Nothing Special   »   [go: up one dir, main page]

Academia.eduAcademia.edu

Fine motor skills and motor control networking in developmental age

American Journal of Human Biology

ObjectivesWe investigated the relationships between fine motor skills, fitness, anthropometrics, gender and perceived motor performance in school beginners. The aim of our study was to delineate whether and to what extent fine motor control would show meaningful synchrony with other motor variables in the age of onset of handwriting in school.MethodsA sample of N = 239 of 6‐to‐8‐year‐old children were tested with an array of tasks measuring fine motor (i.e., dexterity and speed) and grapho‐motor performance (tracing on a tablet screen), anthropometric indexes, and fitness (shuttle run) measures. A subset of 95 children was also tested for perceived motor competence.ResultsIn spite of an overall poor anthropometric condition, our participants were relatively fit. As expected, older children performed better in both, fine motor tasks and the shuttle test. The girls were better in fine motor skills, and an original speed‐quality trade‐off in the drawing was found. However, the magnitud...

Received: 13 October 2021 Revised: 30 April 2022 Accepted: 4 May 2022 DOI: 10.1002/ajhb.23758 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Fine motor skills and motor control networking in developmental age Danilo Bondi1 | Claudio Robazza2 Tiziana Pietrangelo | Christiane Lange-Küttner3,4 | 1 1 Department of Neuroscience, Imaging and Clinical Sciences, University “G. d'Annunzio” of Chieti-Pescara, Chieti, Italy 2 Department of Medicine and Aging Sciences, University “G. d'Annunzio” of Chieti-Pescara, Chieti, Italy 3 Department of Psychology, University of Greifswald, Germany 4 Department of Psychology, University of Bremen, Bremen, Land Bremen, Germany Correspondence Danilo Bondi, Department of Neuroscience, Imaging and Clinical Sciences, University “G. d'Annunzio” of Chieti-Pescara, Via dei Vestini, 31, Chieti, 66100, Italy. Email: danilo.bondi@unich.it Funding information Italian Ministry of Education, University and Research - Departments of Excellence 2018-2022; University “G. d'Annunzio” of Chieti - Pescara grant Abstract Objectives: We investigated the relationships between fine motor skills, fitness, anthropometrics, gender and perceived motor performance in school beginners. The aim of our study was to delineate whether and to what extent fine motor control would show meaningful synchrony with other motor variables in the age of onset of handwriting in school. Methods: A sample of N = 239 of 6-to-8-year-old children were tested with an array of tasks measuring fine motor (i.e., dexterity and speed) and graphomotor performance (tracing on a tablet screen), anthropometric indexes, and fitness (shuttle run) measures. A subset of 95 children was also tested for perceived motor competence. Results: In spite of an overall poor anthropometric condition, our participants were relatively fit. As expected, older children performed better in both, fine motor tasks and the shuttle test. The girls were better in fine motor skills, and an original speed-quality trade-off in the drawing was found. However, the magnitude of difference by grade was greater for boys' fine motor skills than those of girls'. A network analysis revealed three specific clusters, (1) perceived competencies, (2) fitness, and (3) fine motor skills. Conclusions: Given the relative independence of these areas of physical performance, we suggest focusing on these three clusters as distinct areas of physical education. Fine motor skills deserve further consideration, especially at an early school age. We have demonstrated that network analysis and technology devices used to evaluate motor development are useful and meaningful tools. 1 | INTRODUCTION We have considerable knowledge about how gross motor skills develop in children (Ghassabian et al., 2016; Smith & Thelen, 2003) and how to assess them (Griffiths et al., 2018). Thus, such results in gross motor skills become included in educational settings. In contrast, although some effort has been devoted to research on fine motor control development and interventions in babies and pre-school children (Strooband et al., 2020), less is known about fine motor skills in school age children. The aim of our study was to delineate whether and This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs License, which permits use and distribution in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, the use is non-commercial and no modifications or adaptations are made. © 2022 The Authors. American Journal of Human Biology published by Wiley Periodicals LLC. Am J Hum Biol. 2022;34:e23758. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.23758 wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/ajhb 1 of 15 2 of 15 to what extent fine motor control would show meaningful synchrony with other motor variables because of the onset of handwriting in school. The astonishing use of hands is a fundamental achievement of humankind: a wide-ranging sort of handbased interaction with the environment led to a vital loop increasing our manual skills. In particular, the morphology and function of the thumb in manipulation and complex grasping is considered a cornerstone in functional evolutionary development (Marzke, 1992). Phenotypical features in postural and functional kinematics lead to the genesis of grapho-motor gestures, and the pattern of those gestures may reflect the developmental trajectories, allowing to assess grapho-motor levels (Vaivre-Douret et al., 2021). Increasing complexity and meaning in the use of tools was instrumental in the evolution of human intelligence (Lockman & Kahrs, 2017). Gender differences exist in performing grapho-motor tasks during developmental ages, with girls having an early advantage in performing fine motor tasks (Reikerås et al., 2017) and learning novel tasks (Flatters et al., 2014), while boys outperform girls in tasks requiring object control (such as throwing, Escolano-Pérez et al., 2021). The reasons lie both in biological factors—such as the differential involvement of visuo-motor integration (Giammarco et al., 2016)—and environmental factors, such as genderdifferentiated expectations made by parents about suitable and proper motor tasks (Dinkel & Snyder, 2020). Developing object manipulation skills in childhood promotes future physical activity (Barnett et al., 2009). Especially the advanced use of graphic tools has been playing a fundamental role in society: the complex use of such devices in drawing and handwriting was conducive to nearly every culture in this world (Milbrath & Council, 2003). This argument has been extended to digital technologies as they are integrated into school settings and online learning from home as innovative (and socially distanced) learning tools during the COVID pandemic (see also Scaradozzi et al., 2021). The current study investigates how motor skills are related in children who have just begun school. This crucial transition point in child development when they are learning to write changes drawing from basic shapes and minimal detail to visually realistic drawing that captures modulated figure contours (Lange-Küttner et al., 2002; Toomela, 2002). 1.1 | Motor networks in development From a kinesiological point of view, we can consider grapho-motor skills as special tool-related fine motor skills. Drawing, tracing, and writing result from an BONDI ET AL. integrated input of visual and motor modalities (eye-hand coordination) during precise, small-scale movements. Defining fine motor skills, beyond grapho-motricity, other clusters involved are dexterity, manipulation of small objects, and speed-dominated skills in simple and very likely repetitive movements (Martzog et al., 2019). Fine motor skills in school children consist of an interplay of spatio-temporal processing, visuo-motor integration, strength control, perceptual, and cognitive skills (Feder & Majnemer, 2007; Lange-Küttner, 1998; Tabatabaey-Mashadi et al., 2015). These related skills are a great example of networked neuromotor development. Different grapho-motor tasks such as writing and drawing lie on both common and distinct underlying neural circuits in the motor and posterior parietal. Drawing activates a ventral pathway responsible for overseeing and orienting in spatial fields, while writing activates a dorsal pathway that controls letter features (Raimo et al., 2021; Yuan & Brown, 2015). Moreover, delays and disturbances in the fine motor network are present in children with special needs such as ADHD and autism (LangeKüttner & Kochhar, 2020) and are greatly correlated with low IQ, math and reading (Mayes et al., 2018; see also Suggate et al., 2018). Gaul and Issartel (2016) claimed that children's fine motor skill proficiency was not progressing at the expected rate given by normative data, over early school ages. Thus, researchers have been using digital tablets and pen to acquire and evaluate the