Nothing Special   »   [go: up one dir, main page]

skip to main content
10.5555/2447556.2447683acmconferencesArticle/Chapter ViewAbstractPublication PageshriConference Proceedingsconference-collections
research-article

A transition model for cognitions about agency

Published: 03 March 2013 Publication History

Abstract

Recent research in a range of fields has explored people's concepts about agency, and this issue is clearly important for understanding the conceptual basis of human-robot interaction. This research takes a wide range of approaches, but no systematic model of reasoning about agency has combined the concepts and processes involved agency-reasoning comprehensively enough to support research exploring issues such as conceptual change in reasoning about agents, and the interaction between concepts about agents and visual attention. Our goal in this paper is to develop a transition model of reasoning about agency that achieves three important goals. First, we aim to specify the different kinds of knowledge that is likely to be accessed when people reason about agents. Second, we specify the circumstances under which these different kinds of knowledge might be accessed and be changed. Finally, we discuss how this knowledge might affect basic psychological processes of attention and memory. Our approach will be to first describe the transition model, then to discuss how it might be applied in two specific domains: computer interfaces that allow a single operator to track multiple robots, and a teachable agent system currently in use assisting primary and middle school students in learning natural science concepts.

References

[1]
Apperly, I.A., Riggs, K.J., Simpson, A., Chiavarino, C., Samson, D. 2006. Is belief reasoning automatic? Psych. Sci. 17, 841--846.
[2]
Baron-Cohen, S. 1995. Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Bradford Books/MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
[3]
Barr, D. & Keysar, B. 2005. Mindreading in an Exotic Case: The Normal Adult Human. In B. F. Malle and S. D. Hodges (Eds), Other Minds: How Humans Bridge the Divide between Self and Others. New York, Guilford Press: 271--283.
[4]
Barrett, J.L., & Lanman, J.A. 2008. The Science of religious belief. Religion, 38, 109--124.
[5]
Bloom, P. 1997. Intentionality and word learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1, 9--12.
[6]
Butterworth, G. & Jarrett, N. 1991. What minds have in common is space: Spatial mechanisms serving joint visual attention in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 55--72.
[7]
Butler, S.C., Caron, A.J., & Brooks, R. 2000. Infant understanding of the referential nature of looking. Journal of Cognition and Development, 1, 359--377.
[8]
Dennett, D. 1991. Consciousness Explained, Boston, MA: Little, Brown & Co.
[9]
Eliot, A.J., & Devine, P.G. 1994. On the motivational nature of cognitive dissonance: Dissonance as psychological discomfort. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 67(3), 382--394.
[10]
Epley, N., Waytz, A., & Cacioppo, J. T. 2007. On seeing human: A three-factor theory of anthropomorphism. Psychological Review, 114, 864--886.
[11]
Fodor, Jerry A. 1983. Modularity of Mind: An Essay on Faculty Psychology. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. ISBN 0--262--56025--9
[12]
Gao, T., Newman, G. E., & Scholl, B. J. 2009. The psychophysics of chasing: A case study in the perception of animacy. Cognitive Psychology, 59(2), 154 - 179.
[13]
Graesser, A.C., McNamara, D.S., & VanLehn, K. 2005. Scaffolding deep comprehension strategies through Point&Query, AutoTutor, and iSTART. Educational Psychologist, 40, 225--234.
[14]
G. Gutheil, A. Vera, and F. C. Keil. 1998. "Do houseflies think?: Patterns of induction and biological beliefs in development," Cognition, vol. 66, pp. 33--49, 1998.
[15]
Hood, B.M., Willen, J.D., & Driver, J. 1998. Adult eyes trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants. Psychological Science, 9, 131--134.
[16]
Levin, D.T., Killingsworth, S.S., Saylor, M.M. 2008. Concepts about the capabilities of computers and robots: A test of the scope of adults' theory of mind. Proceedings of the 3rd Annual IEEE International Workshop on Human and Robot Interaction, 3, 57--64.
[17]
Levin, D.T., Saylor, M.M., & Lynn, S.D. in review. Distinguishing first-line defaults and second-line conceptualization in reasoning about humans, robots, and computers.
[18]
Levin, D.T., Saylor, M.M., Killingsworth, S.S., Gordon, S., & Kawamura, K. in review. Tests of concepts about different kinds of minds: Predictions about the behavior of computers, robots, and people. Human-Computer Interaction
[19]
Levin, D.T., Drivdahl, S.B., Momen, N., & Beck, M.R. 2002. False predictions about the detectability of unexpected visual changes: The role of beliefs about attention, memory, and the continuity of attended objects in causing change blindness blindness. Consciousness and Cognition, 11, 507--527.
[20]
Levin, D.T., & Beck, M.R. 2004. Thinking about seeing: Spanning the difference between metacognitive failure and success. In D.T. Levin (Ed), Thinking and Seeing: Visual Metacognition in Adults and Children. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.
[21]
Leslie, A.M., Friedman, O., & German, T.P. 2004. Core mechanisms in "theory of mind". Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 528--533.
[22]
Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. 2004. 12- and 18-month-old infants follow gaze to spaces behind barriers, Developmental Science, 7, F1-F9.
[23]
Mori, M. 1970. The uncanny valley. (K. F. MacDorman & T. Minato, Trans.). Energy, 7(4), 33--35.
[24]
Nass, C., & Moon, Y. 2000. Machines and mindlessness: Social responses to computers. Journal of Social Issues, 56, 81--103.
[25]
Parasuraman, R., Galster, S. & Miller, C. 2003. "Human control of multiple robots in the RoboFlag simulation environment." Proceedings of the IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man and Cybernetics.
[26]
R. Parasuraman, S. Galster, P. Squire, H. Furukawa. And C. Miller. 2005. A flexible delegation-type interface enhances system performance in human supervision of multiple robots: Empirical studies with RoboFlag. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man and Cybernetics -- Part A, 35(4): 481--493.
[27]
Posner, G.J., Strike, K.A., Hewson, P.W., & Gertzog, W.A. 1982. Accomodation of a scientific conception: Toward a theory of conceptual change. Science Education, 66, 211--227.
[28]
Searle, J. 1984. Minds, Brains, and Science. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
[29]
Woodward, A.L. 1998. Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach. Cognition, 69, 1--34.
[30]
Wimmer, H. & Perner, J. 1983. Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103--128.
[31]
Apperly, I.A., Riggs, K.J., Simpson, A., Chiavarino, C., & Samson, D. 2006. Is belief reasoning automatic? Psychological Science,17,841--846.
[32]
Fussell, S.R., Kiesler, S., Setlock, L.D., & Yew,V. 2008. How people anthropomorphize robots. In Proceedings of the 3rd ACM/IEEE Intr. Conf. on Human robot interaction, pp. 145--152.
[33]
Kiesler, S., Powers, A., Fussell, S. R., & Torrey, C. 2008. Anthropomorphic Interactions with a Robot and Robot--like Agent. Social Cognition, 26(2), 169--181.
[34]
Hymel, A., Levin, D.T., Barrett, J., Saylor M.M., and Biswas, G. (2011). The interaction of childrens' concepts about agents and their ability to use an agent-based tutoring system.
[35]
Mutlu, B., Kanda, T., Forlizzi, J., Hodgins, J., & Ishiguro, H. 2012. Conversational Gaze Mechanisms for Humanlike Robots. ACM Transactions on Interactive Intelligent Systems, 1(2), 12.
[36]
Avrunin, E., Hart, J., Douglas, A., & Scassellati, B. 2011. Effects related to synchrony and repertoire in perceptions of robot dance. Proceedings of the 6th International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction, 93--100.
[37]
Levin, D.T., Steger, A.P., Lynn, S.D., Adams, J.A., & Saylor, M.M. in prep. Conceptual change in beliefs about agency: The joint roles of deep concept availablility and cognitive dissonance.
[38]
Levin, D.T., Harriott, C., Paul, N., Zhang, T., & Adams, J.A. in press. Cognitive dissonance as a measure of reactions to human-robot interaction. Journal of Human-Robot Interaction.
[39]
Dunning, D., Meyerowtiz, J.A., Holtzberg, A.D. 1989. Ambiguity and self-evaluation: The role of idiosyncratic trait definitions in self-serving assessments of ability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1082--1090.
[40]
Gopnik, A., and Wellman, H.M. 1992. Why the child's theory of mind is really a theory. Mind and Language, 7, 145--171.
[41]
Leslie, A.M., Friedman, O., & German, T.P. 2004. Core mechanisms in "theory of mind". Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 528--533.
[42]
Endsley, M. R. 1988. "Design and evaluation for situation awareness enhancement." Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 32nd Annual Meeting, pp. 97--101.
[43]
Endsley, M. R. 1995. "Toward a theory of situation awareness in dynamic systems." Human Factors, 37(1): 32--64.

