed patients or controls) exhibited decreased blood
flow in the pregenual anterior cingulate (area 24a). Conversely, the depressed groups lacked the increase in area
25 and the decrease in right prefrontal cortex that were
observed in the controls. More recently, Kruger et al.
(2003) studied bipolar patients and found patterns of
cerebral blood flow in response to sad mood challenge
that were similar whether the patients were acutely depressed or in remission. Taken together, these results
suggest that the unique changes seen across patient subtypes, irrespective of clinical state, may be trait markers
of a depressive diathesis.
Can depressive illness also be conceived of as the
product of more chronic maladaptive changes to emotional triggers? Most depressed participants in neuroimaging studies are recruited well after the onset of
symptoms, and may present at various points in the
course of the brainÕs attempts at adaptation. Moreover,
patients recruited from tertiary care centers are likely to
be different from those recruited by newspaper advertising, and their clinical states can range from complete
failure of adaptation to partial adaptation or hypersensitive adaptations. The heterogeneity of neuroimaging
results reported in the literature is therefore not surprising. In fact, this baseline variability may be a potential
clue to understanding differences in both the clinical
and brain response patterns across different treatment
modalities (Mayberg, 2003).
In a placebo-controlled, double-blind trial, Mayberg
et al. (2000a,b) explored changes in brain glucose metabolism using PET in unipolar depressed male inpatients
after 1 week and 6 weeks of treatment with fluoxetine.
Patients receiving active fluoxetine had a similar pattern
of changes in glucose metabolism after 1 week of treatment: increases in the hippocampus and brainstem and
11
decreases in the posterior cingulate, striatum, and thalamus. By 6 weeks, however, fluoxetine responders could
be distinguished from non-responders by decreases in
limbic and striatal glucose metabolism (subgenual cingulate, hippocampus, insula, and pallidum) and reciprocal increases in brainstem and dorsal cortical areas
(prefrontal, parietal, anterior, and posterior cingulate).
In contrast, the patterns seen during the first week persisted in the non-responders. Interestingly, regional
changes were not merely corrections of pretreatment
abnormalities. Some regions showed changes in the
direction of reversing baseline abnormalities, while others were new effects in regions showing apparently normal pretreatment activity. These findings suggest that
successful treatment is not a simple matter of absolute
changes, but instead entails a more complex adaptation
of multiple brain regions to a new setpoint. Conversely,
clinical treatment failure may be the result of an inability
to institute or maintain these adaptations. The metabolic changes with response were not specific to fluoxetine treatment, because they were also noted among the
placebo responders in the study. The time course and
localization of these changes were also consistent with
the data obtained by several groups on upregulation
of the cyclic AMP second messenger system and increased expression of brain-derived neurotrophic factor
(BDNF) in association with antidepressant treatment
(Vaidya et al., 1997; Duman et al., 1999; Chen et al.,
2001). Finally, the changes appear to persist over time;
decreased activity in area 25 was seen 18 months later
(Mayberg, 2003; Liotti et al., 2000) (Fig. 9—Mayberg
et al., 2000a,b; Mayberg et al., 2002; Liotti et al.,
2002a,b; Stefurak and Mayberg, 2003; Goldapple
et al., 2004a,b).
Kennedy et al. (2001) carried out a similar study of
brain glucose metabolism among 13 depressed male patients before and after 6 weeks of therapy with paroxetine 20–40 mg daily, comparing the patterns obtained
with those of 24 healthy male volunteers. All patients responded to treatment (HAM-D scores reduced by at
least 50%). With successful treatment, glucose metabolism increased in the prefrontal cortex, parietal cortex,
and dorsal anterior cingulate, and decreased in left anterior and posterior insula (though it remained higher
than in controls) and right hippocampus and parahippocampal regions. Anterior cingulate metabolism, already
hyperactive before treatment began, increased even further after treatment. Reanalysis of the data obtained by
Kennedy et al showed that the subgenual cingulate (area
25) was also suppressed with paroxetine treatment
(Fig. 9).
Changes in subgenual cingulate may be central to
recovery from depression due to its very high concentrations of serotonin transporters, a marker of presynaptic
serotonin-containing nerve terminal density, and a primary target of SSRIs. This region also has reciprocal
12
C.B. Nemeroff et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research 40 (2006) 1–21
Fig. 9. Common limbic cortical changes with pharmacotherapy. ((a) Mayberg HS et al. Biol Psychiatry 2000;48:830–43. (b) Mayberg HS et al. Am J
Psychiatry 2002;159:728–37. (c) Liotti M et al. Am Psychiatry 2002;159:1830–40. (d) Stefurak T et al. In: Bedard MA et al. (editors). Mental and
behavioral dysfunction in movement disorders, Humana Press; 2003, p. 321–38. (e) Goldapple K et al. Arch Gen Psych 2004;61:34–41.)
cortical and subcortical connections to the hippocampus, hypothalamus, amygdale, brainstem, and frontal
cortex—regions involved in regulating circadian and
autonomic as well as cognitive functions disturbed during a major depressive episode (Freedman et al., 2000).
Modulation of this area therefore has the potential for
broad cortical effects (Ongur et al., 1998; Barbas et al.,
2003).
It might be tempting to conclude that successful treatment of depression occurs through a prerequisite final
pathway—cortical normalization and limbic suppression. This is apparently not the case. Goldapple et al.
(2004a,b) recruited 17 depressed patients with no
comorbid psychiatric illnesses and studied changes in
cerebral metabolism before and after they received 15–
20 sessions of cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT).
Fourteen patients completed the study and all had significant clinical improvement (mean posttreatment
HAM-D score 6.7). The metabolic changes among these
patients were in many regions precisely the opposite of
those seen with paroxetine treatment: increases in the
hippocampus and dorsal cingulate and decreases in the
dorsal, ventral, and medial frontal cortex. Dorsal cingulate and medial frontal regions were unique targets of
CBT. Similarly, the subgenual cortex, thalamus, and
brainstem were targeted with medication but were unaffected by the CBT (Fig. 10). The decreases in cortical
metabolism are consistent with the idea that CBT has
the effect of decreasing rumination about emotionally
salient aversive events and memories.
These results imply that instead of going through a
single final common pathway, different treatment
modalities modulate the system in different ways—drug
treatment primarily via a subcortical approach (‘‘bottom-up’’) and cognitive approaches primarily via a cortical approach (‘‘top-down’’; Fig. 11). They also suggest
that subtyping depression according to the brainÕs baseline metabolic state might be an effective method of
selecting the best treatment modality for a given
individual.