parameters pressure, velocity, and time in children's drawing and writing (Lange-Küttner, 1998; Tabatabaey-Mashadi et al., 2015). There is an emerging field of Artificial Intelligence in Education (AIED) for motor skills learning (Santos, 2016) that needs a clear characterization of the motor tasks in school children to set interactive technologies and foster advanced learning experiences. The multidisciplinary field of “graphonomics” needs to be linked within a broad spectrum of other motor skills and behavior (Van Gemmert & Contreras-Vidal, 2015). Recently, it has been suggested that grapho-motor performances can provide a paradigm to investigate motor skills that may deviate from the expected developmental trajectories (Bondi, Di Sano, Verratti, et al., 2020). While grapho-motor tasks, as other fine motor tasks, share common underlying pathways with gross-motor tasks rely on conscious and intentional motor strategy and control development, they develop differently in the way of how these shared roots emerge. Various factors such as the set of task constraints, goal-oriented decisions, motor synergies, optimal feedback, previous learning experience, and the adaptation processes influence general motor control (Todorov & Jordan, 2002), feeding into an aggregate performance in motor coordination tests. Scordella BONDI ET AL. et al. (2015) showed a functional common basis of grossmotor and grapho-motor skills based on visual–spatial processes. Such performances may be also differently influenced by the fitness and anthropometric status as relevance can be assumed in all coordinative motor skills (Augustijn et al., 2018). Overweight children perform poorly on motor tests and report greater body dissatisfaction and lower self-efficacy than their nonoverweight peers (Colella et al., 2009). Matarma et al. (2018) cogently demonstrated that, in young children, gross-motor coordination was related to anthropometric status, while fine motor coordination was not influenced by body weight. However, D'Hondt et al. (2008) reported childhood obesity to have a negative impact on fine motor performances, both with or without a simultaneous mechanical postural demand, suggesting a likely suffering from underlying perceptual-motor coordination difficulties in obese preschool children. Among several determinant factors, Zaqout et al. (2016) highlighted BMI, age and sex as the strongest determinants of children's physical fitness, independent of physical activity. Boys often overestimate and girls underestimate their own competence regarding sports practice; this mismatch between objective competence and perceived competence may negatively influence health outcomes and motor skills in particular (Pesce et al., 2018). The perceptual judgment of sports rules is related to actual play experience, and gender difference influence the amount of sports experience (LangeKüttner & Bosco, 2016). 1.2 | The current study Because of the onset of handwriting, fine motor skills gained at preschool age are expected to change during the first scholastic years, ensuring a new proactive control of manual tasks (Meulenbroek & Van Galen, 1988). Thus, we explored the grapho-motor performance of early school-age children. Given that the expression of fine motor skills and their development occur in different contexts and are thus task-dependent, our analysis included fine and gross motor tests as well as ancillary variables to assess the different underlying motor pathways. We analyzed the ability clusters of school children's graphonomics performance in relation to fitness and anthropometric status, as well as perceived competence. We also considered gender and age in early school-age children from 6 to 8 years of age. We hypothesized that the different domains of fine motor skills, including those related to graphonomics, were differently related to each other across age and 3 of 15 gender, as the extent and time course of fine motor skills may have different development for boys and girls. We hypothesized a link between physical characteristics and performance with fine motor skills. 2 | METHODS 2.1 | Participants The original sample consisted of 239 children of the first (all children of this grade were 6-to-7 years old) and second (all children of this grade were 7-to-8 years old) grade of First-Level School, living in the Abruzzo region, Italy. No child was years ahead or behind. All children had a similar preschool experience at local public schools. All schools were following similar curricula, that is, teaching of handwriting during the first and second grades. A total of 13 school classes were involved, according to school availability. All children were enrolled in the classrooms with their age peers chronologically matched. All children were invited to take part, with neither inclusion nor exclusion criteria. The number of boys and girls did not differ significantly, χ 2(1) = 0.235, p = .628 (Table 1). Participants' median weight and height were 25.9 kg and 123 cm, respectively (24.9 kg and 121 cm in first grade, 26.9 kg and 126 cm in second grade). Manual dominance was evaluated as a self-report by asking children to write their names. Splitting by manual dominance, 88.1% of children were right-handed (RH) and 11.9% left-handed (LH). In particular, the ratio of LH children was 14.4% in first grade and 8.9% in second grade, 10.6% in girls and 13.1% in boys. Because we focused on healthy motor behavior, the results of the six children with a certificate of disability were excluded from the data analysis. In this way, we were considering the setting of an inclusive experience as they were allowed to be part of the testing experience. In the remaining sample there were no children with graphic or writing problems. We randomly selected a subgroup of seven school classes to administer the Pictorial Scale of Perceived TABLE 1 Boys and girls distribution across school grades Grade Gender First Second Total Girls 60 52 112 Boys 72 55 127 Total 132 107 239 4 of 15 Movement Skill Competence-2 (PMSC-2; Barnett, Robinson, et al., 2015). This was due to time and logistic constraints, Thus, only a subsample of n = 95 children (42% of the first grade and 58% of the second grade, 49% boys and 51% girls, 88% RH and 12% LH) completed this test. 2.2 | Materials and apparatus The grapho-motor test was performed on a tablet PC, carried out following an established procedure (Giammarco et al., 2016) which involved a blank page with a square and a diamond represented in four gray segments shown on a tablet screen (Samsung Galaxy note, 10.1 in., 1280  800). The software was developed by the authors of the cited study (Giammarco et al., 2016) and got by them, who also positively tested the retest reliability with Primary school children. The size of the square and diamond were large enough to cover the entire screen of the tablet (see the original article for a deeper description and images). Children were supposed to redraw the shapes with a digital pen. The Pictorial Scale of Perceived Movement Skill Competence-2 (PMSC-2; Barnett, Robinson, et al., 2015) was administered on printed A4 sheets. It is a widely used pictorial scale used to assess perceived competence on object control—hand competence skills (C), locomotor—dynamic leg competence skills (L), and other skills involved in free time, active play activities (A). Anthropometric testing required a measuring tape and a portable scale. Reliability of this scale was demonstrated for the age group of the present study (internal consistency of 0.73 and test–retest reliability of 0.83 for children aged 5– 7 years; Barnett, Ridgers, et al., 2015; internal consistency of 0.72 and test–retest reliability of 0.73 for children aged 5–8 years: Barnett, Robinson, et al., 2015). Fine motor coordination was assessed with two tests: a transitive (tool-related) action test (Floppy) and an intransitive (non-tool related) action test (Thumb). These tests required a box of floppy disks and a stopwatch; they have been already used for testing children of similar ages (Bondi, Robazza, & Pietrangelo, 2020; Bondi, Robazza, Russo, et al., 2020). For procedural details, see the following sections. 