Cited By

View all
  • (2017)Understanding agency in interactions between children with autism and socially assistive robotsJournal of Human-Robot Interaction10.5898/JHRI.6.3.Short6:3(21-47)Online publication date: 1-Dec-2017
  • (2016)If asimo thinks, does roomba feel?Journal of Human-Robot Interaction10.5898/JHRI.5.3.Jaeger5:3(3-25)Online publication date: 20-Dec-2016

Index Terms

  1. A transition model for cognitions about agency

    Recommendations

    Comments

    Please enable JavaScript to view thecomments powered by Disqus.

    Information & Contributors

    Information

    Published In

    cover image ACM Conferences
    HRI '13: Proceedings of the 8th ACM/IEEE international conference on Human-robot interaction
    March 2013
    452 pages
    ISBN:9781467330558

    Sponsors

    In-Cooperation

    • AAAI: American Association for Artificial Intelligence
    • Human Factors & Ergonomics Soc: Human Factors & Ergonomics Soc

    Publisher

    IEEE Press

    Publication History

    Published: 03 March 2013

    Check for updates

    Author Tags

    1. HRI
    2. concepts
    3. theory of mind

    Qualifiers

    • Research-article

    Conference

    HRI'13
    Sponsor:

    Acceptance Rates

    Overall Acceptance Rate 268 of 1,124 submissions, 24%

    Contributors

    Other Metrics

    Bibliometrics & Citations

    Bibliometrics

    Article Metrics

    • Downloads (Last 12 months)5
    • Downloads (Last 6 weeks)0
    Reflects downloads up to 29 Sep 2024

    Other Metrics

    Citations

    Cited By

    View all
    • (2017)Understanding agency in interactions between children with autism and socially assistive robotsJournal of Human-Robot Interaction10.5898/JHRI.6.3.Short6:3(21-47)Online publication date: 1-Dec-2017
    • (2016)If asimo thinks, does roomba feel?Journal of Human-Robot Interaction10.5898/JHRI.5.3.Jaeger5:3(3-25)Online publication date: 20-Dec-2016

    View Options

    Get Access

    Login options

    View options

    PDF

    View or Download as a PDF file.

    PDF

    eReader

    View online with eReader.

    eReader

    Media

    Figures

    Other

    Tables

    Share

    Share

    Share this Publication link

    Share on social media