In an attempt to define common denominators of
successful treatments, Seminowicz et al. (2004) developed a simplified 7-region model derived from the
results of these treatment studies; this was mathematically tested using structural equation modeling. The
simplified model examining regional interactions of
medial frontal (MF10), orbital frontal (OF11), lateral
Attention-cognition
PF9 P40
CBT
-
mF9/10
Emotioncognition
integration
aCg24
bg
Mood
state
thal
oF11
CBT
SRI inverse
SRI only
CBT only
Fig. 10. Effects on cerebral glucose metabolic after treatment with
paroxetine or cognitive behavioral therapy. (Goldapple K et al. Arch
Gen Psychiatry 2004;61:34–41.)
pCg
hipp ocampus
Cg25 a-ins
-
am hth bs
drug
Vegetative-circadian
Fig. 11. Relationships among regions mediating CBT and drug
response. Schematic model of relationships among regions mediating
CBT and drug response, in which illness remits when critical common
targets (red areas) are modulated via either top-down effects of CBT
(green areas) or bottom-up effects of paroxetine (blue areas). PF9,
dorsolateral prefrontal; p40, inferior parietal; pCg, posterior cingulate;
oF11, orbital frontal; bg, basal ganglia; thal, thalamus; Cg25, ventral
subgenual cingulate; a-ins, anterior insula; am, amygdala; hth,
hypothalamus; bs, brainstem. Solid black lines and arrows = known
connections between areas; gray arrows = reciprocal changes with
treatment. (Goldapple K et al. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004;61:34–41.
Copyright Ó 2004. American Medical Association. All rights reserved.)
(For interpretation of the references to colour in this figure legend, the
reader is referred to the web version of this article.)
13
C.B. Nemeroff et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research 40 (2006) 1–21
6. Neural circuits, memory, and PTSD
The neural circuitry implicated in PTSD probably
involves complex interactions between the thalamus
(a gateway for sensory inputs), the hippocampus
(which is involved in short-term memory and probably
fear of the context of an event), the amygdala (which is
involved in conditioned fear responses), the posterior
cingulate, parietal and motor cortex (which are
involved in visuospatial processing and assessment of
threat), and the medial prefrontal cortex, including
the anterior cingulate, orbitofrontal, and subcallosal
gyrus (which is believed to extinguish more primitive
subcortical responses).
The effects of stress on the hippocampus have been
recognized since the 1990s, but the effects of stress on
neurogenesis are of more recent vintage: Woolley et al.
(1990) demonstrated that exposure of adult rats to excess corticosterone (equivalent to cortisol in humans) reduced dendritic branching in specific neuronal
populations of the hippocampus, suggesting early stages
of degeneration, and Gould et al. (1998) found decreased neurogenesis in socially subordinated non-human primates compared with dominant animals.
Moreover, Duman et al. (2001) demonstrated that
chronic administration of electroconvulsive therapy or
a variety of antidepressants, but not other psychotropic
drugs, to adult rodents upregulated hippocampal neurogenesis, Santarelli et al. (2003) found that irradiation of
the hippocampus in mice prevented the neurogenic and
behavioral effects of fluoxetine or imipramine, suggesting that hippocampal neurogenesis may be one of the
mechanisms of action of antidepressants in the treatment of stress-related disorders.
Bremner et al. (1993a,b) used neuropsychological
testing (paragraph recall) as a probe of hippocampal
function in 26 Vietnam veterans with combat-related
PTSD and 15 matched healthy control subjects. Compared to controls, the combat veterans had decreased
immediate and delayed recall as well as percent retention, but IQ scores were similar in the two groups.
Bremner et al. (1995a,b) also found similar results
among adult survivors of severe childhood physical or
sexual abuse who presented for psychiatric treatment.
The results of these studies introduced the possibility
that stress in humans may also lead to hippocampal
damage (Bremner, 1998; Pitman, 2001; Bremner,
2002). The first neuroimaging study in PTSD was carried out using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) to
measure hippocampal volumes in 26 Vietnam combat
veterans with PTSD and 22 matched control subjects.
Mean right hippocampal volume was 8% smaller in
the veterans than in the control subjects (1,184 mm3 vs
1,286 mm3; p < 0.05; Fig. 12), and was correlated with
short-term memory deficits on the Wechsler Memory
Scale (r = 0.64; p < 0.05). Decreases in right hippocampal volume in the PTSD patients were associated with
Hippocampal volume (mm 3)
prefrontal (PF9), subgenual cingulate (Cg25), anterior
cingulate (Cg24), hippocampus (hc), and thalamus
(thal) activity prior to treatment succeeded in accommodating all patient cohorts tested—a total of 119
patients from two institutions. Patients who went on
to respond to CBT showed a mainly cortical pattern
of aberrant brain connections at baseline, while drug
responders showed a combined limbic-cortical pattern.
Patients who were eventual nonresponders to medication showed a third distinct baseline network pattern
suggestive of a more complex limbic-subcortical disturbance. The modeling results lay the groundwork
for future clinical trials examining treatment response
based on brain subtypes.
Finally, can the interactions between BrodmannÕs
area (BA) 25 and frontal cortex be useful in detecting
populations who are vulnerable to depression? Keightley et al. (2003) examined activation patterns in response
to sad mood challenge among healthy college students
with no personal or family history of depression who
were selected for resilience (low scores on the dispositional Depression facet of Neuroticism [N3] and high
scores on the Positive Emotions face of Extraversion
[E6] on the NEO Personality Inventory-Revised) or for
the reverse pattern—high N3 scores and low E6 scores.
In response to the mood challenge, all subjects had decreases in lateral frontal cortical activation and increases
in the activation of BA 25, but the high-N3 subjects had
low medial frontal activation that resembled the pattern
seen in depressed patients. These data suggest that this
paradigm is highly sensitive to risk of depression, a
hypothesis that requires testing in other vulnerable subject groups.
The foregoing results illustrate the value of a meticulous dissection of the different neural circuits involved in
psychiatric illness and its treatment. While this approach
may initially appear to be oversimplified, it has the
advantage of allowing the complex mechanisms that
underlie vulnerability, treatment, and relapse to be
clarified.
1300
1280
1260
1240
1220
1200
1180
1160
1140
1120
Controls (n=22)
PTSD (n=26)
*
Left
hippocampus
Right
hippocampus
Fig. 12. Decreased right hippocampal volume in combat-related
PTSD *p < 0.05 (Bremner JD et al. Am J Psychiatry 1995;152:973–81.)
C.B. Nemeroff et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research 40 (2006) 1–21
deficits in short-term memory. Left hippocampal volume was also decreased in the veterans, but the difference did not reach statistical significance (Bremner
et al., 1995a,b). Findings of smaller hippocampal volume and/or a reduction in N-acetylaspartate (NAA) in
the hippocampus—a marker of neuronal integrity—in
adults with chronic, long-standing PTSD have been replicated several times in the published literature (Gurvits
et al., 1996; Bremner et al., 1997a,b; Stein et al., 1997a,b;
Freeman et al., 1998; Schuff et al., 2001; Bremner et al.,
2002; Villareale et al., 2002).
Using [15O]H2O PET, Bremner et al. (1999a,b) measured cerebral blood flow in 22 women with histories
of childhood sexual abuse while they listened to scripts
that were either neutral or traumatic (personalized accounts of childhood sexual abuse events). Compared
with those without PTSD, the traumatic scripts evoked
increased blood flow in the posterior cingulate, anterior
prefrontal cortex, and motor cortex, but decreased
blood flow in the right hippocampus, visual association
cortex, and the subcallosal gyrus (area 25) and adjacent
anterior cingulate (area 32) of the medial prefrontal cortex. In tandem with this, women with PTSD (but not
those without the diagnosis) had increased PTSD symptom scores as they listened to the traumatic scripts. This
pattern of activation was similar to that observed in
Vietnam veterans who were exposed to slides and
sounds evoking combat situations (Bremner et al.,
1999a,b). These results are consistent with a general
model of cortical failure to suppress exaggerated subcortical reactions to stress.