2.3 | Procedure BONDI ET AL. approved by the Italian Olympic Committee (CONI) and by the regional Board of Education of the Italian Ministry of Education, University and Research (MIUR), Abruzzo region, Italy. Permission was obtained by the school administration and informed consent was signed by the parents of the children. The study conformed to the Declaration of Helsinki. Children's motor skills were tested individually. Children were assessed at their school in the presence of the teachers, during regular physical education time. The assessment of the subsample of N = 95 children who completed the PMSC-2 assessment procedure took around 10–15 min per child. In the first data collection session, after the PMSC-2 was administered, we collected anthropometric measures and conducted the fitness test (Shuttle test, see below). In the following data collection session, the grapho-motor tablet task and the fine motor tasks were given in random order. The test–retest reliability of Floppy and Thumb tests was assessed by the same observer in three consecutive trials during this data collection session: the Intraclass Correlation Coefficients (ICCs) were good-to-excellent (Koo & Li, 2016), see Table 2. Therefore, the data of first repetition were used for further analyses. The total testing time of the second data collection session was about 15 min per child, plus 5 min for retest. Of the entire sample, we collected height, weight, and waist circumference as anthropometric data, then we calculated body mass index (BMI) and waist-toheight ratio (WtHR; Mamen & Fredriksen, 2018). We assessed speed and agility through the 4  10 m shuttle run test (Shuttle; Ruiz et al., 2011), that is a running and turning test at greatest speed: briefly, two parallel lines are drawn on the floor 10 m apart; the child runs from the starting to the other line and returns with a sponge; the sponge is changed by another sponge, then the child goes back running to the opposite line and changes the sponge by another one and finally runs back to the start. The time to complete the task was the outcome variable. The grapho-motor test started with a familiarization phase and thereafter consisted of two trials: the first trial T A B L E 2 Reliability coefficients of floppy and thumb tests (N = 233) Test The cross-sectional design encompassed physiological, anthropometric, and motor measurements. The time frame of our study consisted of 4–6 months after the onset of the scholastic season. The study ethics was Grade First Second Overall Floppy 0.95 0.76 0.88 Thumb 0.96 0.90 0.93 BONDI ET AL. 5 of 15 2.4 | Data generation T A B L E 3 Exploratory factor analysis for four tablet test parameters (N = 233) Factor 1 0.749 Strokes Pressure / Speed 0.997 Quality 0.572 Note: Root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = .102; TuckerLewis index (TLI) = .947; χ 2 (2, 239) = 5.97, p = .050. required to trace as accurate as possible (Best); the second trial required to trace as accurate as possible with a higher velocity (Fast). All lines drawn with the pen on the tablet screen were stored as strokes. The pressure and the speed exerted on the pen were calculated as scores of the average over the length of lines. For both trials, we acquired the measures of the number of Strokes, the Pressure, the Speed as well as the mean oscillation (quadratic deviation of the points) from the graphic line (Quality). Recording data were processed whenever the digital pen exerted a pressure on the screen. Using this tool permitted us to simulate the effective functional parameters of drawing and to measure the required objective parameters. The Floppy test was used to assess manual dexterity (Martzog et al., 2019). It required participants to insert 12 floppy disks one at a time in the floppy box, as fast as possible, holding the box while inserting the disks (to provide a different role for each hand). The rationale for using this test was based on manual dexterity assessment and, with respect to similar tests—like the coins task of M-ABC 2 battery—the present test was not affected by motor experience of common tasks (Zoia et al., 2018). This test required a strategy to better carry out the task, with increased difficulty during execution (as the disks are placed into the box, the empty space is reduced). The Thumb test was used to assess upper limb coordination, as well as speed-dominated skills (Martzog et al., 2019). Participants were required to touch each finger of one hand with the thumb of the same hand in an alternating pattern (5th, 4th, 3rd, and then 2nd finger, and reverse), as fast as possible. The rationale for using this test was based on the visualmotor control assessment of the BOT-2 battery (Bruininks & Bruininks, 2005) which is a common task in clinical assessment (finger–thumb test) and motor rehabilitation. As for the Floppy test, the outcome variable was time of completing as measured with a stopwatch. We conducted an Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA) on four tablet test parameters, see Table 3, with a Maximum Likelihood rotation. We obtained only one fine motor skills factor, based on an eigenvalue >1 and on the scree plot. We then conducted a Confirmatory Structural Equation Modeling (SEM), considering the expected correlations, and putting into the model the factor from the EFA in Best condition, the same factor in Fast condition, the hypothesized relationships between Best and Fast conditions. We tested age and gender in the model, evaluating fit measures to choose the best model. Such measures suggested including gender and the correlation between Speed and Quality in Best condition. With more than two predictors, it is possible to obtain values larger than +1, as the value of the path between TPB and Speed-B. Error calculation method: Robust; estimator: Diagonally Weighted Least Squares (DWLS). Fit measures: RMSEA = .042, SRMR = .050, TLI = .987, CFI = .995, GFI = .989; χ 2 (9, 239) = 11.56, p = .239. TPB: Tablet Performance Best; TPF: Tablet Performance Fast; B: Best; F: Fast. Four equations were produced: • Girls TPB: Strokes) • Girls TPF: Strokes) • Boys TPB: Strokes) • Boys TPF: Strokes) Quality + (38.28  Speed) (11.62  Quality + (65.10  Speed) (2.76  Quality + (55.66  Speed) (22.78  Quality + (70.04  Speed) (2.22  We evaluated the fit measures for different models, concluding that the best model was the one shown in Figure 1. We applied the assumption of normality and multivariate normality, and subsequently a nonparanormal transformation (Liu et al., 2009) before conducting correlations. The coefficients in Figure 1 were used to build unweighted undirected graphs. The analysis revealed that gender had to be taken into account when defining the structural model of tracing performance. Finally, the four equations were used to calculate TPB and TPF, separately for both boys and girls. 2.5 | Data analysis Statistical analyses were carried out using GraphPad Prism Software, version 9 (GraphPad Software, La Jolla, USA), R-based open-source software Jamovi (https:// www.jamovi.org) and Jasp (https://jasp-stats.org). Data 6 of 15 BONDI ET AL. networks because of the smaller sample sizes of the subgroups. For the details of network analysis, refer to the figure legends. 3 | RESULTS F I G U R E 1 Confirmatory structural equation modeling (SEM) of tablet test parameters. P: Percentile; TPB: Tablet Performance Best; TPF: Tablet Performance Fast; BMI: Body mass index; the showed numbers are the standardized coefficient (standardizing both label and observed variables) distributions were analyzed using Shapiro–Wilk and D'Agostino-Pearson methods. Analysis of outliers was conducted with the ROUT method, implemented in Prism (Motulsky & Brown, 2006). An alpha value of .05 was considered as statistically significant. Considering the assumption check, robust ANOVA and robust t-test were chosen to run the comparison tests. In particular, a series of robust ANOVAs (median method) were carried out to test the gender  age effect on TPB, TPF, Shuttle, Floppy, Thumb, BMI, WtHR, Control, Locomotor, Active play. In case of a significant interaction effect, post-hoc analyses with Tukey correction for multiple comparisons were conducted. Robust t-tests (Yuen's test with 0.1 trimmed proportion and median method), in addition to the nonparametric Mann–Whitney U test, were carried out for the left versus right-side manual dominance effect on TPB, TPF, Shuttle, Floppy, Thumb, BMI, and WtHR. The links between graphonomic outcomes, other fine motor skills, fitness, and anthropometric status, as well as perceived competence, were addressed with network models, using correlations as estimators. Further network analyses were conducted with TPB, TPF, Shuttle, Floppy, Thumb, BMI and WtHR, splitting by age and gender. PMSC-2 parameters were excluded from the split The BMI values of participants were compared to the normative values of provided by the World Health Organization (WHO, 2007). Our P50 values ranged between normative P50 and P75 matching for the same gender and age: BMI of our participants was therefore higher than this reference. We further classified the children according to the obesity cut-off norms of P97 from the WHO reference (Valerio et al., 2018). As a result, 24.76% of children in our sample had obesity. The WtHR values of our participants were compared to the recent values reported by Santomauro et al. (2017) on Italian children from Tuscany, a region in central Italy, north of Abruzzo region. Our P50 values ranged between their P50 and P75 matching for the same gender and age. Therefore, WtHR values of our participants were higher than this reference. We further characterized the participants according to the health hazard cut-off of 0.50 (Maffeis et al., 2008). We found 14.35% of children to be at metabolic risk. Shuttle performances of participants were compared to international norms for this task (Kolimechkov et al., 2019). Median of boys in first grade were between P30 and P40 of that reference, girls in first grade were between P50 and P60; boys in second grade were between P50 and P60, girls in second grade were between P60 and P70. Shuttle performance was therefore overall higher than this reference, except for boys in the school entrance class. PMSC-2 values of participants were compared to two different samples of school children data reported by Barnett, Robinson, et al. (2015). Australian children rated on average 19.9 in object control subscale (C) and 20.7 in locomotor subscale (L); American children rated 20.5 in C and 20.7 in L; present study, children rated 20.4 in C and 22.2 in L. Participants of our study therefore rated higher on locomotor subscale of PMSC-2 than these reference values. 3.1 | Comparison by gender and grade Group means are shown in Tables 4 and 5. Results were analyzed through a series of robust ANOVAs with the median method. All the p values of statistical comparisons are shown in Tables S1 and S2, Supporting BONDI ET AL. TABLE 4 7 of 15 Results of the comparisons by grade and gender, N = 239 Graphonomics Fine motor skills TPB TPF Floppy time (s) First 979 1794 19.9 Second 832 1638 19.2 First 413 906 Second 348 First Second Grade Q1 Girls Boys Median Girls Boys Anthropometrics Fitness BMI (kg/m2) WtHR Shuttle time (s) 8.62 15.7 0.443 15.2 7.53 15.5 0.429 13.6 21.1 8.58 15.9 0.447 14.5 978 19.6 7.89 16.2 0.465 13.1 1292 2424 22.8 10.00 17.2 0.468 15.8 1291 2080 20.0 8.29 16.5 0.448 14.4 649 1191 24.2 16.7 0.464 15.6 18.0 0.478 13.9 19.4 0.492 16.6 18.9 0.468 15.8 First Second Q3 Girls Boys Thumb time (s) 10.6 848 1115 21.5 First 1740 2908 25.4 Second 1643 2590 22.7 First 1011 1711 27.5 12.4 17.9 0.486 16.9 Second 1066 1489 24.3 10.8 20.4 0.499 15.2 983 1643 22.4 9.4 16.9 0.466 15.1 Median (whole sample) 9.29 11.6 9.81 Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; Q, quartile; TPB, Tablet Performance Best; TPF, Tablet Performance Fast; WtHR, waist-to-height ratio. T A B L E 5 Results of the comparisons in PMSC questionnaire by grade and gender, N = 95 Q1 Girls Boys Mdn Girls Boys Q3 Girls Boys Mdn (whole sample) Grade C L A First 16.0 20.5 17.0 Second 19.0 21.0 17.0 First 20.0 20.0 15.0 Second 20.5 22.5 16.0 First 19.0 23.0 19.0 Second 20.5 23.0 20.0 First 21.0 23.0 18.5 Second 22.0 23.0 19.0 First 21.5 24.0 22.0 Second 21.0 24.0 23.0 First 23.0 24.0 22.8 Second 23.0 24.0 21.5 21.0 23.0 19.0 Abbreviations: A, active play; C, object control; L, locomotor; Mdn, median; Q, quartile. Information. Box and whiskers plots of the results split by gender and grade are shown in Figure 2 Overall, the higher the speed of the pen on the screen, the lower the quality of drawing. Regarding the tablet test, we found strong differences by gender in both the TPB and TPF (Q1,188 = 23.50, p < .001, partial η2 = .239; Q1,165 = 59.52, p < .001, partial η2 = .376). The gender  age interaction was not significant (p = .127 and p = .323, respectively). Task-specific analyses showed that older children (Mdn = 14.31 s) performed better than younger ones (Mdn = 15.78 s) in the Shuttle test which measures the fitness domains of speed and agility (Q1,204 = 27.17, p < .001, partial η2 = .139). Older children (Q1,178 = 15.33, p = .040, partial η2 = .042) and girls (Q1,178 = 4.22, p < .001, partial η2 = .042) performed better in the Floppy test which measures fine coordination on a tool-related (namely transitive) action. Both boys and girls performed better with age in the Thumb test (Q1,191 = df1 = 1, df2 = 191, p < .001, partial η2 = .053) which measures fine coordination of fingers on one hand without holding a tool (namely intransitive action). There was a main effect of gender on the WtHR (girls showed a higher WtHR than boys, Q1,200 = 4.74, p = .029, partial η2 = .024), and a significant two-way interaction (Q1,198 = 8.17, p = .004, partial η2 = .037); post-hoc analysis revealed no significant difference between boys and girls in the first grade, whereas boys showed higher WtHR values (Mdn = 0.48) than girls (Mdn = 0.45) in the second grade (pTukey = .005, Cohen's d = 0.699). There was a difference between boys and girls by sex in the BMI (gender  age comparison: Q1,199 = 6.49, p = .011, partial η2 = .035); post-hoc analysis revealed no significant difference in the first grade, but a tendency for girls to show lower BMI values (Mdn = 16.5 kg/m2) than boys (Mdn = 18.0 kg/m2) in the second grade (pTukey = .082, Cohen's d = 0.496). For what concerns the PMSC-2 test, boys perceived their object control skills to be higher than girls 8 of 15 BONDI ET AL. F I G U R E 2 Box and whiskers plots of anthropometric and motor skills results. Each of the three panels represent min-to-max lines and median with IQR boxes. For each dataset, girls (white boxes) and boys (gray boxes) results are shown split by school grade TABLE 6 analysis Centrality measures per variable in overall network Closeness Active play BMI Control F I G U R E 3 Network plot of the 10 parameters. The bolder the line, the higher the weight of the correlation between nodes. Nodes of the same colors belong to the same group, according to clustering measures of the analysis. Nodes are positioned using the Fruchterman-Reingold algorithm, based on the strength of connections. TPB: Tablet Performance Best; TPF: Tablet Performance Fast; BMI: Body mass index; WtHR: Waist-to-height ratio (Mdn = 22 vs. 20; Q1,93 = 4.45, p = .035, partial η2 = .093), see Table 5, while no significant differences were found in Locomotor or Active play subscales. 3.2 | Network analysis To analyze the synchronous relationships between variables, we conducted a network analysis. In Figures 2 and 3, the task components that refer to one test are depicted in the same color. Network clusters are marked by the connections between these components, with thicker lines showing stronger connections. The analysis of the sample of N = 95 revealed three fundamental clusters. One is the PMSC-2 cluster, with the 0.091 0.859 0.180 Strength 0.907 0.410 1.031 Floppy 1.407 0.019 Locomotor 0.788 1.414 Shuttle 0.604 0.997 TPB 0.729 0.831 TPF 2.121 1.736 Thumb 0.365 0.428 WtHR 0.305 0.250 Note: “Closeness” refers to the inverse of the “peripherality” (the sum of the shortest paths starting from the node of interest), “strength” refers to the centrality “degree” (sum of absolute weights) of the node of interest. Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; TPB, Tablet Performance Best; TPF, Tablet Performance Fast; WtHR, waist-to-height ratio. three subscales strongly linked to each other with the following weights: Locomotor and Control = .851, Locomotor and Active play = .894, Control and Active play = .879. The second is the Fitness cluster, with the anthropometric measures and the Shuttle test showing moderate links of the Shuttle test with both BMI (weight = .396) and WtHR (weight = .399), and a strong link between WtHR and BMI (weight = .902). Finally, the third fundamental cluster is the Fine Motor Control cluster, with moderate links between the different tablet and floppy parameters. In particular, the following weights were observed: Floppy and Thumb = .589, Floppy and TPB = .547, Floppy and TPF = .368, Thumb and TPB = .522, Thumb and TPF = .510, TPB and TPF = .433. The centrality measures revealed Floppy to be the most central, although weakly, node of the network (see Table 6). BONDI ET AL. 9 of 15 F I G U R E 4 Network plots split by gender and school grade. The drawings reflect the strength, the closeness, and the clustering of the variables. The bolder the line, the higher the weight of the correlation between nodes. Nodes of the same colors belong to the same group, according to clustering measures of the analysis. Nodes are positioned in space using the Fruchterman-Reingold algorithm, based on the strength of connections. TPB: Tablet Performance Best; TPF: Tablet Performance Fast; BMI: Body mass index; WtHR: Waist-to-height ratio We then ran the networks split by gender and age which revealed different models for boys and girls, see Figure 4. Considering the size of subsamples, we run this model without the aim to infer results, but to provide pilot evidence to be possibly confirmed by ad hoc designed studies. The Shuttle, WtHR, and BMI were strongly linked in girls of both grades, but only to some degree in the younger group of boys. Thumb and Floppy were more linked in second than in first grade in girls showing a growing network of fine motor skills, but in boys, thumb and floppy were linked in both grades. Moreover, in boys, the tablet parameters became linked with thumbs and floppy task showing a generalization of fine motor expertise extending to computers. In contrast, in the older grade of girls, fast tablet motions were not networked anymore. 