Are the hippocampal changes seen in PTSD actually
a function of the early childhood traumas seen in so
many PTSD patients, or are they specific to PTSD itself?
In a more recent study, Bremner et al. (2003) studied 33
women with early childhood sexual abuse and PTSD
(n = 10), early childhood sexual abuse without PTSD
(n = 12) and no abuse or PTSD (n = 11). Hippocampal
volumes (as measured with MRI) among the women
with abuse histories and PTSD were 16% smaller than
among those with abuse histories without PTSD, and
19% smaller than among the normal controls, with the
right hippocampal volume somewhat more diminished
than the left. The women were also given two tasks: to
count the number of times they heard the letter ‘‘d’’ in
a paragraph that was read aloud to them (control condition) and to recall as much as possible of a different paragraph they had been read (verbal memory encoding
task). In contrast to the women with abuse histories
without PTSD, the women with PTSD did not show increased blood flow to the hippocampus during the verbal encoding memory task. Thus, there was a failure
of left hippocampal activation with a memory task
among women with abuse-related PTSD, which remained significant after controlling for differences in
hippocampal volume (measured with MRI in the same
subjects) (Fig. 13). Shin et al. (2004) have also demonstrated a failure of hippocampal activation with a
declarative memory task in PTSD.
However, studies in children with PTSD (De Bellis
et al., 1999; Carrion et al., 2001; De Bellis et al., 2001)
and in adult subjects with new-onset PTSD (Bonne
et al., 2001; Notestine et al., 2002) have not demonstrated a smaller hippocampal volume, suggesting that
chronic PTSD is required for the effect. One study of
monozygotic twins discordant for exposure to trauma
found a correlation between PTSD symptoms and hippocampal volume in the unexposed twin, suggesting a
genetic contribution to smaller hippocampal volume
(Gilbertson et al., 2002). However, another unpublished
twin study of twins discordant for PTSD has shown a
pattern of hippocampal volume that is consistent with
a combined genetic and environmental effect.
85
Normalized hippocampal blood flow
14
84
83
82
81
80
79
78
77
76
75
Non-PTSD
control
Non-PTSD
encoding
PTSD
control
PTSD
encoding
Fig. 13. Failure of hippocampal activation encoding in women with
abuse-related PTSD *p < 0.05 (Bremner JD et al. Am J Psychiatry
2003;160:924–32. Copyright Ó 2003, the American Psychiatric Association. Available from: http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org. Reprinted by
permission.)
Fig. 14. Increased cortisol response to trauma-specific stress in PTSD.
(Elzinga BM et al. Neuropsychopharmacology 2003;28:1656–65. Copyright Ó 2003 Nature Publishing Group, reprinted with permission.)
C.B. Nemeroff et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research 40 (2006) 1–21
(a)
(b)
(c)
Fig. 15. Effects of 9–12 months of treatment with 10–40 mg/day
paroxetine in PTSD. (a) CAPS-2 symptom clusters (n = 23) (Reprinted
from Biol Psychiatry, vol. 54, Vermetten E et al. Long-term treatment
with paroxetine increases verbal declarative memory and hippocampal
volume in posttramatic stress disorder, 693–702, Copyright Ó 2003,
with permission from The Society of Biological Psychiatry.) (b) Effects
of paroxetine on hippocampal-based verbal declarative memory in
PTSD. Effects of 9–12 months of treatment with 10–40 mg paroxetine.
Mean 35% improvement; p < 0.05. Each line connects the individual
score before and after treatment. Horizontal lines indicate the mean
group score. (Reprinted from Biol Psychiatry, vol. 54, Vermetten E
et al. Long-term treatment with paroxetine increases verbal declarative
memory and hippocampal volume in posttramatic stress disorder, 693–
702, Copyright Ó 2003, with permission from The Society of Biological
Psychiatry) (c) Increased hippocampal volume with paroxetine in
PTSD. Effects of 9–12 months of treatment with 10–40 mg paroxetine—5% increase in volume. Asterisk indicates p < 0.05; n = 20.
(Vermetten E et al. Biol Psychiatry, 2003;54:693–702.)
Preclinical studies have suggested that stress-induced
lesions of the hippocampus attenuate its normal inhibitory effect on the HPA axis. The cascade of consequences
15
includes enhanced release of CRF from the hypothalamus, a blunted ACTH response to CRF challenge,
increased peripheral plasma cortisol concentrations,
and—similar to what is observed in depression—resistance to the usual suppressive effects of dexamethasone.
However, there are puzzling inconsistencies in findings
among patients with PTSD; for example, some studies—but not all—have found decreased urinary cortisol
levels (Mason et al., 1986; Yehuda et al., 1995) or
‘‘super’’ suppression of cortisol by low doses of dexamethasone (Yehuda et al., 1993; Stein et al., 1997a,b).
Elzinga et al. (2003) measured cortisol responses to
traumatic reminders (via personalized trauma script) in
24 women with abuse histories in the presence or absence
of PTSD. Cortisol concentrations in the patients with
PTSD were 60% higher during anticipation of the script
reading (from 20 min prior to the reading), 122% higher
during the reading and 69% higher during the recovery
phase (up to 75 min later; Fig. 14). Thus, PTSD appears
to involve low baseline cortisol levels, but an exaggerated
release of cortisol in response to a stressor.
Paroxetine has been shown to be highly effective in
the treatment of PTSD (Marshall et al., 2001; Marshall
et al., 1998; Tucker et al., 2001). Vermetten et al. (2003)
measured hippocampal volume and verbal declarative
memory (a hippocampus-based task) in 28 patients with
PTSD before and after 9–12 months of paroxetine treatment. Twenty-three patients completed the study, and
20 had MRI for assessment of hippocampal volume.
Paroxetine significantly reduced symptoms in all three
domains of the CAPS-2 (Fig. 15(a)), improved verbal
declarative memory (Fig. 15(b)) and increased left and
right hippocampal volume by 5.6% and 3.7%, respectively (p < 0.05; Fig. 15(c)). The effects of other medications on these parameters in PTSD remain to be
explored.
7. Cognitive behavioral approaches to the treatment of
PTSD: what works?
Like re-experiencing, hyperarousal and avoidance
symptoms, negative cognitions are ubiquitous immediately after an individual suffers a trauma (Foa and
Jaycox, 1999). Usually, however, subsequent everyday
life experiences gradually correct these negative cognitions, and the traumatized individual has the opportunity to regain a sense of competence and safety in the
world. In contrast, those who make extensive use of
avoidance and numbing will also avoid the very experiences that could have corrected their cognitive distortions. Thus, these individuals may be at higher risk for
the development of PTSD.
Foa and Jaycox (1999) have suggested that two particular groups of erroneous cognitions are associated
16
C.B. Nemeroff et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research 40 (2006) 1–21
with the later development of PTSD: that the world is
extremely dangerous (for example, that no place is safe
and that people are untrustworthy) and that the sufferer
is extremely incompetent (e.g., that others would have
been able to prevent the trauma somehow, and that
the suffererÕs PTSD symptoms are a clear sign of weakness). Foa et al. (1999a,b) explored self-blame and negative thoughts about the self and the world among
three groups of subjects with PTSD, trauma exposure
without PTSD, or no trauma exposure. The group with
chronic PTSD had significantly higher levels of these
negative cognitions than the other two groups. While
this study was not longitudinal and therefore could
not address causality, the similarity between the nontraumatized subjects and those who had been traumatized but did not develop PTSD suggests that the negative cognitions may be more important than the
experience of trauma per se in driving the development
of PTSD.