4 | DISCUSSION A structured analysis of various motor tasks provides a better understanding of the motor tasks. In this regard, network analyses allow going beyond the typical correlation analyses, thanks to centrality measures and clustering. We examined the important transition into school when fine motor skills become essential for learning how to write. We addressed the structured links between grapho-motor performances and other fine motor skills, speed and agility, anthropometrics, and self-perception of motor skills in early school-age children. Our result demonstrated that, first, grapho-motor skills varied between boys and girls and exhibited gender-related differences, second, that three clusters (namely, perceived competence, fitness, and fine motor skills) emerged from the network model, and third, that the interconnectedness of 10 of 15 fine motor skills with other domains may change with age and gender during early school age. 4.1 | Fine motor skills, cognitive functions, and tasks Our results showed that when the strategy to carry out the grapho-motor task was focused on accuracy, this reduced the speed of the graphical task which confirms the finding by Giammarco et al. (2016) (see also LangeKüttner, 1998) who observed that the slower the speed, the better is the quality. The speed accuracy trade-off has been studied widely, with evidence also in graphic tasks for school age children (e.g., Smits-Engelsman et al., 2006). Looking at the underlying factors of increased accuracy, we refer to Simpson et al. (2017), who suggested that inhibition is the key ability that enables children to draw recognizable objects. Inhibition may be evoked, for instance, when stopping drawing a line of overlapping figures (hidden line elimination) (e.g., Chen & Holman, 1989; Lange-Küttner, 2000). This possible inhibition requirement was here particularly evident in boys, although further research should confirm this finding with a study designed to discriminate slow vs fast tasks. The reason can be found in the gender-based differences in visuo-motor integration. For instance, controlling oscillations in tracing performance varies in boys but not in girls (Giammarco et al., 2016; TabatabaeyMashadi et al., 2015). Drawing development also differs between boys and girls in the near-far conceptualization of pictorial space as boys drew silhouettes as they would be visible from afar, while girls focused on ornaments that could be seen close-up (Lange-Küttner, 2011). We could prove that the development of fine motor skills varies in boys and girls showing gender and agerelated clusters that added further learning-related evidence to the field of graphonomics and grapho-motor performances. Beyond the strong individual motivation in the aim of accomplishing the motor task, both strategy and adaptation to the velocity constraint were gender-related. Fine motor skills seemed to be already networked in girls at the beginning of school but became linked and generalized in boys in the second year supported the view that sex differences; this result should be considered to understand performance strategies and underlying motor, cognitive, and neuronal patterns. This showed how a task can be solved by the two genders with different strategies and may shed new understanding on development of competence (Lange-Küttner, 2017). Indeed, also in the current study, strategy and performance on fine motor control tests were task-related. The link between the two tablet parameters and the two fine BONDI ET AL. coordinative tests were only small and changed with regards to age and gender, as shown in the networks. This dynamic perspective was also shown by SnappChilds and colleagues who reported specific training effects to eliminate drawing performance differences between ages (7–8 vs 10–12-year old children) and different coordination skill levels (Snapp-Childs et al., 2015). 4.2 | Observed and perceived physical performances Our results of Shuttle test were comparable to findings of the study of Kolimechkov et al. (2019). Instead, our participants' data were slightly worse on anthropometric indexes (Santomauro et al., 2017; WHO, 2007). In particular, the high percentage of children over the limits of overweight and health hazard may highlight an alarming situation for public health, with the need for better interventions. However, we found that even if our participants performed quite well on the Shuttle test, they could nevertheless have a poor anthropometric status. The moderate positive weights revealed by network analysis of Shuttle with both BMI and WtHR nevertheless support the call for policy approaches to improve anthropometric status as a modifiable factor to enhance physical fitness (Zaqout et al., 2016). Notwithstanding the anthropometric status, our participants reported high values of perceived movement skills competence. Fine and gross motor skills, fitness performance and morpho-functional parameters represent necessary modules to be evaluated objectively in order to define developmental trajectories (Bondi, Di Sano, Verratti, et al., 2020). Assessment of perceived competence is a measure that integrates such evaluations from a subjective point of view and allows to focus physical education on the possible mismatch between objective and perceived skills. In our study, boys scored higher than girls on the object control sub-scale; this result agrees with the findings of Estevan et al. (2018) on Spanish children. However, this higher self-evaluation is not accompanied by better objective results: ours and other results (Flatters et al., 2014) showed that young girls perform better than young boys on manual skills. Further studies should therefore increase the knowledge about gender differences in the relationship between objective and subjective motor skills, the developmental course of these linkages, sports and social consequences and the related educational interventions. A careful examination of the alignment of the objective skills and children's subjective ratings will help to strengthen the knowledge on these topics. Beyond the subjective measures of perceived skills, further studies should address also some BONDI ET AL. parameters of the biopsychosocial framework (Bortoli et al., 2018) and some parameters of personal feeling such as enjoyment in physical activity experiences (Carraro et al., 2008). 4.3 | Fine motor skills in children's development We consider fine motor control as an emergent issue in the understanding of motor behavior for auxological (auxology is the science of human growth), educational and kinesiological studies. The wide range of expressions of motor control suggests considering all the related subdomains. The current study supports the argument of addressing the several fine motor skills, in addition to gross motor skills, for children. Improvement of fine motor skills in children is specific to practice of fine motor skills, even in an educationoriented household (Suggate et al., 2017). However, fine motor coordination was demonstrated in two studies to be of more importance for math than for literacy because of the visual–spatial imagery required in both pictorial and mathematical space (Pitchford et al., 2016). Thus, one could predict that stimulation of fine motor skills may even be relevant for the improvement of higher cognitive skills in the prepubertal period. More attention should be directed toward this field, providing a wide range of different motor experiences. Although being a fundamental learning experience in the beginning of school when learning to write. Fine motor skills represent a fundamental learning experience in the beginning of school when learning to write. However, children rarely choose to use these skills in other learning contexts such as free play, independently of whether their fine motor skills are well, or less well developed (Marr et al., 2004). We should also take advantage by acquiring longitudinal track records of fine motor coordination data, in the same way as already reported for gross-motor coordination (Reikerås et al., 2017; Vandorpe et al., 2012). Thus, both higher attention on finecoordinative skills in early education and implementation of gross-coordinative activities and evaluation need more articulated planning and organization of physical education, addressing motor control and learning as a whole. Motor control also reflects gender differences in fine motor coordination through the developmental process. Reikerås et al. (2017) showed girls to have a more pronounced and anticipatory fine motor coordination than boys. Also Flatters et al. (2014) reported superior manual control abilities in prepubescent girls performing novel tasks. We contributed to the field showing that 11 of 15 the relationship between fine motor coordination and other domains is different by age and gender in early school age. Both the clustering of nodes of fine motor control and the clustering of physical fitness with anthropometric measures were likely influenced by gender, as shown in the differential networks by the differences in both the nodes positioning and the weight of edges. This topic is worth further investigating with longitudinal studies and we thus suggest that the relationships between motor tasks themselves and other physical outcomes might follow gender-based development trajectories. Also training studies (van Abswoude et al., 2019) may be interesting to the emerging field of research in fine motor skills. Cognition and motor skills are interrelated at the level of brain structure, supported by similar cortical neural substrates (Pangelinan et al., 2011). Therefore, further studies are needed to better characterize the relationships of morphological and neural connections with different coordinative domains, and the advantages of optimizing sensory–motor integration during precisely controlled hand movements to foster the learning of fine motor skills (Ose Askvik et al., 2020; van der Meer & van der Weel, 2017). Interesting findings may result from studies focused on evaluating differences based on handedness and asymmetries, as they have been shown to be related to the type of manual task exerted (Bondi, Prete, Malatesta, & Robazza, 2020; Bondi, Robazza, & Pietrangelo, 2020). 