Although most therapists working with traumatized
individuals use psychodynamic or supportive counseling approaches—for which there are no efficacy
data—most studies of PTSD treatment outcomes have
explored cognitive-behavioral therapies, which fall into
three general subtypes. In exposure therapies such as
systematic desensitization and flooding, patients confront their fears, object, situation, memories, and
images without being as overwhelmed as they had
anticipated. These experiences of exposure thus serve
to disconfirm and correct cognitive distortions harbored by the patient. The anxiety management component includes a variety of techniques such as relaxation,
controlled breathing, and self-distraction (thought stopping); patients carry out exercises designed to improve
their anxiety management skills. Finally, cognitive therapy identifies and challenges dysfunctional and erroneous cognitions, aiming to replace them with more
functional and realistic cognitions. This procedure can
be said to contain elements of exposure in the sense
that the patient is exposed to the traumatic memories
and thoughts and must process them differently in order to recover.
The clinical practice guidelines developed by the
International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies
(Foa et al., 2000) suggest that exposure therapy is the
most empirically supported for PTSD, but cognitive
therapy (e.g., Resick et al., 2002) and interpersonal psychotherapy (Bleiberg and Markowitz, 2005) have also
been shown to be effective for this disorder. Keane
et al. (1989) conducted the first study of implosion therapy (flooding) combined with relaxation in Vietnam
veterans, a difficult population, and obtained a modest
improvement in PTSD symptoms compared to waitlisted control subjects. Foa and Rothbaum (1998) developed a prolonged exposure (PE) therapy program
consisting primarily of prolonged, repeated exposure
to the traumatic memory and repeated in vivo exposure
to situations that are being avoided because of traumarelated fear; the treatment also contains elements of
breathing retraining and psychoeducation about common reactions to trauma. (For a comparison between
the efficacy of imaginal and in vivo exposure, see Bryant
et al., 2003.)
Stress inoculation training (SIT) (Meichenbaum,
1974) contains elements of relaxation training, thought
stopping, self-guided dialogue, cognitive restructuring
and covert modeling, and role play. Foa et al. (1991)
compared SIT, PE, supportive counseling and waiting
list conditions in 45 rape victims with PSTD, and
found that while PTSD symptoms improved in all
groups, SIT produced significantly more improvement
immediately after treatment (55% symptom reduction,
vs 40% for PE, 26% for supportive counseling, and
20% for waiting list controls). However, PE was superior at 3 months post-treatment—PTSD symptom
reduction at this point was 60% for this modality compared with 49% for SIT and 36% for supportive
counseling.
In a second study, Foa et al. (1999a,b) compared the
efficacy of PE, SIT, both treatments combined, and
waiting list among 96 female assault victims with
chronic PTSD. These patients, whose post-assault
intervals averaged 5 years, had a high prevalence of
comorbid depression, but any medication regimens
were to remain unchanged throughout the course of
the study. Treatment consisted of nine sessions over 5
weeks. All three active treatments significantly reduced
both PTSD and depressive symptoms to a similar extent; contrary to initial expectations, combined therapy
did not improve outcomes over either single modality.
In fact, exposure therapy alone yielded an effect size of
1.92, larger than SIT (1.61) or the combination (1.50).
The gains were maintained through a 12-month followup period.
Using a slightly different design in which nine sessions
of exposure therapy were extended over 9 weeks, optionally augmented by three additional sessions for those
patients with a partial treatment response (<70% improvement), Foa et al. (in press) obtained similar results:
adding cognitive restructuring to exposure therapy did
not produce a significant benefit over that conferred
by exposure therapy alone. One possible reason for this
result is that exposure therapy alone is successful in
modifying negative cognitions.
Similar results in favor of exposure therapy were
found by Marks et al. (1998) among 87 male and female subjects who had suffered a variety of traumas.
These authors compared the efficacy of 10 sessions of
prolonged imaginal and live exposure, cognitive
restructuring alone, the combination of the two, or
relaxation therapy without prolonged exposure or cognitive restructuring. Both PE and cognitive restructur-
C.B. Nemeroff et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research 40 (2006) 1–21
ing were effective, but the combination did not offer increased benefits. In fact, PE produced over 50%
improvement in end-state functioning, more than any
of the other conditions including combination treatment (Fig. 16).
Eye movement desensitization and reprocessing
(EMDR) (Shapiro, 1995) is a therapy which involves
accessing traumatic images and memories, evaluating
their aversive qualities, and generating and focusing on
alternative cognitive appraisals of these images and
memories while performing sets of saccadic eye movements. Comparing EMDR with exposure therapy in female rape victims with PTSD, Rothbaum and Astin
(2002) found that the latter produced a numerically
but not statistically higher treatment response rate at 6
months follow-up (defined as a CAPS score reduction
of P50%, a Beck Depression Inventory score < 10,
and a STAI-S score of <40). A meta-analysis (Davidson
and Parker, 2001) encompassing 34 studies of EMDR in
a variety of populations, including 20 studies of PTSD
or treatment of a trauma memory, concluded that while
EMDR is superior to no treatment and to non-exposure
therapies, it is no more effective than other exposure
therapies, and that the eye movements appear to be
unimportant in its overall efficacy.
Does combining medication treatment with exposure
therapy enhance treatment outcomes? In a study by
Rothabuam et al. (unpubished observation), 64 patients
who had been treated with sertraline for 10 weeks were
randomized to continue sertraline alone or to receive, in
addition, PE for 10 sessions over 5 weeks. In the entire
sample, the effect size for the patients who received sertraline treatment alone was 1.7, while that for the combination was a striking 2.8. There was evidence of a floor
effect; adding exposure therapy greatly improved treatment outcomes for patients with partial responses to
Fig. 16. Good end-state functioning post-treatment*. *>50%
improved on PTSD; <7 BDI; <35 STAI-S. BDI, Beck Depression
Inventory; STAI-S, State–Trait Anxiety Inventory–State subscale; PE,
prolonged exposure; SIT, stress inoculation training; PE/SIT, combined prolonged exposure and stress inoculation training WL, wait-list
control; CR, cognitive restructuring; PE/CR, combined prolonged
exposure and cognitive restructuring; R, relaxation (Foa EB et al. J
Consult Clin Psychol 1999;67:194–200. Marks I et al. Arch Gen
Psychiatry 1998;55:317–25.)
17
medication, but not for those with excellent responses.
However, in a subsample of 45 patients, those who
had received PE did not relapse on discontinuation of
medication, whereas 30% of those who had not had
PE relapsed (Cahill et al., 2003).
The techniques of PE readily lend themselves to
dissemination in order to serve a larger population.
In a recent study by Foa et al. (in press), Masters-degree level therapists with no expertise in CBT were
trained and supervised in PE with and without cognitive restructuring. Training consisted of 5 days for PE
and 5 days for cognitive restructuring and 2 h of
weekly supervision. These counselors went on to successfully treat patients with long-standing PTSD
(mean duration of 9 years), some of whom had been
refractory to other treatments. The initial reluctance
of the trainees to make patients ‘‘suffer’’ from the
exposure techniques was replaced by adoption of the
treatment after experiencing its positive results. Prolonged exposure for PTSD has been selected by the
Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA) as a model program for national
dissemination.
8. Future directions for research
What are the key priorities for future research in the
PTSD field? In the area of neurobiology, future studies
are required to determine brain mechanisms underlying
the successful response to treatment. Studies are also
needed to examine changes in brain receptor and neurotransmitter systems in greater detail in PTSD. In the
areas of phenomenology and etiology, it is important
to assess the validation of the PTSD and acute stress disorder constructs and their distinction from other disorders, as well as to improve our understanding of
genetic and other biological contributions to PTSD susceptibility and the complex interrelationships between
these factors and life experiences. The mechanisms that
underlie success in treatment will continue to be elucidated, with the help of a variety of animal models for
PTSD. Increased knowledge of the complex etiology
of PTSD would not only help develop new therapeutic
targets and treatments with improved short- and longterm efficacy, but also assist in identifying predictors
of treatment response: Which patients require psychotherapy, pharmacotherapy, or a combination of modalities? Which drugs and/or types of psychotherapy are
most suitable for a given individual? How can evidence-based psychotherapies be made available to a
wider population in need? Finally, in analogy with major depressive disorder, PTSD needs an operational definition of remission, since the ultimate goal of treament
is not merely reduction of symptoms, but full remission
from this devastating condition.
18
C.B. Nemeroff et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research 40 (2006) 1–21
9. Disclosure statement
Author Financial Disclosures for this article can be
obtained from the U.S. Journal office.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank GlaxoSmithKline for providing
an unrestricted educational grant in support of the
development of this article, and CTC Communications
Corporation for providing editorial support.
References
American Psychiatric Association, Committee on Nomenclature and
Statistics. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders.
3rd ed. Washington (DC): American Psychiatric Association;
1980.
Andrews B, Brewin CR, Rose S. Gender, social support, and PTSD in
victims of violent crime. J Trauma Stress 2003;16:421–7.
Associated Press. Poll: Americans depressed, sleepless. Associated
Press. 9/19/2001.Washington DC, Associated Press. Available
www.chron.com/cs/CDA/story.hts/special/terror/impact/
from:
1055126.
Baker DG, West SA, Nicholson WE, et al. Serial CSF corticotropinreleasing hormone levels and adrenocortical activity in combat
veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry
1999;156:585–8.
Banki CM, Bissette G, Arato M, et al. CSF corticotropin-releasing
factor-like immunoreactivity in depression and schizophrenia. Am
J Psychiatry 1987;144:873–7.
Barbas H, Saha S, Rempel-Clower N, Ghashghaei T. Seial pathways
from preimate prefrontal cortex to autonomic areas may influence
emotional expression. BMC Neurosci 2003;4:25.
Barr CS, Newman TK, Schwandt M, et al. Sexual dichotomy of an
interaction between early adversity and the serotonin transporter
gene promoter variant in rhesus macaques. Proc Natl Acad Sci
USA 2004;101:12358–63.
Beals J, Manson SM, Shore JH, et al. The prevalence of posttraumatic
stress disorder among American Indian Vietnam veterans: disparities and context. J Trauma Stress 2002;15:89–97.
Bennice JA, Resick PA, Mechanic M, Astin M. The relative effects
of intimate partner physical and sexual violence on posttraumatic stress disorder symptomatology. Violence Vict
2003;18: 87–94.
Bleiberg KL, Markowitz JC. A pilot study of interpersonal psychotherapy for posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry
2005;162:181–3.
Bonne O, Brandes D, Gilboa A, et al. Longitudinal MRI study of
hippocampal volume in trauma survivors with PTSD. Am J
Psychiatry 2001;158:1248–51.
Brady K, Pearlstein T, Asnis GM, et al. Efficacy and safety of
sertraline treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder: a randomized
controlled trial. JAMA 2000;283:1837–44.
Bremner JD. Neuroimaging of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatric Ann 1998;28:445–50.
Bremner JD. Does stress damage the brain? In: Understanding
trauma-related disorders from a mind-body perspective. New
York: W.W. Norton; 2002.
Bremner JD, Licinio J, Darnell A, et al. Elevated CSF corticotropinreleasing factor concentrations in posttraumatic stress disorder.
Am J Psychiatry 1997a;154:624–9.
Bremner JD, Narayan M, Staib LH, et al. Neural correlates of
memories of childhood sexual abuse in women with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1999a;156:
1787–95.
Bremner JD, Randall P, Scott TM, et al. Deficits in short-term
memory in adult survivors of childhood abuse. Psychiatry Res
1995a;59:97–107.
Bremner JD, Randall PR, Vermetten E, et al. MRI-based measurement of hippocampal volume in posttraumatic stress disorder
related to childhood physical and sexual abuse: a preliminary
report. Biol Psychiatry 1997b;41:23–32.
Bremner JD, Scott TM, Delaney RC, et al. Deficits in short-term
memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry
1993a;150:1015–9.
Bremner JD, Randall P, Scott TM, et al. MRI-based measurement of
hippocampal volume in patients with combat-related posttraumatic
stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1995b;152:973–81.
Bremner JD, Southwick SM, Johnson DR, Yehuda R, Charney DS.
Childhood physical abuse and combat-related posttraumatic
stress disorder in Vietnam veterans. Am J Psychiatry 1993b;150:
235–9.
Bremner JD, Staib LH, Kaloupek D, et al. Neural correlates of
exposure to traumatic pictures and sound in Vietnam combat
veterans with and without posttraumatic stress disorder: a positron
emission tomography study. Biol Psychiatry 1999b;45:806–16.
Bremner JD, Vythilingam M, Vermetten E, et al. MRI and PET study
of deficits in hippocampal structure and function in women with
childhood sexual abuse and posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J
Psychiatry 2003;160:924–32.
Breslau N, Davis GC. Posttraumatic stress disorder in an urban
population of young adults: risk factors for chronicity. Am J
Psychiatry 1992;149:671–5.
Breslau N, Davis GC, Andreski P, et al. Sex differences in posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1997;54:1044–8.
Bryant RA, Moulds ML, Guthrie RM, et al. Imaginal exposure alone
and imaginal exposure with cognitive restructuring in treatment of
posttraumatic stress disorder. J Consulting Clin Psychol 2003;71:
706–12.
Cahill SP, Foa EB, Rothbaum BO, Connor K, Smith R, Davidson
JRT. Augmentation of sertraline with prolonged exposure (PE) in
the treatment of PTSD: does PE protect against relapse? In:
Poster presented at the annual convention of the anxiety disorders association of America, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, March
2003.
Caldji C, Francis D, Sharma S, et al. The effects of early rearing
environment on the development of GABAA and central benzodiazepine receptor levels and novelty-induced fearfulness in the rat.
Neuropsychopharmacology 2000;22:219–29.
Card JJ. Epidemiology of PTSD in a national cohort of Vietnam
veterans. J Clin Psychol 1987;43:6–17.