4.4 | Limitations The study did not come without limitations, such as the absence of data about social background and academic performance. One other gap in the assessment may have been that we did not measure previous use of digital devices that might have affected fine motor skills (Petrigna et al., 2021). Thus, the results of the study recommend not only including fine motor skills in early school age research, but also studying them as a special cluster. 5 | CONCLUSIONS Our network analysis revealed three specific clusters: perceived competence, fitness, and fine motor skills. Given the relative independence of these physical performance areas, we suggest focusing on these three clusters as separate areas in physical education. Fine motor skills deserve further consideration in early school age, since these skills are important in a variety 12 of 15 of tasks like sorting objects, handwriting, and handling digital media. The latter has become even more urgent during distant learning in the Covid lockdowns. Digital devices represent fashionable and effective tools to be used under an educational perspective; in remote teaching times, teaching and evaluation of academic homework inevitably require digital devices for monitoring and developing children's fine motor skills. Whether learning to write and thus increased fine motor control influences manual dominance is still an open question and further studies should provide clear insights. Motor development can be interpreted in the paradigm of Dynamic System Theory (Smith & Thelen, 2003): person, task, and environment interact dynamically and critical changes in one or more subsystems can produce improvements in the resulting performance. From this perspective, the human movement system is a complex network of subsystems consisting of a plethora of interacting components (Glazier et al., 2003). The constraint of repetition in task-specific practice greatly enhances related skills and has more or less potential to be transferred to other skills. Even a slight change in one component of the dynamic system can lead to reorganization of structure and developmental change (Smith & Thelen, 2003). Therefore, both interventions and evaluations need to be carefully and finely tuned. Specific evidence of several motor tests can lead to a proposal of “sentinels” for early identification of various neural and motor deficits. Network research provides an effective framework for discussing fine and gross motor skills during developmental age; network models can aid better modeling of motor tasks and design of smart classrooms, while providing insights into novel interactive technologies. A U T H O R C O N T R I B U T I O NS Danilo Bondi: Conceptualization (lead); formal analysis (lead); investigation (lead); methodology (lead); resources (supporting); visualization (lead); writing – original draft (lead); writing – review and editing (equal). Claudio Robazza: Conceptualization (supporting); methodology (supporting); project administration (equal); resources (lead); supervision (lead); writing – original draft (supporting); writing – review and editing (equal). Christiane Lange-Küttner: Supervision (supporting); writing – original draft (equal); writing – review and editing (lead). Tiziana Pietrangelo: Conceptualization (equal); funding acquisition (lead); investigation (supporting); methodology (supporting); project administration (equal); resources (supporting); supervision (lead); writing – original draft (supporting); writing – review and editing (supporting). BONDI ET AL. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The authors would like to thank PE experts Sara Ienni and Maristella Melfi who helped with tests and logistics. The authors would also like to thank teachers and both scholastic and sports administrations. Special thanks go to all the children involved in the study. This work was supported by a ‘G. d'Annunzio’ University grant, by 2012N8YJC3_003 PRIN National Grant to Tiziana Pietrangelo and by the “Departments of Excellence 2018-2022” initiative of the Italian Ministry of Education, University and Research for the Department of Neuroscience, Imaging and Clinical Sciences (DNISC) of the University of Chieti-Pescara. Open Access Funding provided by Universita degli Studi Gabriele d'Annunzio Chieti Pescara within the CRUI-CARE Agreement. CONFLICT OF INTEREST The authors declare no competing interests. DA TA AVAI LA BI LI TY S T ATE ME NT The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request. ORCID Danilo Bondi https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1911-3606 RE FER EN CES Augustijn, M. J. C. M., Deconinck, F. J. A., D'Hondt, E., Van Acker, L., De Guchtenaere, A., Lenoir, M., & Caeyenberghs, K. (2018). Reduced motor competence in children with obesity is associated with structural differences in the cerebellar peduncles. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 12(4), 1000–1010. https://doi. org/10.1007/s11682-017-9760-5 Barnett, L. M., Ridgers, N. D., Zask, A., & Salmon, J. (2015). Face validity and reliability of a pictorial instrument for assessing fundamental movement skill perceived competence in young children. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 18(1), 98– 102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2013.12.004 Barnett, L. M., Robinson, L. E., Webster, E. K., & Ridgers, N. D. (2015). Reliability of the pictorial scale of perceived movement skill competence in 2 diverse samples of Young children. Journal of Physical Activity & Health, 12(8), 1045–1051. https://doi. org/10.1123/jpah.2014-0141 Barnett, L. M., van Beurden, E., Morgan, P. J., Brooks, L. O., & Beard, J. R. (2009). Childhood motor skill proficiency as a predictor of adolescent physical activity. The Journal of Adolescent Health, 44(3), 252–259. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth. 2008.07.004 Bondi, D., Di Sano, S., Verratti, V., Neri, G., Aureli, T., & Pietrangelo, T. (2020). New insight on motor behavior: The link between the hopping task and the tracing performance as hint of gross and fine motor functions. Motor Control, 24(3), 349– 364. https://doi.org/10.1123/mc.2019-0044 Bondi, D., Prete, G., Malatesta, G., & Robazza, C. (2020). Laterality in children: Evidence for task-dependent lateralization of motor BONDI ET AL. functions. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(18), 6705. https://doi.org/10.3390/ ijerph17186705 Bondi, D., Robazza, C., & Pietrangelo, T. (2020). The assessment of task-dependent manual laterality in second grade students. Turkish Journal of Sports Medicine., 55, 239–245. https://doi. org/10.5152/tjsm.2020.182 Bondi, D., Robazza, C., Russo, E., Russo, P., & Pietrangelo, T. (2020). Monitoring physical and motor traits in primary school: A local harmful situation for older children. The Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness., 61, 428–434. https://doi. org/10.23736/S0022-4707.20.11251-9 Bortoli, L., Vitali, F., Di Battista, R., Ruiz, M. C., & Robazza, C. (2018). Initial validation of the Psychobiosocial states in physical education (PBS-SPE) scale. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02446 Bruininks, R. H., & Bruininks, B. D. (2005). Bruininks-Oseretsky test of motor proficiency (Second ed.). AGS Publishing. https://www. pearsonclinical.com/therapy/products/100000648/bruininksoseretsky-test-of-motor-proficiency-second-edition-bot-2.html Carraro, A., Young, M. C., & Robazza, C. (2008). A contribution to the validation of the physical activity enjoyment scale in an Italian sample. Social Behavior and Personality, 36(7), 911–918. https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2008.36.7.911 Chen, M. J., & Holman, J. (1989). Emergence of drawing devices for Total and partial occlusion: A longitudinal study. Perception, 18(4), 445–455. https://doi.org/10.1068/p180445 Colella, D., Morano, M., Robazza, C., & Bortoli, L. (2009). Body image, perceived physical ability, and motor performance in nonoverweight and overweight Italian children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 108(1), 209–218. https://doi.org/10.2466/PMS.108. 