Carpenter LL, Tyrka AR, McDougle CJ, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid
corticotropin-releasing factor and perceived early-life stress in
depressed patients and healthy control subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology 2004;29:777–84.
Carrion VG, Weems CF, Eliez S, et al. Attenuation of frontal
asymmetry in pediatric posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2001;50:943–51.
Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, et al. Influence of life stress on
depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene.
Science 2003;301:386–9.
Chen B, Dowlatshahi D, MacQueen GM, Wang JF, Young LT.
Increased hippocampal BDNF immunoreactivity in subjects
treated with antidepressant medication. Biol Psychiatry
2001;50:260–5.
Coplan JD, Andrews MW, Rosenblum LA, et al. Persistent elevations
of cerebrospinal fluid concentrations of corticotropin-releasing
factor in adult nonhuman primates exposed to early-life stressors:
C.B. Nemeroff et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research 40 (2006) 1–21
implications for the pathophysiology of mood and anxiety disorders. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1996;93:1619–23.
Davidson PR, Parker KC. Eye movement desensitization and reprocessing (EMDR): a meta-analysis. J Consult Clin Psychol
2001;69:305–16.
De Bellis MD, Hall J, Boring AM, Frustaci K, Moritz G. A pilot
longitudinal study of hippocampal volumes in pediatric maltreatment-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry
2001;50:305–9.
De Bellis MD, Keshavan MS, Clark DB, et al. A.E. Bennett Research
Award: Developmental traumatology: Part II. Brain development.
Biol Psychiatry 1999;45:1271–84.
DeLisi LE, Maurizio A, Yost M, et al. A survey of New Yorkers after
the Sept. 11, 2001, terrorist attacks. Am J Psychiatry 2003;160:
780–3.
Dobie DJ, Kivlahan DR, Maynard C, Bush KR, Davis TM, Bradley
KA. Posttraumatic stress disorder in female veterans: association
with self-reported health problems and functional impairment.
Arch Intern Med 2004;164:394–400.
Dube SR, Anda RF, Felitti VJ, et al. Childhood abuse, household
dysfunction, and the risk of attempted suicide throughout the life
span: findings from the Adverse Childhood Experiences Study.
JAMA 2001;286:3089–96.
Duman RS, Malberg J, Thome J. Neural plasticity to stress and
antidepressant treatment. Biol Psychiatry 1999;19:1181–91.
Duman RS, Nakagawa S, Malberg J. Regulation of adult neurogenesis
by antidepressant treatment. Neuropsychopharmacology 2001;25:
836–44.
Edwards VJ, Holden GW, Felitti VJ, Anda RF. Relationship between
multiple forms of childhood maltreatment and adult mental health
in community respondents: results from the adverse childhood
experiences study. Am J Psychiatry 2003;160:1453–60.
Elzinga BM, Schmahl CG, Vermetten E, van Dyck R, Bremner JD.
Higher cortisol levels following exposure to traumatic reminders
in abuse-related PTSD. Neuropsychopharmacology 2003;28:
1656–65.
Engel CC, Engel AL, Campbell SJ, McFall ME, Russo J, Katon W.
Posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms and precombat sexual and
physical abuse in Desert Storm veterans. J Nerv Ment Dis
1993;181:683–8.
Flach FF. Psychobiologic resilience, psychotherapy, and the creative
process. Compr Psychiatry 1980;21:510–8.
Foa EB, Hembree EA, Cahill SP, Rauch SA, Riggs DS, Feeny NC,
et al. Randomized trail of prolonged exposure for PTSD with and
without cognitive restructuring: Outcome at academic and community clinics. J Consult Clin Psychol (in press).
Foa EB, Jaycox LH. Cognitive-behavioral theory and treatment of
post-traumatic stress disorder. In: Spiegel DS, editor. Efficacy and
cost-effectiveness of psychotherapy. Washington (DC): American
Psychiatric Press; 1999. p. 23–61.
Foa EB, Rothbaum BO. Treating the trauma of rape. New York
(NY): Guilford Press; 1998.
Foa EB, Dancu CV, Hembree EA, Jaycox LH, Meadows EA, Street
GP. A comparison of exposure therapy, stress inoculation
training, and their combination for reducing posttraumatic stress
disorder in female assault victims. J Consult Clin Psychol
1999a;67:194–200.
Foa EB, Ehlers A, Clark D, Tolin DF, Orsillo SM. Posttraumatic
Cognitions Inventory (PTCI): development and validation. Psychol
Assess 1999b;11:303–14.
Foa EB, Keane TM, Friedman MJ, editors. Effective treatments for
PTSD: practice guidelines from the International Society for
Traumatic Stress Studies. New York: Guilford Press; 2000.
Foa EB, Rothbaum BO, Riggs DS, Murdock TB. Treatment of
posttraumatic stress disorder in rape victims: a comparison
between cognitive-behavioral procedures and counseling. J Consult
Clin Psychol 1991;59:715–23.
19
Frans O, Rimmo PA, Aberg L, Fredrikson M. Trauma exposure and
post-traumatic stress disorder in the general population. Acta
Psychiatr Scand 2005;111:291–9.
Freedman LJ, Insel TR, Smith Y. Subcortical projections of area 25
(subgenual cortex) of the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol
2000;421:172–88.
Freeman TW, Cardwell D, Karson CN, Komoroski RA. In vivo
proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy of the medial temporal
lobes of subjects with combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder.
Magn Reson Med 1998;40:66–71.
Galea S, Ahern J, Resnick H, et al. Psychological sequelae of the
September 11 terrorist attacks in New York City. New Engl J Med
2002;346:982–7.
Gilbertson MW, Shenton ME, Ciszewski A, et al. Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological
trauma. Nature Neurosci 2002;5:1242–7.
Goldapple K, Segal Z, Garson C, et al. Modulation of cortical-limbic
pathways in major depression: treatment specific effects of cognitive
behavioral therapy. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004a;61:34–41.
Goldapple K, Segal Z, Garson C, et al. Modulation of corticallimbic pathways in major depression: treatment-specific effects of
cognitive behavior therapy. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004b;61:
34–41.
Gould E, Tanapat P, McEwen BS, Flugge G, Fuchs E. Proliferation of
granule cell precursors in the dentate gyrus of adult monkeys is
diminished by stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1998;95:3168–71.
Gurvits TG, Shenton MR, Hokama H, et al. Magnetic resonance
imaging study of hippocampal volume in chronic combat-related
posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 1996;40:192–9.
Heim C, Newport DJ, Heit S, et al. Pituitary-adrenal and autonomic
responses to stress in women after sexual and physical abuse in
childhood. JAMA 2000;284:592–7.
Heim C, Plotsky PM, Nemeroff CB. Importance of studying the
contributions of early adverse experience to neurobiological
findings in depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 2004;29:641–8.
Holbrook TL, Hoyt DB, Stein MB, Sieber WJ. Gender differences in
long-term posttraumatic stress disorder outcomes after major
trauma: women are at higher risk of adverse outcomes than men. J
Trauma 2002;53:882–8.
Huot RL, Plotsky PM, Lenox RH, McNamara RK. Neonatal
maternal separation reduces hippocampal mossy fiber density in
adult Long Evans rats. Brain Res 2002;950:52–63.
Huot RL, Thrivikraman KV, Meaney MJ, Plotsky PM. Development
of adult ethanol preference and anxiety as a consequence of
neonatal maternal separation in Long Evans rats and reversal with
antidepressant treatment. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001;158:
366–73.
Kang H, Dalager N, Mahan C, Ishii E. The role of sexual assault on
the risk of PTSD among Gulf War veterans. Ann Epidemiol
2005;15:191–5.
Keane TM, Fairbank JA, Caddell JM, et al. Implosive (flooding)
therapy reduces symptoms of PTSD in Vietnam combat veterans.
Behav Ther 1989;20:245–60.
Keightley ML, Seminowicz DA, Bagby RM, et al. Personality
influences limbic-cortical interactions during sad mood induction.
Neuroimage 2003;20:2031–9.
Keller MB, McCullough JP, Klein DN, et al. A comparison of
nefazodone, the cognitive-behavioral-analysis system of psychotherapy, and their combination for the treatment of chronic
depression. N Engl J Med 2000;342:1462–70.
Kendler KS, Kuhn J, Prescott CA. The interrelationship of neuroticism, sex, and stressful life events in the prediction of episodes of
major depression. Am J Psychiatry 2004;161:631–6.
Kennedy SH, Evans KR, Kruger S, et al. Changes in regional brain
glucose metabolism measured with positron emission tomography
after paroxetine treatment of major depression. Am J Psychiatry
2001;158:899–905.
20
C.B. Nemeroff et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research 40 (2006) 1–21
Kessler RC. Posttraumatic stress disorder: the burden to the individual
and to society. J Clin Psychiatry 2000;61(Suppl 5):4–12, discussion:
13–4.
Kessler RC, Sonnega A, Bromet E, et al. Posttraumatic stress disorder
in the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry
1995;52:1048–60.
Kruger S, Seminowicz D, Goldapple K, et al. State and trait influences
on mood regulation in bipolar disorder: blood flow differences with
an acute mood challenge. Biol Psychiatry 2003;54:1274–83.
Ladd CO, Owens MJ, Nemeroff CB. Persistent changes in corticotropin-releasing factor neuronal systems induced by maternal deprivation. Endocrinology 1996;137:1212–8.
Liotti M, Mayberg HS, Brannan SK, McGinnis S, Jerabek P, Fox PT.
Differential limbic-cortical correlates of sadness and anxiety in
healthy subjects: implications for affective disorder. Biol Psychiatry
2000;48:30–42.
Liotti M, Mayberg HS, McGinnis S, Brannan S, Jerabek P. Mood
challenge in remitted unipolar depression unmasks disease-specific
cerebral
blood
flow
abnormalities.
Am
Psychiatry
2002a;159:1830–40.
Liotti M, Mayberg HS, McGinnis S, et al. Unmasking disease-specific
cerebral blood flow abnormalities: mood challenge in patients with
remitted unipolar depression. Am J Psychiatry 2002b;159:1830–40.
Long N, MacDonald C, Chamberlain K. Prevalence of posttraumatic
stress disorder, depression and anxiety in a community sample of
New Zealand Vietnam War veterans. Aust N Z J Psychiatry
1996;30:253–6.
Marks I, Lovell K, Noshirvani H, Livanou M, Thrasher S. Treatment
of posttraumatic stress disorder by exposure and/or cognitive
restructuring: a controlled study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998;55:
317–25.
Marshall RD, Beebe KL, Oldham M, Zaninelli R. Efficacy and safety
of paroxetine treatment for chronic PTSD: a fixed-dose, placebocontrolled study. Am J Psychiatry 2001;158:1982–8.
Marshall RD, Schneider FR, Fallon BA, et al. An open trial of
paroxetine in patients with noncombat-related, chronic posttraumatic stress disorder. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1998;18:10–8.
Mason JW, Giller EL, Kosten TR, Ostroff RB, Podd L. Urinary freecortisol levels in posttraumatic stress disorder patients. J Nerv
Ment Dis 1986;174:145–9.
Mayberg HS. Modulating dysfunctional limbic-cortical circuits in
depression: towards development of brain-based algorithms for
diagnosis and optimised treatment. Br Med Bull 2003;65:193–207.
Mayberg HS, Brannan SK, Mahurin RK, McGinnin S. Regional
metabolic effects of fluoxetine in major depression: serial changes
and relationship to clinical response. Biol Psychiatry 2000a;48:
830–43.
Mayberg HS, Brannan SK, Tekell JL, et al. Regional metabolic effects
of fluoxetine in major depression: serial changes and relationship to
clinical response. Biol Psychiatry 2000b;48:830–43.
Mayberg HS, Liotti M, Brannan SK, et al. Reciprocal limbiccortical function and negative mood: converging PET findings in
depression and normal sadness. Am J Psychiatry 1999;156:675–82.
Mayberg HS, Silva JA, Brannan SK, et al. The functional neuroanatomy of the placebo effect. Am J Psychiatry 2002;159:728–37.
McCauley J, Kern DE, Kolodner K, et al. Clinical characteristics of
women with a history of childhood abuse: unhealed wounds.
JAMA 1997;277:1362–8.
McCullough JP. Cognitive-behavioral analysis system of psychotherapy: an interactional treatment approach for dysthymic disorder.
Psychiatry 1984;47:234–50.
McHolm AE, MacMillan HL, Jamieson E. The relationship between
childhood physical abuse and suicidality among depressed women:
results from a community sample. Am J Psychiatry 2003;160:
933–8.
McMillen JC. Better for it: how people benefit from adversity? Soc
Work 1999;44:455–68.
Meichenbaum D. Cognitive behavior modification. Morristown
(NJ): General Learning Press; 1974.
Merriam-WebsterÕs Collegiate Dictionary, 10th ed. Springfield (MA):
Merriam-Webster; 1993.
Nemeroff CB. Clinical significance of psychoneuroendocrinology in
psychiatry: focus on the thyroid and adrenal. J Clin Psychiatry
1989;50(Suppl):13–20, discussion: 21–2.
Nemeroff CB, Heim CM, Thase ME, Klein DN, Rush J, Schatzberg
AF, et al. Differential responses to psychotherapy versus pharmacotherapy in patients with chronic forms of major depression
and childhood trauma. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2003;100:
14293–6.
Neumaier JF, Edwards E, Plotsky PM. 5-HT(1B) mrna regulation in
two animal models of altered stress reactivity. Biol Psychiatry
2002;51:902–8.
Norris FH, Friedman MJ, Watson PJ, Byrne CM, Diaz E, Kaniasty
K. 60,000 disaster victims speak. Part I. An empirical review of the
empirical literature, 1981–2001. Psychiatry 2002;65:207–39.
North CS, Nixon SJ, Shariat S, et al. Psychiatric disorders among
survivors of the Oklahoma City bombing. JAMA 1999;282:755–62.
North CS, Spitznagel EL, Smith EM. A prospective study of coping
after exposure to a mass murder episode. Ann Clin Psychiatry
2001;13:81–7.
Notestine CF, Stein MB, Kennedy CM, Archibald SL, Jernigan TL.
Brain morphometry in female victims of intimate partner violence
with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry
2002;51:1089–101.