1.209-218 D'Hondt, E., Deforche, B., De Bourdeaudhuij, I., & Lenoir, M. (2008). Childhood obesity affects fine motor skill performance under different postural constraints. Neuroscience Letters, 440(1), 72–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008.05.056 Dinkel, D., & Snyder, K. (2020). Exploring gender differences in infant motor development related to parent's promotion of play. Infant Behavior & Development, 59, 101440. https://doi.org/10. 1016/j.infbeh.2020.101440 Escolano-Pérez, E., Sanchez-L opez, C. R., & Herrero-Nivela, M. L. (2021). Early environmental and biological influences on preschool motor skills: Implications for early childhood care and education. Frontiers in Psychology, 12. https://doi.org/10.3389/ fpsyg.2021.725832 Estevan, I., Molina-García, J., Abbott, G., Bowe, S. J., Castillo, I., & Barnett, L. M. (2018). Evidence of reliability and validity for the pictorial scale of perceived movement skill competence in Spanish children. Journal of Motor Learning and Development, 6(s2), S205–S222. https://doi.org/ 10.1123/jmld.2016-0065 Feder, K. P., & Majnemer, A. (2007). Handwriting development, competency, and intervention. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 49(4), 312–317. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.14698749.2007.00312.x Flatters, I., Hill, L. J. B., Williams, J. H. G., Barber, S. E., & MonWilliams, M. (2014). Manual control age and sex differences in 4 to 11 year old children. PLoS One, 9(2), e88692. https://doi. org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088692 13 of 15 Gaul, D., & Issartel, J. (2016). Fine motor skill proficiency in typically developing children: On or off the maturation track? Human Movement Science, 46, 78–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. humov.2015.12.011 Ghassabian, A., Sundaram, R., Bell, E., Bello, S. C., Kus, C., & Yeung, E. (2016). Gross motor milestones and subsequent development. Pediatrics, 138(1), e20154372. https://doi.org/10. 1542/peds.2015-4372 Giammarco, E., Di Sano, S., Aureli, T., Cerratti, P., Fanò-Illic, G., & Pietrangelo, T. (2016). Psychological and physiological processes in figure-tracing abilities measured using a tablet computer: A study with 7 and 9 years old children. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01528 Glazier, P. S., Davids, K., & Bartlett, R. M. (2003). Dynamical systems theory: A relevant framework for performance-oriented sports biomechanics research. Sportscience, 7. http://www. sportsci.org/jour/03/psg.htm Griffiths, A., Toovey, R., Morgan, P. E., & Spittle, A. J. (2018). Psychometric properties of gross motor assessment tools for children: A systematic review. BMJ Open, 8(10), e021734. https:// doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2018-021734 Kolimechkov, S., Petrov, L., & Alexandrova, A. (2019). Alpha-fit test battery norms for children and adolescent from 5 to 18 years of age obtained by a linear interpolation of existing European physical fitness references. European Journal of Physical Education and Sport Science, 5(4), 1-14. https://oapub.org/edu/index. php/ejep/article/view/2221 Koo, T. K., & Li, M. Y. (2016). A guideline of selecting and reporting Intraclass correlation coefficients for reliability research. Journal of Chiropractic Medicine, 15(2), 155–163. https://doi.org/10. 1016/j.jcm.2016.02.012 Lange-Küttner, C. (1998). Pressure, velocity, and time in speeded drawing of basic graphic patterns by young children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 86(3 Pt 2), 1299–1310. https://doi.org/10. 2466/pms.1998.86.3c.1299 Lange-Küttner, C. (2000). The role of object violation in the development of visual analysis. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 90(1), 3– 24. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.2000.90.1.3 Lange-Küttner, C. (2011). Sex differences in visual realism in drawings of animate and inanimate objects. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 113(2), 439–453. https://doi.org/10.2466/04.10.24.PMS. 113.5.439-453 Lange-Küttner, C. (2017). Sex differences as a statistical variable, Sex differences as a statistical variable. In Gender differences in different contexts. InTech. https://www.intechopen.com/books/ gender-differences-in-different-contexts/sex-differences-as-astatistical-variable Lange-Küttner, C., & Bosco, G. (2016). On being in the wrong place: The role of Children's conceptual understanding and ballgame experience when judging a football player's offside position. International Journal of Developmental Science, 10(1–2), 73–84. https://doi.org/10.3233/DEV-160185 Lange-Küttner, C., Kerzmann, A., & Heckhausen, J. (2002). The emergence of visually realistic contour in the drawing of the human figure. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20(3), 439–463. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151002320620415 Lange-Küttner, C., & Kochhar, R. (2020). Fine motor skills and unsystematic spatial binding in the common region test: Under-inclusivity in autism Spectrum disorder and 14 of 15 over-inclusivity in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Motor Learning and Development, 8(3), 544–568. https:// doi.org/10.1123/jmld.2019-0033 Liu, H., Lafferty, J., & Wasserman, L. (2009). The nonparanormal: Semiparametric estimation of high dimensional undirected graphs. The Journal of Machine Learning Research, 10, 2295– 2328. Lockman, J. J., & Kahrs, B. A. (2017). New insights into the development of human tool use. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(4), 330–334. https://doi.org/10.1177/096372141 7692035 Maffeis, C., Banzato, C., & Talamini, G. (2008). Waist-to-height ratio, a useful index to identify high metabolic risk in overweight children. The Journal of Pediatrics, 152(2), 207–213.e2. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2007.09.021 Mamen, A., & Fredriksen, P. M. (2018). Anthropometric measures as fitness indicators in primary school children: The health oriented pedagogical project (HOPP). Scandinavian Journal of Public Health, 46(21_suppl), 48–53. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 1403494818770129 Marr, D., Cermak, S., Cohn, E. S., & Henderson, A. (2004). The relationship between fine-motor play and fine-motor skill. https://doi.org/10.1207/ NHSA Dialog, 7(1), 84–96. s19309325nhsa0701_7 Martzog, P., Stoeger, H., & Suggate, S. (2019). Relations between preschool children's fine motor skills and general cognitive abilities. Journal of Cognition and Development, 20(4), 443–465. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2019.1607862 Marzke, M. W. (1992). Evolutionary development of the human thumb. Hand Clinics, 8(1), 1–8. Matarma, T., Lagström, H., Hurme, S., Tammelin, T. H., Kulmala, J., Barnett, L. M., & Koski, P. (2018). Motor skills in association with physical activity, sedentary time, body fat, and day care attendance in 5-6-year-old children-the STEPS study. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 28(12), 2668–2676. https://doi.org/10.1111/sms.13264 Mayes, S. D., Frye, S. S., Breaux, R. P., & Calhoun, S. L. (2018). Diagnostic, demographic, and neurocognitive correlates of dysgraphia in students with ADHD, autism, learning disabilities, and Neurotypical development. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 30(4), 489–507. https://doi.org/10. 1007/s10882-018-9598-9 Meulenbroek, R. G., & Van Galen, G. P. (1988). Foreperiod duration and the analysis of motor stages in a line-drawing task. Acta Psychologica, 69(1), 19–34. Milbrath, C., & Council, J. R. (2003). Book reviews: Child art in context: A cultural and comparative perspective, the illusion of conscious will. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 22(3), 303–313. https://doi.org/10.2190/763E-RLGX-GN13-CDPV Motulsky, H. J., & Brown, R. E. (2006). Detecting outliers when fitting data with nonlinear regression – A new method based on robust nonlinear regression and the false discovery rate. BMC Bioinformatics, 7(1), 123. https://doi.org/10.1186/14712105-7-123 Ose Askvik, E., van der Weel, F. R., & van der Meer, A. L. H. (2020). The importance of cursive handwriting over typewriting for learning in the classroom: A high-density EEG study of 12-year-old children and Young adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1810. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01810 BONDI ET AL. Pangelinan, M. M., Zhang, G., VanMeter, J. W., Clark, J. E., Hatfield, B. D., & Haufler, A. J. (2011). Beyond age and gender: Relationships between cortical and subcortical brain volume and cognitive-motor abilities in school-age children. NeuroImage, 54(4), 3093–3100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. neuroimage.2010.11.021 Pesce, C., Masci, I., Marchetti, R., Vannozzi, G., & Schmidt, M. (2018). When children's perceived and actual motor competence mismatch: Sport participation and gender differences. Journal of Motor Learning and Development, 6(s2), S440–S460. https://doi.org/10.1123/jmld.2016-0081 Petrigna, L., Treigienė, M., Thomas, E., Mani, D., Pajaujiene, S., Drid, P., Lavanco, G., Palma, A., & Bianco, A. (2021). Time spent on the smartphone does not relate to manual dexterity in young adults. BMC Neuroscience, 22(1), 34. https://doi.org/10. 