Ongur D, An X, Price JL. Prefrontal cortical projections to the
hypothalamus in macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol 1998;401:
480–505.
Oquendo MA, Friend JM, Halberstam B, et al. Association of
comorbid posttraumatic stress disorder and major depression with
greater risk for suicidal behavior. Am J Psychiatry 2003;160:580–2.
Ouimette P, Cronkite R, Henson BR, Prins A, Gima K, Moos RH.
Posttraumatic stress disorder and health status among female and
male medical patients. J Traumatic Stress 2004;17:1–9.
Perkonigg A, Kessler RC, Storz S, Wittchen H-U. Traumatic events
and post-traumatic stress disorder in the community: prevalence,
risk factors and comorbidity. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2000;101:
46–59.
Pihoker C, Owens MJ, Kuhn CM, Schanberg SM, Nemeroff CB.
Maternal separation in neonatal rats elicits activation of the
hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis: a putative role for
corticotropin-releasing factor. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1993;18:
485–93.
Pitman RK. Investigating the pathogenesis of posttraumatic stress
disorder with neuroimaging. J Clin Psychiatry 2001;62:47–54.
Plichta SB, Falik M. Prevalence of violence and its implications for
womenÕs health. Womens Health Issues 2001;11:244–58.
Prigerson HG, Maciejewski PK, Rosenheck RA. Population attributable fractions of psychiatric disorders and behavioral outcomes
associated with combat exposure among US men. Am J Public
Health 2002;92:59–63.
Raadsheer FC, Hoogendijk WJ, Stam FC, et al. Increased numbers of
corticotropin-releasing hormone expressing neurons in the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus of depressed patients. Neuroendocrinology 1994;60:436–44.
Resick PA, Nishith P, Weaver TL, Astin MC, Feuer CA. A
comparison of cognitive-processing therapy with prolonged exposure and a waiting condition for the treatment of chronic
posttraumatic stress disorder in female rape victims. J Consulting
Clin Psychol 2002;70:867–79.
Rothbaum BO, Astin MC. Prolonged exposure versus EMDR for
PTSD rape victims. In: Zoellner LA (Chair), Recent innovations in
post traumatic stress disorder treatment. Paper presented at the
22nd national conference of the anxiety disorders association of
America, Austin, TX; March 2002.
C.B. Nemeroff et al. / Journal of Psychiatric Research 40 (2006) 1–21
Safren SA, Gershuny BS, Marzol P, et al. History of childhood abuse
in panic disorder, social phobia, and generalized anxiety disorder. J
Nerv Ment Dis 2002;190:453–6.
Santarelli L, Saxe M, Gross C, et al. Requirement of hippocampal
neurogenesis for the behavioral effects of antidepressants. Science
2003;301:805–9.
Schlenger WE, Caddell JM, Ebert L, et al. Psychological reactions to
terrorist attacks: findings from the National Study of AmericansÕ
Reactions to September 11. JAMA 2002;288:581–8.
Schuff N, Neylan TC, Lenoci MA, et al. Decreased hippocampal Nacetylaspartate in the absence of atrophy in posttraumatic stress
disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2001;50:952–9.
Schuster MA, Stein BD, Jaycox L, et al. A national survey of stress
reactions after the September 11, 2001, terrorist attacks. New Engl
J Med 2001;345:1507–12.
Seminowicz DA, Mayberg HS, McIntosh AR, et al. Limbic-frontal
circuitry in major depression: a path modeling metanalysis.
Neuroimage 2004;22:409–18.
Shapiro F. Eye movement desensitization and reprocessing: basic
principles, protocols, and procedures. New York (NY): Guilford
Press; 1995.
Shin LM, Shin PS, Heckers S, et al. Hippocampal function in
posttraumatic stress disorder. Hippocampus 2004;14:292–300.
Silver RC, Holman EA, McIntosh DN, et al. Nationwide longitudinal
study of psychological responses to September 11. JAMA 2002;288:
1235–44.
Stefurak T, Mayberg H. Cortico-limbic-striatal dysfunction in depression: converging findings in basal ganglia disease & primary
affective disorders. In: Bedard MA, Agid Y, Chouinard, et al.,
editors. Mental & behavioral dysfunction in movement disorders. Clifton, UK: Humanae Press; 2003. p. 321–38.
Stein MB. A 46-year-old man with anxiety and nightmares after a
motor vehicle collision. JAMA 2002;288:1513–22.
Stein MB, Kennedy C. Major depressive and post-traumatic stress
disorder comorbidity in female victims of intimate partner violence.
J Affect Disord 2001;66:133–8.
Stein MB, Walker JR, Forde DR. Gender differences in susceptibility to
posttraumatic stress disorder. Behav Res Ther 2000;38:619–28.
Stein MB, Koverola C, Hanna C, Torchia MG, McClarty B.
Hippocampal volume in women victimized by childhood sexual
abuse. Psychol Med 1997a;27:951–9.
21
Stein MB, Yehuda R, Koverola C, Hanna C. Enhanced dexamethasone suppression of plasma cortisol in adult women
traumatized by childhood sexual abuse. Biol Psychiatry
1997b;42: 680–6.
Tucker P, Zaninelli R, Yehuda R, Ruggiero L, Dillingham K, Pitts
CD. Paroxetine in the treatment of chronic posttraumatic stress
disorder: results of a placebo-controlled, flexible-dosage trial. J
Clin Psychiatry 2001;62:860–8.
Unpublished observations.
Vaidya VA, Marek GJ, Aghajanian GK, Duman RS. 5-HT2A
receptor-mediated regulation of brain-derived neurotrophic factor
mRNA in the hippocampus and the neocortex. J Neurosci
1997;17:2785–95.
Vaisrub S. Editorial: Da Costa syndrome revisited. JAMA 1975;232:
164.
Van Loey NE, Maas CJ, Faber AW, Taal LA. Predictors of chronic
posttraumatic stress symptoms following burn injury: results of a
longitudinal study. J Trauma Stress 2003;16:361–9.
Vermetten E, Vythilingam M, Southwick SM, Charney DS, Bremner
JD. Long-term treatment with paroxetine increases verbal declarative memory and hippocampal volume in posttraumatic stress
disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2003;54:693–702.
Villareale G, Hamilton DA, Petropoulos H, et al. Reduced hippocampal volume and total white matter in posttraumatic stress
disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2002;15:119–25.
Walker EA, Katon W, Russo J, et al. Health care costs associated
with posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in women. Arch Gen
Psychiatry 2003;60:369–74.
Wolin SJ. The resilient self: how survivors of troubled families rise
above adversity? New York: Villard Books; 1993.
Woolley CS, Gould E, McEwen BS. Exposure to excess glucocorticoids alters dendritic morphology of adult hippocampal pyramidal
neurons. Brain Res 1990;531:225–31.
Yehuda R, Kahana B, Binder-Brynes K, Southwick S, Mason J,
Giller E. Low urinary cortisol excretion in Holocaust survivors
with posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1995;152:
982–90.
Yehuda R, Southwick SM, Krystal JH, Bremner D, Charney DS,
Mason JW. Enhanced suppression of cortisol following dexamethasone administration in posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J
Psychiatry 1993;150:83–6.