1186/s12868-021-00639-y Pitchford, N. J., Papini, C., Outhwaite, L. A., & Gulliford, A. (2016). Fine motor skills predict Maths ability better than they predict Reading ability in the early primary school years. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00783 Raimo, S., Santangelo, G., & Trojano, L. (2021). The neural bases of drawing. A meta-analysis and a systematic literature review of Neurofunctional studies in healthy individuals. Neuropsychology Review, 31, 689–702. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-02109494-4 Reikerås, E., Moser, T., & Tønnessen, F. E. (2017). Mathematical skills and motor life skills in toddlers: Do differences in mathematical skills reflect differences in motor skills? European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 25(1), 72–88. https:// doi.org/10.1080/1350293X.2015.1062664 Ruiz, J. R., España, V. R., Castro, J. P., Artero, E. G., Ortega, F. B., Cuenca, M. G., Jiménez, D. P., Chill on, P., Girela, M. R., Mora, J., Gutiérrez, A., Suni, J., Sjöstrom, M., & Castillo, M. J. (2011). ALPHA-fitness test battery: Health-related field-based fitness tests assessment in children and adolescents. Nutricion Hospitalaria, 26(6), 1210–1214. https://doi.org/10.1590/S021216112011000600003 Santomauro, F., Lorini, C., Pieralli, F., Niccolai, G., Picciolli, P., Vezzosi, S., & Bonaccorsi, G. (2017). Waist-to-height ratio and its associations with body mass index in a sample of Tuscan children in primary school. Italian Journal of Pediatrics, 43(1), 53. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13052-017-0372-x Santos, O. C. (2016). Training the body: The potential of AIED to support personalized motor skills learning. International Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education, 26(2), 730–755. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40593-016-0103-2 Scaradozzi, D., Guasti, L., Di Stasio, M., Miotti, B., Monteriù, A., & Blikstein, P. (2021). Makers at school, educational robotics and innovative learning environments: Research and experiences from FabLearn Italy 2019, in the Italian schools and beyond (Vol. 240). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10. 1007/978-3-030-77040-2 Scordella, A., Di Sano, S., Aureli, T., Cerratti, P., Verratti, V., FanòIllic, G., & Pietrangelo, T. (2015). The role of general dynamic coordination in the handwriting skills of children. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00580 Simpson, A., Al Ruwaili, R., Jolley, R., Leonard, H., Geeraert, N., & Riggs, K. J. (2017). Fine motor control underlies the association between response inhibition and drawing skill in early BONDI ET AL. development. Child Development, 90, 911–923. https://doi.org/ 10.1111/cdev.12949 Smith, L. B., & Thelen, E. (2003). Development as a dynamic system. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(8), 343–348. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00156-6 Smits-Engelsman, B. C. M., Sugden, D., & Duysens, J. (2006). Developmental trends in speed accuracy trade-off in 6–10-year-old children performing rapid reciprocal and discrete aiming movements. Human Movement Science, 25(1), 37–49. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.humov.2005.12.002 Snapp-Childs, W., Fath, A. J., Watson, C. A., Flatters, I., MonWilliams, M., & Bingham, G. P. (2015). Training to improve manual control in 7–8 and 10–12 year old children: Training eliminates performance differences between ages. Human Movement Science, 43, 90–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov. 2015.07.006 Strooband, K. F. B., De Rosnay, M., Okely, A. D., & Veldman, S. L. C. (2020). Systematic review and meta-analyses: Motor skill interventions to improve fine motor development in children aged birth to 6 years. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 41(4), 319–331. https://doi.org/10.1097/ DBP.0000000000000779 Suggate, S., Pufke, E., & Stoeger, H. (2018). Do fine motor skills contribute to early reading development? Journal of Research in Reading, 41(1), 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12081 Suggate, S., Stoeger, H., & Pufke, E. (2017). Relations between playing activities and fine motor development. Early Child Development and Care, 187(8), 1297–1310. https://doi.org/10. 1080/03004430.2016.1167047 Tabatabaey-Mashadi, N., Sudirman, R., Khalid, P. I., & LangeKüttner, C. (2015). Automated syntax analyses of drawing two tangent patterns in children with low and average handwriting ability. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 120(3), 865–894. https:// doi.org/10.2466/24.10.PMS.120v15x1 Todorov, E., & Jordan, M. I. (2002). Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination. Nature Neuroscience, 5(11), 1226–1235. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn963 Toomela, A. (2002). Drawing as a verbally mediated activity: A study of relationships between verbal, motor, and visuospatial skills and drawing in children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26(3), 234–247. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 01650250143000021 Vaivre-Douret, L., Lopez, C., Dutruel, A., & Vaivre, S. (2021). Phenotyping features in the genesis of pre-scriptural gestures in children to assess handwriting developmental levels. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 731. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-79315-w Valerio, G., Maffeis, C., Saggese, G., Ambruzzi, M. A., Balsamo, A., Bellone, S., Bergamini, M., Bernasconi, S., Bona, G., Calcaterra, V., Canali, T., Caroli, M., Chiarelli, F., Corciulo, N., Crinò, A., Di Bonito, P., Di Pietrantonio, V., Di Pietro, M., Di Sessa, A., … Zito, E. (2018). Diagnosis, treatment and prevention of pediatric obesity: Consensus position statement of the Italian Society for Pediatric Endocrinology and Diabetology 15 of 15 and the Italian Society of Pediatrics. Italian Journal of Pediatrics, 44(1), 88. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13052-018-0525-6 van Abswoude, F., van der Kamp, J., & Steenbergen, B. (2019). The roles of declarative knowledge and working memory in explicit motor learning and practice among children with low motor abilities. Motor Control, 23(1), 34–51. https://doi.org/10.1123/ mc.2017-0060 van der Meer, A. L. H., & van der Weel, F. R. (2017). Only three fingers write, but the whole brain works†: A high-density EEG study showing advantages of drawing over typing for learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 706. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg. 2017.00706 Van Gemmert, A. W. A., & Contreras-Vidal, J. L. (2015). Graphonomics and its contribution to the field of motor behavior: A position statement. Human Movement Science, 43, 165– 168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2015.08.017 Vandorpe, B., Vandendriessche, J., Vaeyens, R., Pion, J., Matthys, S., Lefevre, J., Philippaerts, R., & Lenoir, M. (2012). Relationship between sports participation and the level of motor coordination in childhood: A longitudinal approach. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 15(3), 220–225. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2011.09.006 WHO. (2007). Growth reference 5-19 years. https://www.who.int/ growthref/who2007_bmi_for_age/en/ Yuan, Y., & Brown, S. (2015). Drawing and writing: An ALE metaanalysis of sensorimotor activations. Brain and Cognition, 98, 15–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2015.05.004 Zaqout, M., Vyncke, K., Moreno, L. A., De Miguel-Etayo, P., Lauria, F., Molnar, D., Lissner, L., Hunsberger, M., Veidebaum, T., Tornaritis, M., Reisch, L. A., Bammann, K., Sprengeler, O., Ahrens, W., & Michels, N. (2016). Determinant factors of physical fitness in European children. International Journal of Public Health, 61(5), 573–582. https://doi.org/10. 1007/s00038-016-0811-2 Zoia, S., Biancotto, M., Guicciardi, M., Lecis, R., Lucidi, F., Pelamatti, G. M., Carrozzi, M., Skabar, A., Sugden, D. A., Barnett, A. L., & Henderson, S. E. (2018). An evaluation of the movement ABC-2 test for use in Italy: A comparison of data from Italy and the UK. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 84, 43–56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2018.04.013 SU PP O R TI N G I N F O RMA TI O N Additional supporting information may be found in the online version of the article at the publisher's website. How to cite this article: Bondi, D., Robazza, C., Lange-Küttner, C., & Pietrangelo, T. (2022). Fine motor skills and motor control networking in developmental age. American Journal of Human Biology, 34(8), e23758. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb. 23758